SELECTIVE PRESSURES INFLUENCING COLOR-VISION IN NEOTROPICAL PRIMATES

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "SELECTIVE PRESSURES INFLUENCING COLOR-VISION IN NEOTROPICAL PRIMATES"

Transcription

1 SELECTIVE PRESSURES INFLUENCING COLOR-VISION IN NEOTROPICAL PRIMATES A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts by Eric S. Seemiller August, 2011

2 ii Thesis written by Eric Scott Seemiller B.S., The Pennsylvania State University, 2009 M.A., Kent State University, 2011 Approved by Marilyn Norconk, Advisor Richard Meindl, Chair, Department of Anthropology John R. D. Stalvey, Dean, College of Arts and Sciences ii

3 iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES... iv LIST OF TABLES......v ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS... vi Chapter I. INTRODUCTION...1 The Front Side: Light Absorption in the Primate Eye...1 The Backside: Interpretation and Comparison of Signals...7 Explanations for the Evolution of Trichromacy in Primates...10 Research Objectives...12 II. METHODS...14 Experiment 1: Foraging Efficiency in Captive Primates...14 Methods and Materials...15 Experiment 2: Within-tree Foraging Preferences in Wild Howlers 19 III. RESULTS...23 Color-based foraging efficiency...23 Feeding preferences in free ranging howlers...28 IV. DISCUSSION...31 Conclusion...36 REFERENCES...37 iii

4 iv LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1: Diagram of the neural physiology of the primate vision subsystems...8 Figure 2.1: Experimental design used in foraging trials...16 Figure 2.2: Schematic of the concentric divisions of trees...22 Figure 3.1: Results of foraging trials for A. nigroviridis and C. wolfi...25 Figure 3.2: Results of foraging trials for S. sciureus...26 Figure 3.3: Results of foraging trials for A. caraya...27 Figure 3.4: Within-tree feeding preferences...29 Figure 3.5: Spatial distribution of feeding trees...30 iv

5 v LIST OF TABLES Table 1.1: Amino acid substitutions in vertebrate M/L opsin proteins...3 Table 1.2: Census of color-vision among mammals...5 Table 3.1: Results of within-tree feeding preferences and Chi-square tests...30 v

6 vi ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Foremost, I must acknowledge my advisor, Dr. Marilyn Norconk, whose invaluable patience and expert guidance brought me to this point. She pulled me through academically and physically trying times and stuck with me when she didn t have to and for that I thank her. Additionally, I must thank the members of my committee, Drs. Chi-hua Chiu and Richard Meindl for their appreciated input in the completion of this thesis. Furthermore, I must thank Drs. Chris Vinyard, Chris Kuhar and Kenneth Glander for letting me use their monkeys and offering their scientific input along the way. Without Dr. Tremie Gregory and Eric Henthorn, who picked me up off the ground on more than one occasion, I would not be in this position and therefore special thanks are directed toward them. Also, I must thank my parents, Donald and Patti Seemiller, and my sisters, Megan and Carrie Seemiller, for not thinking I was crazy when I said that I wanted to study monkeys. vi

7 1 Chapter One Introduction One of the hallmarks of the primate lineage is an increased reliance on vision, compared with other mammals, often at the expense of the other special senses. Many adaptations have evolved to improve specializations in the primate visual system including binocular vision, increased foveal acuity, and trichromacy in many species (Walls 1942). Trichromacy, or the ability to perceive and interpret light of three different wavelengths (i.e. color-vision), has been the topic of intense interest over the last 30 years because, among mammals, it exists almost exclusively in the primate lineage. Even then, trichromacy in primates exhibits variable phenotypic expression: fixed in catarrhine primates and variable in platyrrhine and strepsirrhine primates (Bowmaker et al. 1991; Jacobs et al. 1996). With the help of genetics, histology and behavioral studies, the full range of variability is emerging (e.g., Jacobs 1993; Cropp et al. 2002; Talebi et al. 2006; Veilleux and Bolnick 2009; Bunce et al. 2011). This thesis seeks to address the variation of trichromacy in the platyrrhine infraorder, while focusing on the selective forces that either drove it to fixation in some species or maintains its variability in others. The Front Side: Light Absorption in the Primate Eye 1

8 2 Trichromacy, or the ability to see red, green and blue wavelengths discretely, is the phenotype exhibited with the expression of three different opsins in the retina, resulting from a genotype of either three different opsin genes or two different opsins genes with multiple alleles. Opsins are 364-amino-acid septa-transmembrane proteins that exist on the outer layer of cone cells in the retina of the chordate eye. These proteins serve to absorb light of specific wavelengths, activating the neural response of the cone cell. There are two classes of opsin proteins in mammals: the shortwave sensitive opsin (S) and the middle/longwave sensitive opsin (M/L). The former is sensitive to wavelengths nearing 430nm, primarily blue light, while the latter is sensitive to wavelengths between 535 and 562nm, which consists of yellow-green light. While the S opsin is fixed in all mammals with few exceptions (e.g., Aotus, see below), the M/L opsin exhibits considerable genotypic and phenotypic variability, which will be addressed later (Bowmaker et al. 1991; Jacobs 1993) Although opsins are highly conserved exhibiting little variation among taxa in extant mammal lineages, very specific amino-acid substitutions were discovered to yield discrete shifts in spectral sensitivity in primate M/L opsins (Neitz et al. 1991). That is, at three specific amino acid locations in an opsin protein, a substitution will shift the wavelength sensitivity of the molecule in a discrete and predictable way. Initially studied in humans and tamarins, the three amino acids are at sites 180, 277 and 285. More recently, the full range of variation in the primate M/L opsin sensitivity has been mapped (Shyue et al. 1998). Specific amino acid substitutions will yield predictable shifts in color perception from 535 to 562nm (Table 1.1). Despite this system of variation in the M/L 2

9 3 opsin, the S opsin remains largely invariable, exhibiting no noteworthy amino acid polymorphisms, except a loss-of-function mutation in the nocturnal Aotus (Jacobs et al. 1996) (see below). Sensitivity AA 180 AA 277 AA nm Serine Tyrosine Threonine 556 nm Alanine Tyrosine Threonine 550 nm Alanine Phenylalanine Threonine 543 nm Alanine Tyrosine Alanine 535 nm Alanine Phenylalanine Alanine Table 1.1- Amino acid substitutions in mammal M/L Opsin proteins. Specific substitutions yield predictable shifts in spectral sensitivity (Neitz et al. 1991; Shyue et al. 1998). In the ancestral dichromatic mammalian condition, one autosomal S opsin gene and one X-linked M/L opsin gene are present, making mammals obligate dichromatic (Jacobs 1993). Obligate trichromats require a third distinct opsin gene. This is achieved in a two-step process. First, there must be allelic variation within the population. That is, the M/L opsin varies in the ways mentioned above, creating a third possible opsin allele within the population. This variation, because it is X-linked in mammals, allows the propensity for females to be trichromatic as they possess two X chromosomes and thus two M/L opsin alleles. Second, in order for males to exhibit trichromacy, gene duplication is necessary. The M/L opsin must duplicate and recombine, resulting in two M/L opsin genes on a single X-chromosome. After this has occurred in evolutionary history, with proper variation, both males and females will express three opsins. All catarrhines are trichromatic; the most recent common ancestor of catarrhines had one autosomal S opsin locus and experienced a duplication of the M/L opsin locus on 3

10 4 the X chromosome and recombination to create three unlinked opsin genes in both sexes (Nathans et al. 1986). These opsins have spectral tunings at 430, 535 and 562nm (Bowmaker et al. 1991; Hunt et al. 1998). However, there is tremendous variability in the platyrrhine lineage (Table 1.2). With two exceptions, all genera exhibit a sex-based phenotypic difference in color-vision (Jacobs 1984; Jacobs et al. 1996): two-thirds of females are trichromats while one-third of females and all males are dichromats. That is, there is allelic variation within the population, but they lack the genetic recombination necessary for a trichromatic phenotype in males. There are two exceptions to this widespread event in platyrrhines: Aotus (night monkeys), as mentioned above, has a loss-of-function mutation in its S opsin and is monochromatic (Jacobs 1993). Furthermore there is no allelic variation in the M/L opsin, and all night monkeys possess only a 535nm opsin. The other exception in platyrrhines occurs in the genus, Alouatta. All individuals of Alouatta are trichromatic, exhibiting the spectral tuning characteristic of the catarrhine lineage due to a lineage specific duplication of the M/L opsin locus on the X chromosome, leading to three unlinked opsin genes (Hunt et al. 1998; Jacobs et al. 1996). 4

11 5 Taxon Phenotype Comments and References Non-primate mammals Uniformly dichromatic Excepting marsupials (1) and primates, trichromacy is rare in the mammalian class (2) Catarrhines (including humans) Uniformly trichromatic Platyrrhines Atelinae (Ateles, Brachyteles, Lagothrix) Alouattinae (Alouatta) Dichromatic ; Polymorphic Uniformly trichromatic All possess the same three photopigments, allowing them to distinguish three colors (2) Three M/L opsin alleles exist in the lineage with tunings of 535, 550 and 562nm (3)(4) A gene duplication allows them the same genotype and phenotype as catarrhines (5) Pitheciinae (Pithecia) Dichromatic ; Polymorphic Cebinae (Cebus, Saimiri) Dichromatic ; Polymorphic Callitrichinae (Callithrix, Callimico, Cebuella, Saguinis, Leontopithecus) Dichromatic ; Polymorphic Only Pithecia has been studied, with the same alleles as Atelinae (6) Alleles for all possible opsins have been discovered, though frequencies of each are unknown (6)(7) Some individuals are functionally dichromatic despite being genetically trichromatic (6)(7)(8) Callicebinae (Callicebus) Dichromatic ; Polymorphic Possess only three alleles, similar to Atelinae.(9) Aotinae (Aotus) Monochromatic All individuals possess only one photopigment, making them monochromatic (2) Table 1.2- A census of color-vision among mammals. (1- Arrese et al. 2005; 2- Jacobs 1993; 3- Jacobs and Deegan 2001; 4- Talebi et al. 2006; 5- Jacobs et al. 2006; 6- Jacobs and Deegen 2003; 7- Cropp et al. 2002; 8- Surridge and Mundy 2002; 9- Bunce et al. 2011) 5

12 6 Although opsins are highly conserved at the amino acid level, especially at spectral tuning sites, there are some significant amino acid differences between catarrhine and platyrrhine opsin proteins (Hunt et al. 1998). This suggests that opsin gene evolution occurred after the Oligocene split of the primate infraorders. Because of the presence of trichromacy in all extant catarrhines, Hunt et al. (1998) propose that allelic variation and recombination occurred very early in the lineage and was quickly driven to fixation. Molecular dating in the platyrrhine lineage, however, shows that allelic variation stalled until 20 mya (Boissinot et al. 1998), and recombination occurred only in one lineage, the Alouattinae, at around 16 mya (Hunt et al. 1998). In addition to inter-specific opsin variation, cone concentrations are also variable among primates. S cones make up roughly 10% of the entire complement of cones with the remaining 90% consisting of one M/L cone type in dichromats or two M/L cone types in trichromats (Calkins 2001). However, individual parts of the eye display higher concentrations of cones in different primates. Cones are densest in the fovea region, where visual acuity is highest. Most primates, including humans, have ca. 200,000 cones/mm 2 (Curcio et al. 1991) in the fovea, but there is a tendency for platyrrhines to have lower foveal cone densities and slightly higher peripheral densities than catarrhines (Franco et al. 2000), although it remains largely unclear how this affects visual acuity. Again the two exceptions among platyrrhines are Aotus with maximum cone densities of only 20,000 cones/mm 2 (Yamada et al. 2001) and Alouatta, which has 376,000 cones/mm 2, nearly twice the density as the average primate (Finlay et al. 2008; Franco et al. 2000). The decreased concentration of cones in Aotus is believed to be beneficial for 6

13 7 low-light vision (Jacobs et al. 1993), however, it is unknown why Alouatta possesses such a high density of cones in the fovea, and if this impacts visual acuity. The Backside: Interpretation and Comparison of Signals There are two neural visual subsystems in primates (Figure 1.1). The first system is the ancestral mammalian subsystem that is present in all mammals and the second is the derived subsystem that augments the ancestral condition in primates. The ancestral subsystem consists of two classes of bipolar nerve cells that transmit information from S cones and M/L cones, terminating the signal at the Koniocellular layer of the lateral genticular nucleus (LGN) in the thalamus (Dacey and Lee 1994). These cells then compare the ratio of signals, known as blue-yellow opponency. The derived subsystem is made of midget ganglion cells that correspond specifically to M/L cones, with the signal terminating in the parvocellular layer of the LGN, where red-green opponency is calculated (Kremers and Lee 1998). 7

14 8 Figure 1.1- Diagram of the neural physiology of the primate vision subsystems. In the ancient subsystem, signals are activated through S and M/L cones and, after passing through bipolar cells, activate small bistratified ganglion cells en route to the Koniocellular layer of the LGN where blue-yellow opponency is calculated. In the derived primate subsystem, on M and L cones in the fovea centralis are activated, and the signal goes through midget ganglion cells to the Parvocellular layers of the LGN where red-green opponency is calculated (derived from Regan et al. 2001; Lee 2004). 8

15 9 The greater bush baby (Otolemur garnetti), an extant representative of the earliest primate lineages, lacks trichromatic vision but employs both subsystems, having bipolar cells and midget ganglion cells (Yamada et al. 1998), suggesting that the advent of the derived primate subsystem occured very early in the primate lineage. Additionally, there is no substantive difference in the backside physiology of vision between catarrhines and platyrrhines (Lee et al. 2000), nor is there one between dichromatic and trichromatic platyrrhines (Solomon 2002). This suggests that the comparison of trichromatic signaling probably co-opted a pre-existing existing primate visual system. It is likely that midget ganglion cells of the derived subsystem evolved as an adaptation to increase visual acuity in the fovea of primates en masse early in their evolutionary history and has since been used in a way to compare and interpret red and green light following the addition of a third opsin (Lee 2004). Furthermore, interpretation of red-green opponency in the ancestral system is dependent upon spectral tuning differentiation: parvocellular cells lack the ability to discriminate colors efficiently when comparing opsin signals that differ by fewer than 7nm, increasing in efficiency when opsin signals differ by more than 20nm (Blessing et al. 2004). For example, a female Saguinus with two M/L opsins tuned to 556 and 562nm (Jacobs and Deegen 2003) may be anatomically and genetically trichromatic, but functionally dichromatic as she is unable to interpret red-green opponency. The spectral difference between opsins tuned at 535 and 562nm is much greater and would allow her the ability interpret red-green opponency. 9

16 10 Explanations for the Evolution of Trichromacy in Primates The angiosperm radiation hypothesis (Sussman 1991) posits that the primate lineage evolved at the same time as flowering plants, offering many opportunities for coevolutionary relationships. One possible result of this interaction involves the evolution of frugivory and seed dispersal. In this scenario, frugivorous primates are rewarded with carbohydrate and nutrient dense, edible fruits that are often brightly colored in exchange for seed dispersal, as the primate will either carry the seed away from the plant, or most often defecate the seed some time after ingesting the fruit, far from the parent tree (e.g. Janzen 1969, 1970; Clark and Clark 1984). Although the evolution of visual cues to prospective seed dispersers comes at some cost to the plant, colorful pericarps are found in many plant families and the investment in a signal seems worth the benefit of seed removal. According to this hypothesis, color-vision phenotypes will be most advantageous when the possessor is efficient at foraging for colorful fruits, creating a major selective pressure in plant-vertebrate interactions. Mollon (1989) first hypothesized that color vision in primates had primary utility for finding fruit, offering seed dispersal as a reward for plants that invested in making more visible fruit. Through computational modeling, Osorio and Vorobyev (1996) demonstrated that trichromats should be better than dichromats at spotting red fruits contrasted against a green background. Regan et. al (2001) reiterated this finding with a more comprehensive model that included a wide analysis of food and non-food fruits along with foliage in French Guiana. Finally, Caine and Mundy (2000) demonstrated a 10

17 11 foraging advantage for trichromatic Callithrix geoffryi by simulating fruit-foraging situations. However, a number of researchers have failed to corroborate these findings in other species. For example, researchers have not found color-based fruit or insect foraging advantages in either Cebus capucinus (Melin et al. 2007; Melin et al. 2009), Ateles geoffroyi (Hiramatsu et al. 2008), or in a number of catarrhines including Pan troglodytes, Colobus guereza, Cercopithecus ascanius (Dominy and Lucas 2001) and Macaca fasicularis (Lucas et al. 1998). Other researchers have pointed to long-range visual acuity as the benefit of trichromacy, proposing that color-vision is most effective compared to dichromacy over a long distance (Caine and Mundy 2000). According to this hypothesis, trichromatic females should be more adept at spotting brightly colored fruits from a distance than her dichromatic conspecifics and should provide a social feeding advantage to groups with trichromatic females (Travis et al. 1988). However, troop leaders for groups that have been tested were found to be (obligate dichromatic) males, nullifying any long-range advantage trichromatic females might have (Saguinus imperator and Saguinus fuscicollis- Dominy et al. 2003; Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis- Smith et al. 2003). Possible interpretations of these results include a) that there is no feeding benefit to being a trichromat as other cues (e.g., olfactory, tactile, etc.) are equally reliable; b) there is a visual benefit to foraging, but social factors can invalidate these benefits; c) groups accrue a wider variety of benefits (e.g. anti-predation) by having individuals with diverse visual capabilities in the group. 11

18 12 The finding that Alouatta, with a diet that is more than 50% leaves (Charello 1994; Julliot and Sabatier 1993), is the only uniformly trichromatic platyrrhine has led to a reorganization of evolutionary scenarios. If not red fruit, then perhaps red leaves provide the dietary justification for trichromacy. Dominy, Lucas and colleagues suggest that trichromacy has maximum utility for discerning young, red, nutrient-dense leaves from mature, tough green leaves (Dominy and Lucas 2001; Lucas et al. 1998; Dominy and Lucas 2004). This hypothesis suggests that there is a nutritious advantage in younger (red) leaves that would be beneficial to an animal whose trichromacy enables them to distinguish them from green background foliage. Additionally fewer achromatic cues (i.e. luminance, depth, tactile, scent, etc.) in leaves would make trichromacy vital for the detection of desirable young leaves (Hiramatsu et al. 2008). While trichromacy should benefit animals that can use color cues to detect edible fruits and leaves, there are some costs to giving up dichromacy. For example, dichromats are more efficient foragers in low-light conditions than trichromats (Caine et al. 2010, Yamashita et al. 2005) and actually monochromacy is predicted to be most efficient in low-light settings (Vorobyev et al. 2001). Additionally, dichromats are largely immune to camouflage. Caine and colleagues discovered that they were far more effective foragers when food is camouflaged with its background than trichromatic conspecifics (Caine and Mundy 2000; Caine et al. 2003). These two drawbacks suggest that trichromacy is not without expense and likely has a selective force maintaining it. Research Objectives 12

19 13 Two short studies were conducted here to assess foraging differences between dichromatic and trichromatic haplorhines. The first experiment addresses the hypothesis that dichromats and trichromats do not utilize color cues to forage for fruit-like objects. In this study conducted at Cleveland Metroparks Zoo, two male platyrrhines were compared and contrasted with two male catarrhines. I predicted that given the uniform nature of the food item, differing only in color, if the individuals rely on color discrimination they will express a preference for the foods that are presented in a colorcontrast situation. I also predict that if there is no difference in food choice, then they likely rely on achromatic cues so that both trichromats and dichromats will forage indiscriminately and without discrepancies in preference between red cereal and green cereal on a green background. The second study took place on free-ranging howling monkeys in Costa Rica. Because trichromacy loses efficiency in low-light settings, as trichromats lose foraging ability as light levels decrease (Caine et al. 2010; Vorobyev et al. 2001), I predict that uniformly trichromatic Alouatta palliata will forage preferentially in areas with higher levels of ambient light, i.e., the periphery of trees. 13

20 14 Chapter Two Methods Experiment 1: Foraging preference in captive primates at Cleveland Metroparks Zoo The goal of this experiment is to test whether trichromatic and dichromatic primates demonstrate any difference in foraging preference when provided with an identical food in which only color is changed. I hypothesized that color does not play an important role in fruit foraging. I predicted that there would be no difference in foraging preference between the two phenotypes and that both would forage indiscriminate to color. To test this, four species of primates were presented with foraging tests that sought whether color factored in food selection: two platyrrhines (Alouatta caraya and Saimiri sciureus) and two catarrhines (Allenopithecus nigroviridis and Cercopithecus wolfi). A. nigroviridis and C. wolfi are both catarrhines and therefore trichromatic (Bowmaker et al. 1991) as is A. caraya, of the family Alouattinae (Jacobs et al. 1996). Because S. sciureus females are polymorphic, only males were used to ensure that there was limited variability in color perception; these male primates are dichromatic (Jacobs 1984). 14

21 15 Materials and Methods All subjects are housed in the Primate, Cats and Aquatics building of the Cleveland Metroparks Zoo, Cleveland, Ohio. A total of 5 trials were presented to each individual or group at weekly intervals. Trials for A. nigroviridis, C. wolfi and S. sciureus occurred during March and April 2011, between 9:00 and 10:00 A.M. Because of other time commitments for zoo staff, trials for A. caraya occurred during May and June 2011 between 10:00 and 10:30 A.M. The test apparatus consisted of a plastic tray (42 x 25 x 10cm) lined with green construction paper and bisected with a fastened block of wood. Shredded green construction paper was layered in both sides of the tray to a uniform level to make food choices less obvious. Fruity Cheerios were placed on both sides of the tray. A total of 20 cereal (10 of each color) were used in each trial (Figure 2.1). 15

22 16 Figure 2.1- Experimental design used in foraging trials. Above is a grayscale representation, which demonstrates no difference in achromatic contrast between red and green cereal, with variable difference against the background. Below is the full color display. 16

23 17 Zoo employees isolated subjects from their enclosures briefly so that they could place the testing apparatus in a position that could be viewed by the researchers from outside the enclosure. Since this isolation is part of their daily routine for enclosure cleaning and maintenance, they are trained to transfer in such a way and were not handled in the course of the experiment. Though the catarrhines involved were tested alone, due to cage configurations, S. sciureus and A. caraya individuals were unable to be isolated for the experiment allowing other individuals in the group to interfere (see below). Once the experiment was completed, the subject was again transferred out of the enclosure so that the test apparatus could be removed. All remaining paper materials were discarded between trials. Generally, three species were tested on a single morning. Feeding was not restricted prior to the test, although that did not appear to affect the subjects interest in the experiment. The red and green sides of the tray were alternated with each trial, so that handedness or simple convenience did not have an effect on the results. Trials were filmed using a Panasonic video recorder on a tripod to prevent errors in data collection during the trials. Subjects were allowed to forage for a maximum of five minutes or until all food items were eaten. Adobe Photoshop (7.0) was used to ascertain luminance data of the two colors of cereal (Bergman and Beehner 2008). A color photograph was taken with a Nikon D3100 camera and a grayscale image was rendered. Using this image, achromatic percentages (i.e. the percentage of light and dark, with absolute black being 100%) were taken of both colors of cereal and the background. All cereal ranged between 48-52%, while the 17

24 18 background ranged between 54-60%. This ensures that the cereal does not differ in luminance, but only in color. Also, both colors are different than the background. Essentially, this means that both colors of cereal are equally bright, precluding the subjects from using this as a way to discriminate between cereal colors, but still allowing it to be used to discriminate from the background. Trials were scored in one of two ways. If there was not interference from other monkeys (in the case of A. nigroviridis and C. wolfi) rank-scores were calculated. To do this, the order in which each individual cereal item was eaten was documented. Then, each cereal was given a rank based on the order it was eaten, between 1 and 20. The first cereal eaten was ranked with a 1, the second was ranked with a 2 and so on. Two rank sum scores were summed, based on the two colors. Therefore, the rank-sum score for green cereal in a particular trial is the sum of the ranks for all green cereal eaten. A complementary red rank-sum score was calculated in the same way. Scores ranged from 55, if the subject ate all of that color before moving to the other color, (i.e., = 55) to 155, if the subject ate all of that color last (i.e., = 155). Lower scores for a color indicate that color was eaten first. Because these values are complementary, only the green rank-sums were used for analysis. Graphs were made for both subjects that showed the green-rank sum of each trial. T-tests were used to determine if the trials differed significantly from an expected mean of 105. The second option for quantification was similar. If interference from another individual occurred causing the subject to not eat all of the cereal in the test (as was the case for S. sciureus and A. caraya), the mean-rank sum was ascertained. To do this, the 18

25 19 rank-sums were calculated in the same way as the first option. However, this value was then divided by the total number of that color eaten during the trial. For example, if the subject only ate 5 red cereal, with a rank-sum of 35, this value was divided by 5, to arrive at a mean rank-sum of 7 for that color in that trial. These red and green values for each trial are not complementary, however they can be compared. The values were compiled into a chart for analysis. Again, a lower mean-rank sum indicates that the subject ate more of that color first. There are two scenarios in which the mean-rank sum could be a deceptive figure, although only one was encountered. If the subject did not eat any of one color, it would have a rank-sum of zero for that trial, as there would be no values to sum. If this happened, it was specifically noted. Additionally, if, for example, the subject ate only one or two of one color, this could generate a misleadingly low score and would need to be noted. However, this was not observed for any trial in this study. Experiment 2: Within-tree foraging preferences for wild howlers in Costa Rica Variable ambient light conditions are known to affect the ability of wild trichromats to perceive color variation in food items (Yamashita 2005). I hypothesized that trichromatic primates would attempt to behaviorally overcome the drawbacks of trichromacy. I predicted that monkeys would selectively feed in areas with higher light levels, such as the periphery of tree, to offset some of the poor low-light vision that comes with trichromacy. To test this, a habituated group of semi-free ranging mantled howling monkeys (Alouatta paliatta) was followed for 264 hours between July and 19

26 20 August 2010 in La Pacifica, Costa Rica (10 28 N, W). The site is situated in the lowland tropical dry forest zone (Holdridge 1967) meaning that the forest changes dramatically seasonally, with a number of deciduous trees that lose their leaves. The study group resided on a 1330 ha cattle ranch and tilapia fish farm called Hacienda La Pacifica (Glander 1979), in which 42 distinct social groups have been recognized (Teaford and Glander 1996). Further, A. paliatta is the only nonhuman primate on the ranch (Teaford and Glander 1996). Though much of the forest on the ranch is connected, this study focused on Group 1, which inhabited a partially isolated 3 ha forest segment established as wind-break forest strip (Glander 1992). The forest fragment is surrounded by two pastures and a major road, which restricted the movement of the monkeys. Although there is a connection to larger forest patches, Group 1 remained cohesive and stayed inside this forest patch for the entire study period. Group 1 consisted of eight individuals: 1 adult male, 4 adult females, 2 juveniles and 1 infant. One adult female died during the study period due to a fall. The study occurred during the long wet season (June through December), predominately during the peak weeks of the wet season (mid to late July) (Glander et al. 1991). During the period daily rainfall ranged from 0 mm to 44 mm, with a daily average of 9.3 mm. The neardaily rainfall allowed all vegetation in the range to remain lush, effectively precluding food availability as a confounding factor in this study. Data were collected during a series of all-day follows, in which animals were followed from 6:00 to 18:00, approximately dawn to dusk. A random sequence of focal animals was determined each day and included all individuals, as the infant was weaned 20

27 21 and could eat on its own. This sequence was used to determine the order of observation once group feeding began in order to ensure that there was no bias toward individuals that were more easily observed. When feeding in the group commenced, the first individual to start eating was observed for 20 minutes or until it stopped feeding. There was no individual in the group who demonstrated any bias toward eating first, and thus this generally proved to be a random individual each time. Data were recorded at five-minute intervals including: within-tree location (defined below), tree ID number and food type. After the focal animal stopped feeding, or at 20 minutes, the next individual on the list was observed for 20 minutes in the same way. If the next individual was not feeding or could not be seen, the next individual in the observation order was chosen. This process continued until all feeding stopped. If one animal was observed to feed alone, either before or after a general group feeding time, data were recorded in the same way. To determine in which part of the tree the animal was feeding, each tree was divided into 4 concentric spheres: inner, middle, outer and edge (Figure 2.2). The edge was defined as the area that is within arms length of the outer-most part of the tree. The remainder of the tree was trisected into equal concentric layers. Also, food type was determined as fruit, leaf or other. Only trees that were visited more than once were included in the analysis of within-tree foraging preferences. This served to eliminate passive feeding as a confounding factor and to focus on deliberate feeding. Chi-square tests were used for each tree to determine if there was a significant difference between sections of the tree. Though there are volumetric differences between each section of the 21

28 22 tree, they were variable for each tree analyzed. Therefore, I assumed equal likelihood for each part of the tree as a null hypothesis (α =.05). Figure 2.2- Schematic of the concentric divisions of trees. This was used to determine the part of the tree in which a focal animal was feeding. It is important to note that in reality, the divisions would be hemispheric, and this is only a two-dimensional rendering. 22

29 23 Chapter Three Results Color-based Foraging Preference The results of the foraging preference trials satisfied the predictions of the hypothesis that primates are indifferent to color when foraging for foods with many properties similar to fruit. In five trials, neither trichromatic catarrhine showed a significant deviation from the expected green rank-sum value of 105 (A. nigroviridis: t= 0.729, p=.506; C. wolfi: t= 1.233, p=.285), meaning that they did not use color to discriminate the two halves of the test apparatus. A. nigroviridis showed a tendency toward extremes, generally eating all of one color first, which could be interpreted as a preference. However, if it was a preference, it changed between colors over the course of the five trials. Conversely, C. wolfi s results were distributed normally (Figure 3.1). Because females interfered with the dichromatic test subject male in S. sciureus the total green rank-sum could not be ascertained and compared against an expected value. Therefore the mean rank-sum of both colors in five trials is shown in Figure 3.2. As predicted, the subject showed no preference for either color. Like S. sciureus, the A. caraya subject could not be isolated and the mean rank-sum of both colors was used (Figure 3.3). Though the subject showed no interest in the test during trial one, he was receptive for the remaining four trials. The results mimicked those of the other primates in the study; the subject showed no preference for either color, and foraged 23

30 24 indiscriminately. Because there was no expected value or comparable mean, hypothesis testing was not possible for S. sciureus and A. caraya. 24

31 25 Figure 3.1- Results of foraging trials for A. nigroviridis and C. wolfi. The dotted line represents the expected results of the trial. A. nigroviridis tended toward extremes, while C. wolfi distributed normally around the expected value. Neither differed significantly from expectations. 25

32 26 Figure 3.2- Results of foraging trials for S. sciureus. Because females interfered with the test subject, the mean rank-sum for each color was used. With the exception of trial 1, in which the subject ate only green cereal, there is no difference between red and green cereal in any trial. Because the total number of cereal eaten in each trial was different, there was no expected value, and thus hypothesis testing was not possible. 26

33 27 Figure 3.3- Results of foraging trials for A. caraya. Despite no participation in the first trial, the remaining four trials went as expected, with no clear preference for either color. 27

34 28 Feeding Preferences in Free Ranging Howlers The spatial distribution of the trees is shown in Figure 3.5. The feeding samples are concentrated heavily in the northwest corner of the range, where 3 of the 4 most frequented trees were clustered (Trees #411, 67, 412). The remaining trees are distributed evenly throughout the range. Members of Group 1 were observed feeding on more than one occasion in seven different trees: 3 Enterolobrium cyclocarpum, Muntingia calabura, Mastichodendron tempisque, Maclura tinctoria, and one unknown species. The remaining observed feedings were deemed to be only passive feeding and not included in the analyses. Individuals were observed feeding from these trees a total of 360 times. Of these observations, 213 were from Tree 411 (Maclura tinctoria), 69 were from Tree 67 (Enterolobrium cyclocarpum) and the remaining 5 trees were each sampled fewer than 30 times (Figure 3.4). Howlers fed on the edge greater than 50% of the time in all trees except one, and percentages declined toward the inner part of the tree. Trees #411 (χ 2 =142.9, p<.0001), #67 (χ 2 = 79.1, p<.0001), #412 (χ 2 = 13.3, p =.001), #447 (χ 2 = 19.1, p<.0001), #627 (χ 2 = 6.4, p =.011), and #417 (χ 2 = 6.2, p =.045) were all significant for a feeding bias on the periphery of the tree compared with other portions of the tree. Conversely, only Tree #416 (χ 2 = 1.4, p =.497) showed no significant feeding bias (Table 3.1). 28

35 29 Figure 3.4- Within-tree feeding preferences. 29

36 30 Edge (%) 53.5 Outside (%) 34.3 Middle (%) 11.7 Inner (%) 0.5 Food n χ2 p Fruit <.0001 Enterolobrium cyclocarpum Mastichodendron tempisque Unknown Leaf < Leaf < Leaf Muntingia calabura Enterolobrium cyclocarpum Enterolobrium cyclocarpum Fruit Leaf Leaf Tree ID 411 Species Maclura tinctoria Table 3.1- Results of within-tree feeding preferences and Chi-square tests. Percentages of each tree sample are shown for each part of the tree. Of the seven feeding-trees visited more than once during the study, six showed a significant deviation from expected within-tree feeding frequencies. Figure 3.5- Spatial distribution of feeding trees. The tree locations are graded in size based on frequency of feeding. 30

37 31 Chapter Four Discussion The findings of this research corroborate the ideas presented by Lucas et. al (1998, 2003) and Dominy and Lucas (2001), in that color-based foraging preference is not readily demonstrated by primates when dealing with a three-dimensional object such as fruit. Though the experiments used cereal, it does mimic the texture and dimensionality that a primate might face when foraging for fruit in the wild. When foraging, there are numerous cues that a primate can consider: visual cues like color or depth, olfactory cues, tactile cues, etc. This experiment controlled many of these, because the cereal only differed in color. When everything else is the same, both trichromatic and dichromatic primates in this study seemed to ignore color when foraging, and ate the cereal indiscriminately with no preference for either color. However, when a primate is feeding on a flat object, such as a leaf, there are fewer non-chromatic cues available, as the forager often forgoes depth and possibly olfaction and texture depending on the species of leaf. Furthermore, young red tropical leaves are protein-rich and often less hard and easier to eat (Dominy and Lucas 2004) and unlike fruit that can ripen to a multitude of colors, they operate primarily on a red-green axis (Dominy 2004) almost requiring trichromacy for maximum foraging efficiency. But the majority of trichromatic primates do not subsist entirely, or even in any large capacity 31

38 32 on leaves. How then does folivory provide an adequately powerful selective force to favor trichromacy? For many primates, leaves are a fallback food. A consensus definition of fallback foods is that they are low-quality foods that which, despite augmenting the diet year round, are vastly important during seasonal periods when higher-quality foods are unavailable (Lambert 2007; Marshall et al. 2009; Wrangham et al. 1998). Though periods of fallback food subsistence can be rare (e.g. El Niño years), they are potent and are widely considered to have an intense influence on morphological adaptations: dental (Lambert et al. 2004; Lucas 2000; Rosenberger and Kinzey 1976), digestive (Davies et al. 1988; Porter et al. 2009; Remis et al. 2001), and now even visual. The experiment conducted here as well as others have demonstrated that color-vision is largely irrelevant when foraging for fruit. Therefore, the most parsimonious explanation for the evolution of trichromacy is that it is most advantageous during periods of fallback subsistence, when the primary food source is leaves. It is during this period that the capacity for distinguishing young, nutritious, red leaves from more mature green leaves is most essential, and also when trichromacy is most beneficial due to the increased reliance on chromatic cues when foraging. So what is so special about Alouatta that led to its trichromacy while other platyrrhines have maintained polymorphic color vision? The South and Central American rainforests are equally susceptible to seasonal variation and periods of fallback subsistence as the Old World rainforests, and often, due to coincidental flushing and fruiting, experience exaggerated fallback periods (van Schaik et al. 1993) forcing a 32

39 33 number of morphological adaptations for fallback subsistence (Rosenberger 1992). Though other Neotropical primates exploit an array of fallback foods (e.g. seeds, insects) howlers are much quicker to revert to fallback folivory subsistence. Greater than half of their diet consists of leaves (Di Fiore and Campbell 2007), causing them to undergo a suite of adaptations for folivory (Milton 1977; Teaford and Glander 1996). Then the question becomes which came first: trichromacy or folivory? There are two distinct possibilities: 1) Alouatta is the only taxon that managed the mutation necessary for uniform trichromacy, opening the door to such intense reliance on folivory or 2) the selective force for trichromacy is just simply higher in howlers than in other platyrrhine species because of a reliance on folivory. The fossil record of Neotropical primates contains a Miocene folivore similar to extant Alouatta from 12.6 mya called Stirtonia (Hershkovitz 1970). Molecular evidence, however, suggests that Alouatta split from the other Atelids at 16.0 mya (Cortes-Ortiz 2003). Because the same recombination is present in all extant Alouatta species, it almost certainly preceded the radiation of howlers, thus making it older than the oldest known fossil folivore in the lineage. This evidence suggests that trichromacy preceded other folivorous adaptations in Alouatta, and likely set the stage for a dependence on fallback folivory; trichromacy came first in Alouatta. If that is the case, folivory might not be the selective force favoring trichromacy, but instead a convenient by-product. My second hypothesis was also supported: in the wild howler study, subjects fed in the periphery of the tree more often than expected by chance. This could demonstrate a behavioral response to maximize utility in a morphological adaptation. Because 33

40 34 trichromatic primates forfeit foraging efficiency as light levels decrease (Caine et al. 2010; Yamashita et al. 2005), a trichromatic primate should feed in areas with maximum ambient light, like the outside of a tree. Though Marshall and Wrangham (2007) argue that behavioral adaptations are generally responses to primary food sources, this appears to be an exception. As hypothesis one reiterates, color-vision is not necessary for fruit foraging, thus there would be no need to maximize trichromatic efficiency when foraging for fruit. Therefore, this behavior is likely an adaptation to maximize visual efficiency for leaf foraging. One might argue that there are other benefits to feeding of the periphery of tree crowns, namely nutritional ones. For instance, the terminal buds in the periphery might offer more nutritious value. However, this experiment controlled for that; terminal buds are equally likely to exist in all parts of the tree, or at the very least are present in all parts of the tree. Further, there was no difference in texture or color between parts of the tree, two characteristics that can often be correlated with nutritious attributes (Lucas 2000). Though it is possible that there may be outstanding, uncontrollable advantages to peripheral feeding, this experiment has made sufficient attempts to control for them. Variation among food types is equally likely in all parts of the tree. This hypothesis requires further investigation in other primates. In order to verify the utility of peripheral feeding, dichromatic arborealists and polymorphic arborealists must be studied in the same context. If a dichromatic primate is also foraging on the periphery, it could nullify the results of hypothesis 2 of this thesis, at least in the context 34

41 35 of color-vision, and peripheral feeding would likely be a response to some extraneous cue, one for which I was unable to control. Though they help highlight the relationship of folivory and trichromacy in Alouatta, the results of these two studies do not satisfactorily explain the maintenance of polymorphisms in other platyrrhines. Though the lineage exhibits an array of fallback food-types, trichromacy is still maintained in females when available, and it has been that way for 20 million years, the age of the allelic variation (Boissinot et al. 1998). By exploiting alternative fallback niches, according to my findings, trichromacy should not be excessively beneficial to most Neotropical primates. This leads to two possibilities: 1) the trait is either drifting, with no significant purifying selection or 2) the trait is extrinsically beneficial to all possessors and there is merely a mutational barrier which Alouatta and Old World monkeys have overcome. The results of the second study, in which howlers behaviorally counteracted a disadvantage to trichromacy, suggests that downfalls of trichromacy are real and have the potential to be powerful purifying selective agents. If these disadvantages are strong enough, this would preclude the first possibility and lead to more questions: if the heterozygote advantage in polymorphic platyrrhines is not for eating fruit, or insects (Melin 2007), is it for another food type, or something altogether different? The catarrhine lineage seems to have exploited trichromacy for a diverse set of diets, suggesting that it might supersede feeding altogether. Though much of the research in trichromacy is relevant to feeding, it is possible that predator defense or sexual selection offer selective pressures (Bradley and Mundy 2008), and it is in this direction that some future research should be directed. 35

42 36 Conclusion Though the findings of this thesis are not in conflict with any recent findings, they do lead to more questions. These results, and others, have demonstrated that frugivory is not substantially impacted by a trichromatic phenotype. One benefit of trichromacy is that it allows the exploitation of a leaf-eating niche, which happened in Alouatta after recombination led to its fixation. However, its maintenance in platyrrhines that do not heavily rely on leaves is baffling. Further, trichromacy has several apparent disadvantages that must be overcome. Based on that, is unlikely that genetic drift has led to color-vision polymorphisms, and the phenotype has been actively maintained by some selective force. Additional research is required to paint a complete picture on the selective benefits of trichromacy. Future research in this field has a macro and micro approach. Additional ecological research is required to analyze the patterns of color-based foraging, which can be a murky endeavor as food color has many confounding attributes that must be considered (e.g. blue-yellow opponency, luminance, ripening, etc.). Other aspects of color-vision ecology must also be analyzed. Are there benefits outside of feeding, like predator defense, sexual selection or conspecific recognition, that are aided by trichromacy? Likewise, advances in retinal mapping and topography have opened the door to a micro approach. Retinal mapping on a broad sample of primates will provide a more complete picture on the physiological nature of color-vision. Both of these approaches must be considered as the study of primate vision moves forward. 36

43 37 References Arrese CA, Oddy AY, Runham PB, Hart NS, Shand J, Hunt DM, and Beazley LD Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus). Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society 272: Bergman TJ, and Beehner JC A simple method for measuring colour in wild animals: validation and use on chest patch colour in geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 94: Blessing EM, Solomon SG, Hashemi-Nezhad M, Morris BJ, and Martin PR Chromatic and spatial properties of parvocellular cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). The Journal of Physiology 557: Boissinot S, Tan Y, Shyue SK, Schneider H, Sampaio I, Neiswanger K, Hewett-Emmett D, and Li WH Origins and antiquity of X-linked triallelic color vision systems in New World monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95: Bowmaker JK, Astell S, Hunt DM, and Mollon JD Photosensitive and photostable pigments in the retinae of Old World monkeys. Journal of Experimental Biology 156:

Teaching Workshop: Color Vision in Primates and Other Mammals

Teaching Workshop: Color Vision in Primates and Other Mammals Teaching Workshop: Color Vision in Primates and Other Mammals Carrie C. Veilleux & Amber Heard-Booth Anthropology Department, University of Texas at Austin Trichromatic Color Vision Trichromatic Color

More information

DID PRIMATE TRICHROMACY EVOLVE FOR FRUGIVORY OR FOLIVORY?

DID PRIMATE TRICHROMACY EVOLVE FOR FRUGIVORY OR FOLIVORY? CHAPTER 3 DID PRIMATE TRICHROMACY EVOLVE FOR FRUGIVORY OR FOLIVORY? P. SUMNER AND J. D. MOLLON Introduction Primate colour vision Most mammals have two types of cone photopigment, short-wave-sensitive

More information

New World Monkeys and Color

New World Monkeys and Color Int J Primatol (2007) 28:729 759 DOI 10.1007/s10764-007-9168-y New World Monkeys and Color Gerald H. Jacobs Received: 24 March 2006 / Accepted: 6 June 2006 / Published online: 9 August 2007 # Springer

More information

Perspectives in Basic Science

Perspectives in Basic Science Perspectives in Basic Science Colour Vision: Why Are We Primates Unique? Petroc Sumner Colour is such an integral part of our visual experience that most people assume that the colours we see are physical

More information

How the eye sees. Properties of light. The light-gathering parts of the eye. 1. Properties of light. 2. The anatomy of the eye. 3.

How the eye sees. Properties of light. The light-gathering parts of the eye. 1. Properties of light. 2. The anatomy of the eye. 3. How the eye sees 1. Properties of light 2. The anatomy of the eye 3. Visual pigments 4. Color vision 1 Properties of light Light is made up of particles called photons Light travels as waves speed of light

More information

a retinal gross potential, the electroretinogram (ERG), recorded the radiance of a flickering monochromatic test light is

a retinal gross potential, the electroretinogram (ERG), recorded the radiance of a flickering monochromatic test light is Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 84, pp. 2545-2549, April 1987 Neurobiology Inheritance of color vision in a New World monkey (Saimiri sciureus) (photopigments/polymorphism/x chromosome/evolution) GERALD

More information

The effect of colour vision status on the detection and selection of fruits by tamarins (Saguinus spp.)

The effect of colour vision status on the detection and selection of fruits by tamarins (Saguinus spp.) The Journal of Experimental Biology 206, 3159-3165 2003 The Company of Biologists Ltd doi:10.1242/jeb.00536 3159 The effect of colour vision status on the detection and selection of fruits by tamarins

More information

Effect of polymorphic colour vision for fruit detection in the spider monkey Ateles geoffroyi

Effect of polymorphic colour vision for fruit detection in the spider monkey Ateles geoffroyi The Journal of Experimental Biology 207, 2465-2470 Published by The Company of Biologists 2004 doi:10.1242/jeb.01046 2465 Effect of polymorphic colour vision for fruit detection in the spider monkey Ateles

More information

Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution

Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution Scientific American Magazine - March 16, 2009 Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution Analyses of primate visual pigments show that our color vision evolved in an unusual way and that the

More information

Detection of fruit and the selection of primate visual pigments for color vision

Detection of fruit and the selection of primate visual pigments for color vision Detection of fruit and the selection of primate visual pigments for color vision Article (Unspecified) Osorio, D., Smith, A.C., Vorobyev, M. and Buchanan-Smith, H.M. (2004) Detection of fruit and the selection

More information

Your Eye, My Eye, and the Eye of the Aye Aye: Evolution of Human Vision from 65 Million Years Ago to the Present

Your Eye, My Eye, and the Eye of the Aye Aye: Evolution of Human Vision from 65 Million Years Ago to the Present # 75 Your Eye, My Eye, and the Eye of the Aye Aye: Evolution of Human Vision from 65 Million Years Ago to the Present Dr. Christopher Kirk December 2, 2011 Produced by and for Hot Science - Cool Talks

More information

The Case of Color Vision Evolution in New World Monkeys

The Case of Color Vision Evolution in New World Monkeys The Case of Color Vision Evolution in New World Monkeys slide version 2.0 http://www.evo-ed.com About this Case: 1. These slides were created by the Evo-Ed Project: http://www.evo-ed.com 2. Funding for

More information

Color vision perception in the capuchin monkey (Cebus apella): a re-evaluation of procedures using Munsell papers

Color vision perception in the capuchin monkey (Cebus apella): a re-evaluation of procedures using Munsell papers Behavioural Brain Research 129 (2002) 153 157 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Color vision perception in the capuchin monkey (Cebus apella): a re-evaluation of procedures using Munsell papers

More information

A case of achromatopsia. Perceptual Colour Space. Spectral Properties of Light. Subtractive Colour Mixture. Additive Colour Mixture

A case of achromatopsia. Perceptual Colour Space. Spectral Properties of Light. Subtractive Colour Mixture. Additive Colour Mixture A case of achromatopsia The wrongness of everything was disturbing, even disgusting he turned increasingly to black and white foods to black olives and white rice, black coffee and yoghurt. These at least

More information

Perception & Attention Course. George Mather

Perception & Attention Course. George Mather Perception & Attention Course George Mather A case of achromatopsia The wrongness of everything was disturbing, even disgusting he turned increasingly to black and white foods to black olives and white

More information

Color Vision by Prof/Faten zakareia King Saud University Physiology Dept

Color Vision by Prof/Faten zakareia King Saud University Physiology Dept Color Vision by Prof/Faten zakareia King Saud University Physiology Dept Objectives: Define color vision Identify and describe the mechanism of colour vision and the three types of cones, including the

More information

Primate photopigments and primate color vision (opsin genes polymorphism cones evolution)

Primate photopigments and primate color vision (opsin genes polymorphism cones evolution) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 577 581, January 1996 Colloquium Paper This paper was presented at a colloquium entitled Vision: From Photon to Perception, organized by John Dowling, Lubert Stryer

More information

Chapter 13 Polymorphism and Adaptation of Primate Colour Vision

Chapter 13 Polymorphism and Adaptation of Primate Colour Vision Chapter 13 Polymorphism and Adaptation of Primate Colour Vision Amanda D. Melin, Chihiro Hiramatsu, Linda M. Fedigan, Colleen M. Schaffner, Filippo Aureli and Shoji Kawamura Abstract Opsins provide an

More information

EVOLUTIONARY GENETICS (Genome 453) Midterm Exam Name KEY

EVOLUTIONARY GENETICS (Genome 453) Midterm Exam Name KEY PLEASE: Put your name on every page and SHOW YOUR WORK. Also, lots of space is provided, but you do not have to fill it all! Note that the details of these problems are fictional, for exam purposes only.

More information

Evolutionary Trade-Offs in Mammalian Sensory Perceptions: Visual Pathways of Bats. By Adam Proctor Mentor: Dr. Emma Teeling

Evolutionary Trade-Offs in Mammalian Sensory Perceptions: Visual Pathways of Bats. By Adam Proctor Mentor: Dr. Emma Teeling Evolutionary Trade-Offs in Mammalian Sensory Perceptions: Visual Pathways of Bats By Adam Proctor Mentor: Dr. Emma Teeling Visual Pathways of Bats Purpose Background on mammalian vision Tradeoffs and bats

More information

ANTHR 1L Biological Anthropology Lab

ANTHR 1L Biological Anthropology Lab ANTHR 1L Biological Anthropology Lab Name: DEFINING THE ORDER PRIMATES Humans belong to the zoological Order Primates, which is one of the 18 Orders of the Class Mammalia. Today we will review some of

More information

Bi156 Lecture 1/13/12. Dog Genetics

Bi156 Lecture 1/13/12. Dog Genetics Bi156 Lecture 1/13/12 Dog Genetics The radiation of the family Canidae occurred about 100 million years ago. Dogs are most closely related to wolves, from which they diverged through domestication about

More information

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Elk (Cervus canadensis) Extra credit assignment. Sad Underwing (Catocala maestosa) 10/11/2017

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Elk (Cervus canadensis) Extra credit assignment. Sad Underwing (Catocala maestosa) 10/11/2017 PSY 2364 Animal Communication Elk (Cervus canadensis) Kingdom: Phylum: Class: Order: Family: Genus: Species: Animalia Chordata Mammalia Artiodactyla Cervidae Cervus canadensis Extra credit assignment Sad

More information

NAME: DATE: SECTION:

NAME: DATE: SECTION: NAME: DATE: SECTION: MCAS PREP PACKET EVOLUTION AND BIODIVERSITY 1. Which of the following observations best supports the conclusion that dolphins and sharks do not have a recent common ancestor? A. Dolphins

More information

Ecology and Genetics of Color Vision in Callicebus brunneus, a Neotropical Monkey

Ecology and Genetics of Color Vision in Callicebus brunneus, a Neotropical Monkey Ecology and Genetics of Color Vision in Callicebus brunneus, a Neotropical Monkey By JOHN ANDREW BUNCE B.S. Bates College 2002 M.A. University of California, Davis 2004 DISSERTATION Submitted in partial

More information

The effect of colour vision status on insect prey capture efficiency by. captive and wild tamarins (Saguinus spp.)

The effect of colour vision status on insect prey capture efficiency by. captive and wild tamarins (Saguinus spp.) 1 2 The effect of colour vision status on insect prey capture efficiency by captive and wild tamarins (Saguinus spp.) 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 Abstract The colour vision polymorphism

More information

CLADISTICS Student Packet SUMMARY Phylogeny Phylogenetic trees/cladograms

CLADISTICS Student Packet SUMMARY Phylogeny Phylogenetic trees/cladograms CLADISTICS Student Packet SUMMARY PHYLOGENETIC TREES AND CLADOGRAMS ARE MODELS OF EVOLUTIONARY HISTORY THAT CAN BE TESTED Phylogeny is the history of descent of organisms from their common ancestor. Phylogenetic

More information

1.5 C: Role of the Environment in Evolution Quiz

1.5 C: Role of the Environment in Evolution Quiz 1. Numbers of reported cases of bedbug infestations have been increasing over the past ten years in the United States. In an attempt to combat the infestations, people began using pesticides to kill the

More information

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females By Scientific American, adapted by Newsela staff on 02.06.17 Word Count 779 Mandarin ducks, a male (left) and a female, at WWT Martin

More information

Effects of Natural Selection

Effects of Natural Selection Effects of Natural Selection Lesson Plan for Secondary Science Teachers Created by Christine Taylor And Mark Urban University of Connecticut Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Funded by the

More information

Chromatic discrimination in young carriers of red-green colour vision deficiencies

Chromatic discrimination in young carriers of red-green colour vision deficiencies Chromatic discrimination in young carriers of red-green colour vision deficiencies By Elise Wiken Dees A thesis submitted to Buskerud University College for the degree of Master of Philosophy 2010 Department

More information

1 This question is about the evolution, genetics, behaviour and physiology of cats.

1 This question is about the evolution, genetics, behaviour and physiology of cats. 1 This question is about the evolution, genetics, behaviour and physiology of cats. Fig. 1.1 (on the insert) shows a Scottish wildcat, Felis sylvestris. Modern domestic cats evolved from a wild ancestor

More information

SKELETONS: Museum of Osteology Tooth and Eye Dentification Teacher Resource

SKELETONS: Museum of Osteology Tooth and Eye Dentification Teacher Resource SKELETONS: Museum of Osteology Tooth and Eye Dentification Teacher Resource Grade Levels: 3 rd 5 th Grade 3 rd Grade: SC.3.N.1.1 - Raise questions about the natural world, investigate them individually

More information

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc 1. The money in the kingdom of Florin consists of bills with the value written on the front, and pictures of members of the royal family on the back. To test the hypothesis that all of the Florinese $5

More information

Comparing DNA Sequences Cladogram Practice

Comparing DNA Sequences Cladogram Practice Name Period Assignment # See lecture questions 75, 122-123, 127, 137 Comparing DNA Sequences Cladogram Practice BACKGROUND Between 1990 2003, scientists working on an international research project known

More information

Lecture 11 Wednesday, September 19, 2012

Lecture 11 Wednesday, September 19, 2012 Lecture 11 Wednesday, September 19, 2012 Phylogenetic tree (phylogeny) Darwin and classification: In the Origin, Darwin said that descent from a common ancestral species could explain why the Linnaean

More information

Feeding the Commercial Egg-Type Replacement Pullet 1

Feeding the Commercial Egg-Type Replacement Pullet 1 PS48 Feeding the Commercial Egg-Type Replacement Pullet 1 Richard D. Miles and Jacqueline P. Jacob 2 TODAY'S PULLET Advances in genetic selection make today's pullets quite different from those of only

More information

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING.

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING. MIDTERM EXAM 1 100 points total (6 questions) 8 pages PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING. PLEASE NOTE: YOU MUST ANSWER QUESTIONS 1-4 AND EITHER QUESTION 5 OR

More information

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below.

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below. IDTER EXA 1 100 points total (6 questions) Problem 1. (20 points) In this pedigree, colorblindness is represented by horizontal hatching, and is determined by an X-linked recessive gene (g); the dominant

More information

Spot the Difference: Using the domestic cat as a model for the nutritional management of captive cheetahs. Katherine M. Bell

Spot the Difference: Using the domestic cat as a model for the nutritional management of captive cheetahs. Katherine M. Bell Spot the Difference: Using the domestic cat as a model for the nutritional management of captive cheetahs Katherine M. Bell Edited by Lucy A. Tucker and David G. Thomas Illustrated by Justine Woosnam and

More information

TOPIC CLADISTICS

TOPIC CLADISTICS TOPIC 5.4 - CLADISTICS 5.4 A Clades & Cladograms https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/4/46/clade-grade_ii.svg IB BIO 5.4 3 U1: A clade is a group of organisms that have evolved from a common

More information

BioSci 110, Fall 08 Exam 2

BioSci 110, Fall 08 Exam 2 1. is the cell division process that results in the production of a. mitosis; 2 gametes b. meiosis; 2 gametes c. meiosis; 2 somatic (body) cells d. mitosis; 4 somatic (body) cells e. *meiosis; 4 gametes

More information

Biodiversity and Distributions. Lecture 2: Biodiversity. The process of natural selection

Biodiversity and Distributions. Lecture 2: Biodiversity. The process of natural selection Lecture 2: Biodiversity What is biological diversity? Natural selection Adaptive radiations and convergent evolution Biogeography Biodiversity and Distributions Types of biological diversity: Genetic diversity

More information

Dacnis cayana (Blue Dacnis or Turquoise Honeycreeper)

Dacnis cayana (Blue Dacnis or Turquoise Honeycreeper) Dacnis cayana (Blue Dacnis or Turquoise Honeycreeper) Family: Thraupidae (Tanagers and Honeycreepers) Order: Passeriformes (Perching Birds) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig.1. Blue dacnis, Dacnis cayana, male (top)

More information

TECHNICAL BULLETIN Claude Toudic Broiler Specialist June 2006

TECHNICAL BULLETIN Claude Toudic Broiler Specialist June 2006 Evaluating uniformity in broilers factors affecting variation During a technical visit to a broiler farm the topic of uniformity is generally assessed visually and subjectively, as to do the job properly

More information

Phenotype Observed Expected (O-E) 2 (O-E) 2 /E dotted yellow solid yellow dotted blue solid blue

Phenotype Observed Expected (O-E) 2 (O-E) 2 /E dotted yellow solid yellow dotted blue solid blue 1. (30 pts) A tropical fish breeder for the local pet store is interested in creating a new type of fancy tropical fish. She observes consistent patterns of inheritance for the following traits: P 1 :

More information

Although owls can t move their eyes, many other adaptations help these raptors spot prey.

Although owls can t move their eyes, many other adaptations help these raptors spot prey. This website would like to remind you: Your browser (Apple Safari 7) is out of date. Update your browser for more security, comfort and the best experience on this site. Media Spotlight Bird s Eye View

More information

One group (Tarsiers) is off on it's own (note clear where they belong). All tarsiers are endangered or threatened to some extent.

One group (Tarsiers) is off on it's own (note clear where they belong). All tarsiers are endangered or threatened to some extent. Primates, part II Suborder Haplorrhini Divided into two clades (Infraorders?) Platyrrhini (flat noses, with nostrils to side) Catarrhini (nostrils pointing down) One group (Tarsiers) is off on it's own

More information

LAB. NATURAL SELECTION

LAB. NATURAL SELECTION Period Date LAB. NATURAL SELECTION This game was invented by G. Ledyard Stebbins, a pioneer in the evolution of plants. The purpose of the game is to illustrate the basic principles and some of the general

More information

Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1

Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1 Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1 Learning the rules of inheritance is at the core of all biologists training. These rules allow geneticists to predict the patterns

More information

Worksheet for Morgan/Carter Laboratory #9 Mendelian Genetics II: Drosophila

Worksheet for Morgan/Carter Laboratory #9 Mendelian Genetics II: Drosophila Worksheet for Morgan/Carter Laboratory #9 Mendelian Genetics II: Drosophila Ex. 9-1: ESTABLISHING THE ENZYME REACTION CONTROLS Propose a hypothesis about AO activity in flies from vial 1a and flies from

More information

EOQ 3 Exam Review. Genetics: 1. What is a phenotype? 2. What is a genotype?

EOQ 3 Exam Review. Genetics: 1. What is a phenotype? 2. What is a genotype? EOQ 3 Exam Review Genetics: 1. What is a phenotype? 2. What is a genotype? 3. The allele for freckles (f) is recessive to not having freckles (F). Both parents have freckles but only 3 of their 4 children

More information

Biodiversity and Extinction. Lecture 9

Biodiversity and Extinction. Lecture 9 Biodiversity and Extinction Lecture 9 This lecture will help you understand: The scope of Earth s biodiversity Levels and patterns of biodiversity Mass extinction vs background extinction Attributes of

More information

Lab 7. Evolution Lab. Name: General Introduction:

Lab 7. Evolution Lab. Name: General Introduction: Lab 7 Name: Evolution Lab OBJECTIVES: Help you develop an understanding of important factors that affect evolution of a species. Demonstrate important biological and environmental selection factors that

More information

Name(s): Period: Date:

Name(s): Period: Date: Evolution in Action: Antibiotic Resistance HASPI Medical Biology Lab 21 Background/Introduction Evolution and Natural Selection Evolution is one of the driving factors in biology. It is simply the concept

More information

Title: Phylogenetic Methods and Vertebrate Phylogeny

Title: Phylogenetic Methods and Vertebrate Phylogeny Title: Phylogenetic Methods and Vertebrate Phylogeny Central Question: How can evolutionary relationships be determined objectively? Sub-questions: 1. What affect does the selection of the outgroup have

More information

Biol 160: Lab 7. Modeling Evolution

Biol 160: Lab 7. Modeling Evolution Name: Modeling Evolution OBJECTIVES Help you develop an understanding of important factors that affect evolution of a species. Demonstrate important biological and environmental selection factors that

More information

2013 Holiday Lectures on Science Medicine in the Genomic Era

2013 Holiday Lectures on Science Medicine in the Genomic Era INTRODUCTION Figure 1. Tasha. Scientists sequenced the first canine genome using DNA from a boxer named Tasha. Meet Tasha, a boxer dog (Figure 1). In 2005, scientists obtained the first complete dog genome

More information

Bio homework #5. Biology Homework #5

Bio homework #5. Biology Homework #5 Biology Homework #5 Bio homework #5 The information presented during the first five weeks of INS is very important and will be useful to know in the future (next quarter and beyond).the purpose of this

More information

Biology *P40125RA0116* P40125RA. Unit: 4BI0 Paper: 2B. Edexcel International GCSE. Tuesday 10 January 2012 Afternoon Time: 1 hour.

Biology *P40125RA0116* P40125RA. Unit: 4BI0 Paper: 2B. Edexcel International GCSE. Tuesday 10 January 2012 Afternoon Time: 1 hour. Write your name here Surname Other names Edexcel International GCSE Biology Unit: 4BI0 Paper: 2B Centre Number Candidate Number Tuesday 10 January 2012 Afternoon Time: 1 hour You must have: Calculator.

More information

Talks generally last minutes and take place in one of our classrooms.

Talks generally last minutes and take place in one of our classrooms. Key Stage 1 & Key Stage 2 REPTILES General points about this talk: Talks generally last 30-40 minutes and take place in one of our classrooms. Talks are generally lead by the keepers on this section so

More information

The Development of Behavior

The Development of Behavior The Development of Behavior 0 people liked this 0 discussions READING ASSIGNMENT Read this assignment. Though you've already read the textbook reading assignment that accompanies this assignment, you may

More information

Introduction to phylogenetic trees and tree-thinking Copyright 2005, D. A. Baum (Free use for non-commercial educational pruposes)

Introduction to phylogenetic trees and tree-thinking Copyright 2005, D. A. Baum (Free use for non-commercial educational pruposes) Introduction to phylogenetic trees and tree-thinking Copyright 2005, D. A. Baum (Free use for non-commercial educational pruposes) Phylogenetics is the study of the relationships of organisms to each other.

More information

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified):

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified): Modification of Brain Circuits as a Result of Experience Chapter 24, Purves et al. 4 th Ed. Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified): (1/6/2010) Mona Buhusi Postnatal

More information

Biology 2108 Laboratory Exercises: Variation in Natural Systems. LABORATORY 2 Evolution: Genetic Variation within Species

Biology 2108 Laboratory Exercises: Variation in Natural Systems. LABORATORY 2 Evolution: Genetic Variation within Species Biology 2108 Laboratory Exercises: Variation in Natural Systems Ed Bostick Don Davis Marcus C. Davis Joe Dirnberger Bill Ensign Ben Golden Lynelle Golden Paula Jackson Ron Matson R.C. Paul Pam Rhyne Gail

More information

Testing Phylogenetic Hypotheses with Molecular Data 1

Testing Phylogenetic Hypotheses with Molecular Data 1 Testing Phylogenetic Hypotheses with Molecular Data 1 How does an evolutionary biologist quantify the timing and pathways for diversification (speciation)? If we observe diversification today, the processes

More information

The Effect of Aerial Exposure Temperature on Balanus balanoides Feeding Behavior

The Effect of Aerial Exposure Temperature on Balanus balanoides Feeding Behavior The Effect of Aerial Exposure Temperature on Balanus balanoides Feeding Behavior Gracie Thompson* and Matt Goldberg Monday Afternoon Biology 334A Laboratory, Fall 2014 Abstract The impact of climate change

More information

Establishing a routine

Establishing a routine Establishing a routine As already mentioned, dogs are creatures of habit, and it s a good idea to establish a daily routine for your Cockapoo as soon as possible. This will also simplify house-training;

More information

Animal Behavior and Evolution

Animal Behavior and Evolution nimal ehavior and Evolution Name: ate: 1. Western coral snakes have a striped color pattern and are poisonous. rizona mountain kingsnakes look like western coral snakes but are not poisonous. The color

More information

COMPARING DNA SEQUENCES TO UNDERSTAND EVOLUTIONARY RELATIONSHIPS WITH BLAST

COMPARING DNA SEQUENCES TO UNDERSTAND EVOLUTIONARY RELATIONSHIPS WITH BLAST COMPARING DNA SEQUENCES TO UNDERSTAND EVOLUTIONARY RELATIONSHIPS WITH BLAST In this laboratory investigation, you will use BLAST to compare several genes, and then use the information to construct a cladogram.

More information

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush Sample paper critique #2 The article by Hayes, Nahrung and Wilson 1 investigates the response of three rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush rat), Uromys

More information

Yes, heterozygous organisms can pass a dominant allele onto the offspring. Only one dominant allele is needed to have the dominant genotype.

Yes, heterozygous organisms can pass a dominant allele onto the offspring. Only one dominant allele is needed to have the dominant genotype. Name: Period: Unit 4: Inheritance of Traits Scopes 9-10: Inheritance and Mutations 1. What is an organism that has two dominant alleles for a trait? Homozygous dominant Give an example of an organism with

More information

Evolution. Evolution is change in organisms over time. Evolution does not have a goal; it is often shaped by natural selection (see below).

Evolution. Evolution is change in organisms over time. Evolution does not have a goal; it is often shaped by natural selection (see below). Evolution Evolution is change in organisms over time. Evolution does not have a goal; it is often shaped by natural selection (see below). Species an interbreeding population of organisms that can produce

More information

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics OVERVIEW This activity serves as a supplement to the film The Origin of Species: The Beak of the Finch and provides students with the opportunity to develop

More information

Darwin and the Family Tree of Animals

Darwin and the Family Tree of Animals Darwin and the Family Tree of Animals Note: These links do not work. Use the links within the outline to access the images in the popup windows. This text is the same as the scrolling text in the popup

More information

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color Madeleine van der Heyden, Kimberly Debriansky, and Randall Clarke

More information

Comments on the Ridge Gene, by Clayton Heathcock; February 15, 2008

Comments on the Ridge Gene, by Clayton Heathcock; February 15, 2008 Comments on the Ridge Gene, by Clayton Heathcock; February 15, 2008 Note: This article originally appeared in the March 2008 issue of "The Ridgeback", the official publication of the Rhodesian Ridgeback

More information

Chapter 1: The Field Trip

Chapter 1: The Field Trip Chapter 1: The Field Trip We have twenty minutes until we have to be back at the bus, Lisa said, shielding the sun from her eyes with the back of one hand as she glanced up from the zoo map. Is there anything

More information

Our Origins Discovering Physical Anthropology

Our Origins Discovering Physical Anthropology W. W. Norton & Company Our Origins Discovering Physical Anthropology Second Edition by Clark Spencer Larsen Chapter 7 Clark Spencer Larsen Our Origins DISCOVERING PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY C. Milner-Rose Chapter

More information

Reproductive physiology and eggs

Reproductive physiology and eggs Reproductive physiology and eggs Class Business Reading for this lecture Required. Gill: Chapter 14 1. Reproductive physiology In lecture I will only have time to go over reproductive physiology briefly,

More information

Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD

Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD Glossary Gene = A piece of DNA that provides the 'recipe' for an enzyme or a protein. Gene locus = The position of a gene on a chromosome.

More information

Biology 164 Laboratory

Biology 164 Laboratory Biology 164 Laboratory CATLAB: Computer Model for Inheritance of Coat and Tail Characteristics in Domestic Cats (Based on simulation developed by Judith Kinnear, University of Sydney, NSW, Australia) Introduction

More information

The Inheritance of Coat Colour in the Cardigan Welsh Corgi by Ken Linacre

The Inheritance of Coat Colour in the Cardigan Welsh Corgi by Ken Linacre The Inheritance of Coat Colour in the Cardigan Welsh Corgi by Ken Linacre In a working dog, colour is undoubtedly of secondary importance to construction, but the wide range of colours found in the Cardigan

More information

Evolution by Natural Selection

Evolution by Natural Selection Evolution by Natural Selection 2006-2007 DOCTRINE TINTORETTO The Creation of the Animals 1550 But the Fossil record OBSERVATION Anaerobic Bacteria Photosynthetic Bacteria Dinosaurs Green Algae Multicellular

More information

Do the traits of organisms provide evidence for evolution?

Do the traits of organisms provide evidence for evolution? PhyloStrat Tutorial Do the traits of organisms provide evidence for evolution? Consider two hypotheses about where Earth s organisms came from. The first hypothesis is from John Ray, an influential British

More information

Chapter 22 Darwin and Evolution by Natural Selection

Chapter 22 Darwin and Evolution by Natural Selection Anaerobic Bacteria Photosynthetic Bacteria Dinosaurs Green Algae Multicellular Animals Flowering Molluscs Arthropods Chordates Jawless Fish Teleost Fish Amphibians Insects Reptiles Mammals Birds Land Plants

More information

Breeding Icelandic Sheepdog article for ISIC 2012 Wilma Roem

Breeding Icelandic Sheepdog article for ISIC 2012 Wilma Roem Breeding Icelandic Sheepdog article for ISIC 2012 Wilma Roem Icelandic Sheepdog breeders should have two high priority objectives: The survival of the breed and the health of the breed. In this article

More information

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics Objectives In today s lab you will explore some of the simpler principles of Mendelian genetics using a computer program called CATLAB. By the end of this

More information

RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS.

RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS. RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS. Published on the AWEBSA webpage with the kind permission of the author: Robert Manvell. Please visit his page and view photos of

More information

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production May 2013 Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Michael Longley, Global Technical Transfer Manager Summary Introduction Chick numbers are most often reduced during the period

More information

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production Michael Longley, Global Technical Transfer Manager May 2013 SUMMARY Introduction Chick numbers are most often reduced during the period

More information

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING J. exp. Biol. 180, 247-251 (1993) Printed in Great Britain The Company of Biologists Limited 1993 247 BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING AUD THESEN, JOHAN B. STEEN* and KJELL B. DØVING Division

More information

AP Biology. AP Biology

AP Biology. AP Biology Evolution by Natural Selection 2006-2007 DOCTRINE TINTORETTO The Creation of the Animals 1550 But the Fossil record OBSERVATION mya Quaternary 1.5 Tertiary 63 Cretaceous 135 Jurassic 180 Triassic 225 Permian

More information

FEEDING EWES BETTER FOR INCREASED PRODUCTION AND PROFIT. Dr. Dan Morrical Department of Animal Science Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa

FEEDING EWES BETTER FOR INCREASED PRODUCTION AND PROFIT. Dr. Dan Morrical Department of Animal Science Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa FEEDING EWES BETTER FOR INCREASED PRODUCTION AND PROFIT Dr. Dan Morrical Department of Animal Science Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa Introduction Sheep nutrition and feeding is extremely critical to

More information

Activity 1: Changes in beak size populations in low precipitation

Activity 1: Changes in beak size populations in low precipitation Darwin s Finches Lab Work individually or in groups of -3 at a computer Introduction The finches on Darwin and Wallace Islands feed on seeds produced by plants growing on these islands. There are three

More information

Beyond Mendel. Extending Mendelian Genetics. Incomplete Dominance. Think about this. Beyond Mendel. Chapter 12

Beyond Mendel. Extending Mendelian Genetics. Incomplete Dominance. Think about this. Beyond Mendel. Chapter 12 Beyond Mendel Extending Mendelian Genetics Chapter 12 Mendel s work did, however, provide a basis for discovering the passing of traits in other ways including: Incomplete Dominance Codominance Polygenic

More information

Mendelian Genetics Part 4: Dihybrid Cross

Mendelian Genetics Part 4: Dihybrid Cross Mendelian Genetics Part 4: Dihybrid Cross Name Terms and Explanations Explain the following terms and concepts, using both a diagram and an explanation in sentences or statements: Monohybrid cross Meiosis

More information

Mendelian Genetics SI

Mendelian Genetics SI Name Mendelian Genetics SI Date 1. In sheep, eye color is controlled by a single gene with two alleles. When a homozygous brown-eyed sheep is crossed with a homozygous green-eyed sheep, blue-eyed offspring

More information

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19, AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19, 575-582 SHIFTS OF 'ATTENTION' IN CHICKS DURING FEEDING BY MARIAN DAWKINS Department of Zoology, University of Oxford Abstract. Feeding in 'runs' of and grains suggested the possibility

More information

Interpreting Evolutionary Trees Honors Integrated Science 4 Name Per.

Interpreting Evolutionary Trees Honors Integrated Science 4 Name Per. Interpreting Evolutionary Trees Honors Integrated Science 4 Name Per. Introduction Imagine a single diagram representing the evolutionary relationships between everything that has ever lived. If life evolved

More information