Importance of internal pattern contrast and contrast against the background in aposematic signals

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Importance of internal pattern contrast and contrast against the background in aposematic signals"

Transcription

1 Behavioral Ecology doi: /beheco/arp141 Advance Access publication 2 November 2009 Importance of internal pattern contrast and contrast against the background in aposematic signals Marianne Aronsson and Gabriella Gamberale-Stille Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden Aposematic color patterns that signal prey unprofitability are suggested to work best when there is high contrast within the animal color pattern or between the animal and its background. Studies show that prey contrast against the background increases the signal efficiency. This has occasionally been extended to also explain the presence of internal patterns. We used domestic chicks, Gallus gallus domesticus, to investigate the relative importance for avoidance learning of within-prey pattern contrast and prey contrast against the background. In a series of trials, birds were first trained to avoid artificially made aposematic mealworms that were plain red or red with black stripes, and to discriminate them from palatable brown mealworms, on either a red or a brown background. Second, we investigated how the birds generalized between striped and nonstriped prey. The chicks showed faster avoidance learning when the basic color of the aposematic prey (red) contrasted with the background color (brown). However, there was no similar effect of internal pattern contrast. The generalization test showed a complete generalization between the nonstriped and the striped prey. We conclude that contrasting internal patterns do not necessarily affect predator avoidance learning the same way as shown for prey-to-background contrast in aposematic prey. Key words: avoidance learning, conspicuousness, domestic chick, generalization behavior, internal pattern contrast, signal design, warning coloration. [Behav Ecol 20: (2009)] There are several different strategies used in animal communication, both within and between species. Some use visual signals, whereas others make use of audio and olfactory signals (Guilford 1990; Searcy and Nowicki 2005). Most studies on aposematism have focused on the concept of warning coloration where animals signal their unprofitability to potential predators by having a conspicuous color pattern (Guilford 1990). It has been argued that the function of this conspicuousness is to increase signal efficiency, because warning signals, as all signals, benefit by being easy to detect, discriminate, and memorize by the intended receivers (Guilford and Dawkins 1991). Aposematic animals often have conspicuous markings of bright colors such as red, orange, or yellow, often in combination with a black pattern (Cott 1940). Having such bright hues that contrast strongly against a typical green or brown background makes them easy to recognize and discriminate both from the background and from the palatable and camouflaged prey that the predators usually hunt (Turner 1975; Sherratt and Beatty 2003). An aposematic pattern is suggested to work best when it shows a high contrast within the color pattern itself or a high contrast against the background (Endler 1991). There are many experimental studies suggesting that the conspicuousness of aposematic prey, specifically in the form of contrast against the background, increases signal efficiency (Ruxton et al. 2004). Unpalatable prey items that contrast against the background have been shown to increase initial wariness (Roper and Cook 1989; Lindström et al. 2001), increase the speed and durability of avoidance learning (Gittleman and Harvey 1980; Gittleman et al. 1980; Roper and Wistow 1986; Address correspondence to G. Gamberale-Stille. gabriella. gamberale@zoologi.su.se. Received 18 February 2009; revised 1 September 2009; accepted 19 September Ó The Author Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org Roper and Redston 1987; Roper and Cook 1989; Riipi et al. 2001), and decrease recognition errors (Gamberale-Stille 2001). However, these effects of background contrast have been suggested to also explain the existence of contrasting internal color patterns (Guilford 1990). Guilford (1990) argues that prey by producing its own internal color boundaries of high contrast will promote conspicuousness and thus the aposematic effect. Still, to our knowledge, there is little experimental evidence for such a corresponding effect of conspicuousness generated by internal pattern contrasts. However, one recent study with domestic chicks as predators implies that the presence of a black striped pattern may improve avoidance learning when presented together with some specific colors (green) but not with others (yellow) (Hauglund et al. 2006). Additionally, a study by Mason (1988) suggests that a more complex stimulus, consisting of both a black pattern and a conspicuous color, may produce a more durable avoidance in wild-caught red-winged blackbirds. That internal pattern contrast is equivalent to increased conspicuousness is an untested conclusion. There are other hypotheses that conflict with this idea. For instance, Papageorgis (1975) suggested that patterns makes an animal look cryptic at longer distance and at the same time remain conspicuous at a shorter distance. In the case of disruptive cryptic coloration, contrasting patterns may be used to camouflage the body shape and thus decrease conspicuousness (Cott 1940; Edmunds 1974), and Kassarov (2003) argues that a prey can reduce its conspicuousness by having a pattern, because stripes, for instance, blur together when looked at from a distance. In snakes, stripes are thought to confuse the predator because they make it difficult to visually focus on any specific point along the animal and thereby making it difficult to tell the direction and speed of the animal when moving (Wolf and Werner 1994; Creer 2005). In connection to this, some recent studies suggest that birds may need color to recognize prey as aposematic and that they may disregard the presence of a black pattern (Exnerová et al. 2006;

2 Aronsson and Gamberale-Stille Contrasting patterns in aposematism 1357 Aronsson and Gamberale-Stille 2008). If this is true, taken together with the fact that there is an effect of signal size on bird avoidance of an aposematic prey (Gamberale and Tullberg 1996, 1998; Forsman and Merilaita 1999; Gamberale- Stille 2000) and that the size of a signaling area affects detectability (Robson and Graham 1981; Polat and Tyler 1999), the presence of a pattern may potentially decrease the aposematic effect, and/or conspicuousness. The aim of the present study is to investigate the separate effects of prey-to-background contrast and within-prey pattern contrast on avoidance learning in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). This in order to determine if one can generalize positive aposematic effects of conspicuousness generated by contrast against the background to the presence of internal color patterns. In a series of avoidance learning trials, birds are trained to discriminate aposematic plain red or red-and-black striped mealworms (Tenebrio molitor) from palatable brown mealworms either on a red or on a brown background. This experimental setup is similar to that of Gittleman et al. (1980) in the sense that in both studies, birds were presented with a situation where either the color of the palatable or the unpalatable prey matched or contrasted with the background. Thus, if contrast against the background increases avoidance learning, we expect the chicks presented with unpalatable prey on a contrasting backgroundtolearnfasterthanbirdspresentedwithunpalatable prey on a matching background. Likewise, if internal color patterns increase avoidance learning, we expect birds presented with striped unpalatable prey to learn faster than birds presented with nonstriped unpalatable prey, especially on the background where the color of the unpalatable prey otherwise would match the background. After avoidance learning, we also conduct a generalization test to investigate how the birds generalize between striped and nonstriped prey. In an additional control experiment, we also investigate if the black pattern used in the above mentioned experiment is visible to domestic chicks. It is important for the interpretation of the results to know that the pattern used in the experiment is actually perceivable by the birds. MATERIALS AND METHODS Predators We used a total of 60 male domestic chicks as predators that arrived newly hatched and unfed from a commercial hatchery in batches of about 10 individuals. The chicks were housed in cages ( cm, length 3 width 3 height) with the floor covered with wood chips and heated with a 60 W carbon filament lamp. The housing and experimental room was lit by daylight from the windows, by daylight-strip lights (BIOLUX L36W/72, OSRAM, München, Germany, now very low in UV), and 4 fluorescent strip-lights (L36W/73, OSRAM, emits light between 350 and 420 nm), between 08:00 and 18:00 h. They were fed chick Starter crumbs (Pullfor, Lantmannen, Stockholm, Sweden) and water ad lib. On the day of arrival, the chicks were individually marked with numbers on their backs using a black marker pen. The markings did not cause any increased pecking by other individuals on this area. Prey We used painted dead mealworms as prey, placed on colored paper backgrounds. Prey were painted using nontoxic children s paint (Gouache Tempera, Color & Co, Lefranc and Bourgeois, Le Mans, France). The mealworms were first killed by freezing them for a couple of hours, after which about half of them were painted brown ( Burnt Umber ) and then returned to the freezer. These mealworms were palatable to the chicks. The remaining mealworms were first made distasteful by injecting 0.02 ml of 3% solution of quinine hydrochloride after which they were painted red ( Brilliant Red ) and returned to the freezer. We also painted half of the red mealworms with 2 black stripes ( Black ) across them. The stripes were painted so that the red-black-red-black-red areas were of approximately the same size (Figure 1). Finally, to increase the distastefulness of both the nonstriped and striped mealworms they were painted with 3 drops of Stop n Grow (Mentholatum, East Kilbride, Scotland). Stop n Grow is a bitter tasting, nontoxic, solution used to stop nail-biting and contains the bitter substances denatonium benzoate and quinine sulfate, both of which have been used in previous studies to make prey unpalatable to birds (e.g., Marples and Roper 1997; Skelhorn and Rowe 2005). The mealworms were collected from the freezer about half an hour before each trial to make sure that they were unfrozen and that the paint was dry. The background paper that covered the bottom of the Petri dishes around the runway were of 2 different colors, brown and red, and were chosen to match the colors of the painted mealworms as much as possible (Figure 2). We measured the reflectance of the paints and backgrounds as the percentage of the reflectance of a white standard from the painted mealworms (with Stop n Grow) and background papers using an Ocean Optics (Dunedin, FL) USB 2000 spectrophotometer and PX-2 pulsed xenon light source (Figure 2). This to make sure that the red paint was more similar to the red background paper than to the brown paper as well as the other way around and to control for possible UV reflection. Experimental arena The preexperimental training and the experiment took place in a square shaped arena ( cm high) made of wood, with a 25-cm-wide runway surrounding a smaller enclosure with net walls. This enclosure was used during the preexperimental training for companion chicks. There were 16 circular wells, 4 cm in diameter, sunk about 1.5 cm into the floor, and spaced uniformly along the runway. These wells contained Petri dishes in which the prey were presented on differently colored backgrounds. The experimental arena was situated in the same room as the housing cages. Preexperimental training Before the experiment started the chicks were trained to forage alone for mealworms in the wells along the experimental runway. First, on the day of arrival the chicks received live mealworms in their home cages to get familiarized with the prey. The training in the arena started on the day after arrival and consisted of 5 training sessions distributed more than 2 days. Before the first session, all chicks were divided into 2 groups where half would be trained to forage for prey on brown background and the other half on red background. Throughout the preexperimental training, the avoidance learning trials and the generalization test, each chick only encountered one of the 2 possible background colors. In the first session, groups of 3 chicks at a time were allowed into the arena to forage for live mealworms that were presented 3 in each well. The rest of the birds in the batch that would encounter the same background color acted as companions during this first session and were kept in the companion enclosure of the experimental arena with food and water ad lib. During the afternoon of the first day of arena training, there were 2 consecutive sessions where birds in groups of 5 were allowed to forage

3 1358 Behavioral Ecology alone without companions. Between the morning and afternoon sessions, all chicks were left undisturbed in their home cage for a minimum of 1 h, when no experiments at all took place. Figure 1 The experimental setup. There were 4 treatment groups (column 1), 2 of which received prey on a brown background and 2 on a red background (column 2). The experimental subjects participated in 5 trials of avoidance learning (column 3) with brown palatable prey (S1) and either red nonstriped or red-and-black-striped unpalatable prey (S2). The numbers of prey items encountered in each trial of avoidance learning is given underneath the prey in column 3. After the avoidance learning, birds took part in a generalization test (column 4) where all chicks were presented all 3 kinds of prey (all palatable); brown, red nonstriped, and red and black striped on their respective background color. The numbers of prey present in the generalization test is given to the right of each prey type. together along the runway. During these sessions, and the rest of the training in the arena, the chicks were always presented with single dead, brown painted mealworms in each well. As an addition, at the end of the first training day the chicks also received brown painted mealworms in their home cage to get even more familiarized with the palatable prey used in the experiment. On the second day of arena training there were 2 sessions. During these sessions the chicks were always alone in the runway and in the first session there were 2 chicks in the companion enclosure, but in the last session there were no companions present at all. Additionally, in this last training trial, and for the remainder of the experiment, the walls of the runway that previously had allowed birds to view into the companion enclosure were now covered with plywood boards. After the preexperimental training, all chicks had learned to forage Experimental procedure Before the start of the experiment, the chicks in the 2 background groups (Br and R) were subdivided into 2 new treatment groups each; nonstriped (ns) and striped (S). Thus, there were 4 experimental groups in the experiment, brown/nonstriped (Br/nS), brown/striped (Br/S), red/nonstriped (R/nS), and finally red/striped (R/S) (Figure 1). Each batch of chicks was evenly divided between treatment groups with the final sample sizes of 15 per group (N ¼ 15). The experiment consisted of 2 parts, starting with avoidance learning for 5 trials and then a generalization test under extinction conditions. The experiment started on the fourth day with one trial in the afternoon of each consecutive day. There was no food deprivation before the trials. During the avoidance learning, half of the 16 wells had the positive stimulus with a palatable mealworm, and the other half had a negative stimulus with a distasteful mealworm. The palatable and the distasteful prey were evenly distributed along the wells of the runway, with no more than 2 of the same prey type in succession. Each trial session started at a different position along the runway. The trial ended when the chick passed the 16th well and the number of mealworms attacked, eaten, or pecked at was noted for each trial. After the last avoidance learning trial, all chicks took part in a generalization test, under extinction conditions, to investigate if the presence of a pattern in the aposematic prey affected the chicks generalization behavior. The chicks were presented with both nonstriped and striped mealworms as well as the brown that they knew as palatable; however, inthistestall prey werepalatable. Half of the mealworms were brown, and half were red with one quarter nonstriped and one quarter striped. The background colors were the same as experienced during avoidance learning. Chicks were allowed one round around the arena, and we recorded the number of each prey type that was attacked. Detection test To make sure that the chicks were able to detect the black stripes on the mealworms an additional test was performed with 11 new chicks. The test took part in a small arena made out of a screened-off part of a housing cage measuring cm. The floor of the cage was covered with white paper, and in the middle of the floor was a cm red painted rectangle situated. The paint was the same as was used on the mealworms in the experiment. First, we accustomed the chicks to the arena by presenting them in pairs with a few live mealworms placed on the red rectangle. This was done twice for all chicks. After that, we added 5 pairs of black stripes distributed evenly over the area of the red rectangle. The pairs of stripes were made to mimic the stripes of the experimental mealworms and were of the same size and at the same distance to each other. The chicks were tested singly and were first presented 2 live mealworms placed on the rectangle, to get them to start foraging. For the following 2 minutes, we registered the number of pecks inside the red area, and we noted especially whether the pecks were directed to the black patterns or not. Figure 2 The reflectance spectra from the brown and red background papers and paints that we used in the experiment. The measurements from the paints are when covered with Stop n Grow. Data analysis For the avoidance learning, the proportion palatable prey attacked in each trial was calculated as (palatable prey attacked)/(all prey attacked). The data were transformed

4 Aronsson and Gamberale-Stille Contrasting patterns in aposematism 1359 (X^[20.947]) to fit the assumptions of parametric tests and were compared using repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with trial number as within factor and background color and prey pattern as between factors. For the generalization test, we calculated the proportion of the presented prey of each type that were attacked as (number of brown attacked)/8, (number of nonstriped attacked)/4, or (number of striped attacked)/4. We used Friedman tests to investigate general differences in attack probability on the 3 prey types within each treatment group, followed by Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for more detailed comparisons. We also used a Mann Whitney U test to compare attacks between the 2 background colors. Additionally, we compared the attack probability of the novel prey pattern in the generalization test with the attack probability of the same pattern during the first trial of avoidance training (i.e., how the birds that were to be trained with that same pattern reacted to it on the first encounter when it was novel to them). These data were compared with a Mann Whitney U test. For the detection test, we used a Sign Test to compare the number of pecks delivered on the stripes to the number of pecks on other areas within the red rectangle to determine the visibility of the black pattern used in the experiment. RESULTS Although both types of aposematic prey in the experiment (red and red with stripes) were initially novel to the birds and had classical warning colors, all birds except one (from group R/ ns) attacked the prey on first encounter. Thus, the birds in this experiment showed no initial aversion toward the aposematic prey. Furthermore, chicks in all groups, irrespective of background color or the presence or absence of stripes on the prey, learned to some degree to avoid the unpalatable prey and to discriminate it from the palatable prey (Figure 3, Table 1). The repeated measures ANOVA shows a significant effect of background color, indicating a difference in the degree of avoidance learning; chicks presented with prey on a brown background, matching the palatable prey and contrasting with the unpalatable prey, learned faster compared with those trained on the red background color that matched the unpalatable prey (Figure 3 and Table 1). There was, however, no general effect of pattern, suggesting that the presence or absence of stripes did not affect the degree to which the birds learned to avoid the unpalatable prey in the experiment. There was also a significant effect of trial, showing that the birds in general learned to avoid attacking the unpalatable prey, and a significant interaction between trial and pattern (Table 1). The latter suggests different shapes of the learning curves depending on the presence/absence of a black pattern. Figure 3 shows that the major difference between the learning curves occurs in the second trial. There, birds trained with red prey without a black pattern (especially on brown background) show a greater avoidance of the aposematic prey than do birds trained with striped prey, and this difference is significant (Post hoc test, Fisher Least Significant Difference, P ¼ 0.017). However, this benefit for the nonstriped prey is already gone in the next trial. The result from the generalization test is shown in Figure 4, and there is a clear difference in all treatment groups in attack probability of the 3 prey types (Freidman tests, degrees of freedom ¼ 2; Br/nS, v 2 ¼ 22.53, P, ; Br/S, v 2 ¼ 22.8, P, ; R/nS, v 2 ¼ 8.93, P ¼ 0.012; R/S, v 2 ¼ 9.03, P ¼ 0.011). In all 4 groups, there is no significant difference between the 2 warningly colored prey (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, Br/nS, Z ¼ 20.54, P ¼ 0.593; Br/S, Z ¼ 21.21, P ¼ 0.225; R/nS, Z ¼ , P ¼ 0.515; R/S, Z ¼ , P ¼ 0.753), suggesting a complete Figure 3 The proportion of all attacked prey that were palatable in each of 5 trials of avoidance learning for the 4 treatment groups (mean 6 standard error). Circles represent brown background color, and triangles represent red background color. Solid lines and symbols represent nonstriped prey, and broken lines and empty symbols represent striped prey. generalization over pattern in this study. There is, however, a difference between the proportion palatable brown prey attacked compared with the warningly colored prey (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, Br/(nS 1 S) pooled, Z ¼ 24.78, P, ; R/ (ns 1 S) pooled, Z ¼ 24.21, P, ), and there is a difference in avoidance of the warningly colored prey between the 2 background colors (Mann Whitney U test, Br vs. R: U ¼ , P, ). This means that the generalization test not only supports the results from the avoidance learning with respect to the higher effect of background contrast in avoidance of aposematic prey but also adds the information that chicks did not differentiate between the prey types with or without the contrasting internal pattern (Figure 4). It could be argued that the avoidance of the novel warningly colored prey in the generalization test may not be due to generalization but to neophobia. To investigate this possibility, we Table 1 Results of a repeated measures ANOVA where prey background (red or brown) and pattern (red prey with or without stripes) were the between factors and trial number (1 5) were the within factors Effects SS df MS F P Background < Pattern Background 3 Pattern Error Trial < Trial 3 background Trial 3 pattern Trial 3 background 3 pattern Error Bold text indicates significance level P, 0.05; df, degrees of freedom; SS, sum of squares; MS, mean square.

5 1360 Behavioral Ecology Figure 4 The proportion of presented prey of each type that was attacked (mean 6 standard error) during the generalization test by chicks in the different treatment groups. White bars represent attacks on brown prey, black bars represent attacks on red prey, and gray bars represent attacks on striped prey. All prey were palatable in the generalization test. compare the behavior of birds in the first trial of avoidance learning to the behavior of birds in the generalization test with respect to the proportions attacked of presented aposematic prey with or without a black pattern on the same respective background. The attack probability of the novel aposematic prey in the first learning trial was much higher than in the generalization test ( as compared with ; mean 6 1 standard error, Mann Whitney U Test, U ¼ , P, ), indicating that the avoidance in the generalization test was due to generalization after avoidance learning. In the detection test, 10 of 11 birds pecked at the black stripes whereas one bird did not peck at all. Only one of the birds pecked (once) outside of the black stripes, but this bird also repeatedly pecked at the stripes. Thus, all the 10 pecking birds pecked predominantly on the black stripes, and this is significant (Sign Test, N ¼ 10, P, 0.001). From this we conclude that the black stripes were visible to the birds. DISCUSSION The chicks in our experiment showed faster avoidance learning when the unpalatable prey had a basic coloration (red) that contrasted with the background color. This is true independently of the presence or absence of internal patterns. This finding is supported by previous studies (Gittleman and Harvey 1980; Roper and Wistow 1986; Roper and Redston 1987; but see Sillén- Tullberg 1985). We found no similar effect of internal pattern contrast because birds in general avoided striped and nonstriped prey to a similar degree on their respective backgrounds. However, there was a significant effect of internal pattern on the shape of the learning curves (Figure 3), but contrary to the suggestion that internal contrasts may improve avoidance learning, the nonstriped prey were avoided more than the striped prey. This difference could imply that learning is faster for prey without internal contrasts but if so the effect is very small in the present experiment and already gone in the following trials. In any case, this experiment provides no evidence to any benefits in predator avoidance learning from having an internally contrasting black pattern. Also in the generalization test, the chicks that were presented prey on a contrasting brown background attacked significantly less of the warningly colored prey compared with the chicks presented prey on the matching red background. This is in accordance with a greater avoidance learning on the brown background. Additionally, the chicks completely generalized their previous experience of either plain red prey or red prey with black stripes to the respective novel color pattern where black stripes were either present or absent. There is a possibility that avoidance of novel color patterns can be due to other factors such as innate avoidance or neophobia (e.g., Marples and Roper 1996; Jetz et al. 2001) rather than generalization from learned avoidance. However, we find it very unlikely that this is the case in the present experiment. First, because the chicks did not show any avoidance of these color patterns when first encountered in the experiment. Second, the detailed comparison of attack probabilities between trial 1 and the generalization test of each prey type on respective background (when the prey were novel to birds in both situations) showed a considerably greater avoidance in birds that had undergone the avoidance learning trials. Thus, because the avoidance of the novel color pattern, plain red or striped, only occurs after avoidance learning of the other respective color pattern and not before, we feel quite safe to ascribe the avoidance to generalization. However, a noxious experience may enhance unlearned biases against specific warning colors (e.g., Rowe and Skelhorn 2005), and we cannot rule out the possibility that this may have affected birds behavior in the generalization test. Nevertheless, we conclude that the birds did not bias their behavior based on the presence or absence of internal contrasting patterns in the present experiment. As mentioned in the introduction, a pattern consisting of contrasting elements may actually reduce conspicuousness and is an important component in disruptive crypsis, especially when some color components match colors in the background (Cott 1940; Edmunds 1974; Schaefer and Stobbe 2006; Stevens et al. 2006). Generally, and in the present experiment, it is difficult to evaluate what pattern characteristics birds find conspicuous in aposematic prey. For instance, the presence of a black pattern may increase conspicuousness due to internal color and intensity contrasts but at the same time reduce prey-to-background contrasts. This may have played a role in the present experiment if the birds found the black color of the striped prey less contrasting than the red color to the brown background and therefore found the striped prey in total less contrasting to the background in that trial. This could possibly explain the lack of positive effects on avoidance learning from internal patterns on the brown background, but it cannot explain the lack of effect of internal contrasts on the red background. On this background, the striped prey were, if anything, more contrasting both with respect to background contrast and to internal contrast and neither here did the pattern positively affect the avoidance learning. In addition, the detection test showed that the stripes are quite visible to the birds because they were pecked at repeatedly in a foraging situation. As mentioned above, the positive antipredatory effects of conspicuousness due to background contrast in aposematic prey has been hypothetically extended to conspicuousness generated by internal pattern contrasts (Guilford 1990). We found no support for this in the present experiment, and our results suggest that this extension of conspicuousness in aposematic prey, to include within-prey pattern contrast, is not so straightforward. When discussing their experiments on initial wariness toward aposematic prey, Roper and Cook (1989) argue that neither striped prey nor bicolored prey (both containing color boundaries) were shown to be more aversive

6 Aronsson and Gamberale-Stille Contrasting patterns in aposematism 1361 than a plain black prey, and many color combinations were even preferred compared with control prey. Neither were the striped prey more aversive than the bicolored prey which, according to Roper and Cook (1989), hypothesis of Guilford (1990) predicts. Several studies with birds as predators have shown that they often use only parts of a warning signal as cues, whereas other components are ignored (Schmidt 1960; Terhune 1977; Evans et al. 1987; Aronsson and Gamberale-Stille 2008), and when learning to avoid prey, their attention is often biased toward the most salient component within a color pattern. Less salient components seem to be attended to only if they solely predict the unprofitability of the prey (Gamberale-Stille and Guilford 2003). Rowe et al. (2004) investigated the importance of pattern similarity between Müllerian mimics and found no effect on learning in great tits when there were 2 different patterns signaling unpalatability compared with one and that an increase in pattern similarity between the mimics did not affect their mortality. This suggests a quite broad generalization over pattern in that study. That the chicks in this present study generalize completely between prey with or without the black contrasting pattern suggests that their main attention was on the color component within the color pattern. Similarly, a study using 4 species of wild birds and color mutants of a normally red and black aposematic heteropteran bug shows that white mutants (otherwise equally defended) with the same black pattern but without the red color are not at all avoided and generalized (Exnerová et al. 2006; see also Svádová et al. 2009). Taken together, these studies suggest that birds often show selective attention to some characteristics of the color pattern and disregard others. However, the fact that there are many cases of mimicry where pattern similarity is very exact indicates that at least some important predators generalize very narrowly over many color pattern components. Especially in situations where Batesian mimicry is very common, it would be advantageous for a predator to learn exact color patterns when they alone predict unpalatability of the prey. There are many hypotheses about the benefits to the contrasting elements in aposematic color patterns, both related to signal efficacy and to other benefits. For instance, one reason to evolve a contrasting internal pattern would be if the predators perceive the presence of a pattern as more symmetrical than the absence. Symmetric signals have been suggested to positively influence prey detection and the association with unpalatability (Forsman and Herrström 2004). For instance, it has been shown that pigeons have easier to detect, to learn, and to reproduce patterns from their memory when they are symmetric compared with asymmetric patterns (Delius and Nowak 1982), and an otherwise strong signal could be weakened by asymmetries in the pattern, the color, or the shape of the signaling pattern elements (Forsman and Merilaita 1999; Forsman and Herrström 2004). Another possibility is that regular patterns may make the prey animal stand out from the chaotic background environment and thereby improve recognition and detection (Kenward et al. 2004). Also, it has been suggested that contrasting patterns within warning colors may facilitate the learning of a specific hue, so that a possible function of patterns (i.e., black) might be to direct the attention of the predator to the warning color itself (i.e., red, Rowe and Guilford 2000). According to Osorio et al. (1999) different parts of a prey s color pattern may play different roles in a predator s foraging behavior; the contrasting pattern attracts the attention, whereas the other colors give specific information and are remembered more accurately. They also suggest that it may be beneficial to have a high contrasting internal pattern due to predators supernormal responses to such patterns (Osorio et al. 1999). In this study, we have only aimed to investigate one specific hypothesis explaining the common occurrence of contrasting black patterns in colorful aposematic displays; the idea that the internal pattern contrasts promotes a conspicuousness that have the same effect as prey color contrast against the background in that it improves avoidance learning in predators (Guilford 1990). From this experiment, we conclude that internal pattern contrasts do not necessarily promote predator avoidance learning in the same way as prey contrast against the background, and the positive signal effects shown for preyto-background contrast cannot simply be expanded to also explain the presence of contrasting internal patterns in aposematic prey. As mentioned above, there are a vast number of hypotheses explaining the role of contrasting patterns in aposematic signal design (see e.g., Kenward et al. 2004; Aronsson and Gamberale-Stille 2008, for short reviews), but there is little experimental support for any of them. Thus, more studies are warranted to investigate if there is a general explanation to why aposematic prey often have evolved these contrasting patterns. In this study, the focus has been on warning coloration and the question of how different components of a complex signal contribute to signal efficacy. However, our results may also be relevant to the general understanding of other biological signaling systems where learning is involved. FUNDING The Swedish Research Council grants (D-numbers & to G.G.-S.). We thank Olof Leimar, Birgitta S. Tullberg, and 3 anonymous referees for their valuable comments on the manuscript and Carolina Bragée for helping out with the experiments. This work was carried out under permit from Stockholms djurförsöksetiska nämnd (D-nr 191/07). REFERENCES Aronsson M, Gamberale-Stille G Domestic chicks primarily attend to colour, not pattern, when learning an aposematic coloration. Anim Behav. 75: Cott HB Adaptive coloration in animals. London: Methuen. Creer DA Correlations between ontogenetic change in color pattern and antipredator behavior in the racer, Coluber constrictor. Ethology. 111: Delius JD, Nowak B Visual symmetry recognition by pigeons. Psychol Res. 44: Edmunds M Defence in animals: a survey of anti-predator defences. Harlow (UK): Longman. Endler JA Interactions between predators and prey. In: Krebs JR, Davies N, editors. Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Oxford: Blackwell. p Evans DL, Castoriades N, Badruddine H The degree of mutual resemblance and its effect on predation in young birds. Ethology. 74: Exnerová A, Svádová K, Stŷs P, Barcalová S, Landová E, Prokopová M, Fuchs R, Socha R Importance of colour in the reaction of passerine predators to aposematic prey: experiments with mutants of Pyrrhocoris apterus (Heteroptera). Biol J Linn Soc. 88: Forsman A, Herrström J Asymmetry in size, shape, and color impairs the protective value of conspicuous color patterns. Behav Ecol. 15: Forsman A, Merilaita S Fearful symmetry: pattern size and asymmetry affects aposematic signal efficacy. Evol Ecol. 13: Gamberale G, Tullberg BS Evidence for a peak-shift in predator generalization among aposematic prey. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 263:

7 1362 Behavioral Ecology Gamberale G, Tullberg BS Aposematism and gregariousness: the combined effect of group size and coloration on signal repellence. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 265: Gamberale-Stille G Decision time and prey gregariousness influence attack probability in naive and experienced predators. Anim Behav. 60: Gamberale-Stille G Benefit by contrast: an experiment with live aposematic prey. Behav Ecol. 12: Gamberale-Stille G, Guilford T Contrast versus colour in aposematic signals. Anim Behav. 65: Gittleman JL, Harvey PH Why are distasteful prey not cryptic? Nature. 286: Gittleman JL, Harvey PH, Greenwood PJ The evolution of conspicuous coloration: some experiments in bad taste. Anim Behav. 28: Guilford T The evolution of aposematism. In: Evans DL, Schmidt JO, editors. Insect defenses: adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators. Albany (NY): State University of New York Press. p Guilford T, Dawkins MS Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Anim Behav. 42:1 14. Hauglund K, Hagen SB, Lampe HM Responses of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus) to multimodal aposematic signals. Behav Ecol. 17: Jetz W, Rowe C, Guilford T Non-warning odors trigger innate color aversions as long as they are novel. Behav Ecol. 12: Kassarov L Are birds the primary selective force leading to evolution of mimicry and aposematism in butterflies? An opposing point of view. Behaviour. 140: Kenward B, Wachtmeister CA, Ghirlanda S, Enquist M Spots and stripes: the evolution of repetition in visual signal form. J Theor Biol. 230: Lindström L, Rowe C, Guilford T Pyrazine odour makes visually conspicuous prey aversive. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 268: Marples NM, Roper TJ Effects of novel colour and smell on the response of naive chicks towards food and water. Anim Behav. 51: Marples NM, Roper TJ Response of domestic chicks to methyl anthranilate odour. Anim Behav. 53: Mason JR Direct and observational learning by redwinged blackbirds (Agelaius poeniceus): the importance of complex visual stimuli. In: Zentall TR, Galef BGJ, editors. Social learning: psychological and biological perspectives. Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum. p Osorio D, Jones CD, Vorobyev M Accurate memory for colour but not pattern contrast in chicks. Curr Biol. 9: Papageorgis C Mimicry in neotropical butterflies. Am Sci. 63: Polat U, Tyler CW What pattern the eye sees best. Vision Res. 39: Riipi M, Alatalo RV, Lindström L, Mappes J Multiple benefits of gregariousness cover detectability costs in aposematic aggregations. Nature. 413: Robson JG, Graham N Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual-field. Vision Res. 21: Roper TJ, Cook SE Responses of chicks to brightly colored insect prey. Behaviour. 110: Roper TJ, Redston S Conspicuousness of distasteful prey affects the strength and durability of one-trial avoidance-learning. Anim Behav. 35: Roper TJ, Wistow R Aposematic coloration and avoidancelearning in chicks. Q J Exp Psychol. 38: Rowe C, Guilford T Aposematism: to be red or dead. Trends Ecol Evol. 15: Rowe C, Lindström L, Lyytinen A The importance of pattern similarity between Müllerian mimics in predator avoidance learning. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 271: Rowe C, Skelhorn J Colour biases are a question of taste. Anim Behav. 69: Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed MP Avoiding attack. The evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Schmidt RS Predator behaviour and the perfection of incipient mimetic resemblances. Behaviour. 16: Searcy WA, Nowicki S The evolution of animal communication: reliability and deception in signaling systems. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Schaefer HM, Stobbe N Disruptive coloration provides camouflage independent of background matching. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 273: Sherratt TN, Beatty CD The evolution of warning signals as reliable indicators of prey defense. Am Nat. 162: Sillén-Tullberg B The significance of coloration per se, independent of background, for predator avoidance of aposematic prey. Anim Behav. 33: Skelhorn J, Rowe C Tasting the difference: do multiple defence chemicals interact in Müllerian mimicry? Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 272: Stevens M, Cuthill IC, Windsor AMM, Walker HJ Disruptive contrast in animal camouflage. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 273: Svádová K,ExnerováA,Štys P, Landová E,ValentaJ,Fučíková A,SochaR Role of different colours of aposematic insects in learning, memory and generalization of naïve bird predators. Anim Behav. 77: Terhune EC Components of a visual stimulus used by scrub jays to discriminate a batesian model. Am Nat. 111: Turner JRG A tale of two butterflies. Nat Hist. 84: Wolf M, Werner YL The striped color pattern and striped nonstriped polymorphism in snakes (Reptilia, Ophidia). Biol Rev. 69:

The effect of aposematism and food calls on the feeding behaviour of domestic chicks André Bommo

The effect of aposematism and food calls on the feeding behaviour of domestic chicks André Bommo The effect of aposematism and food calls on the feeding behaviour of domestic chicks André Bommo Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary synthesis, Department of Biology University of Oslo, Norway Contents

More information

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19, AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19, 575-582 SHIFTS OF 'ATTENTION' IN CHICKS DURING FEEDING BY MARIAN DAWKINS Department of Zoology, University of Oxford Abstract. Feeding in 'runs' of and grains suggested the possibility

More information

Crypsis versus intimidation anti-predation defence in three closely related butterflies

Crypsis versus intimidation anti-predation defence in three closely related butterflies Behav Ecol Sociol DOI 10.1007/s00265-005-0069-9 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Adrian Vallin. Sven Jakobsson. Johan Lind. Christer Wiklund Crypsis versus intimidation anti-predation defence in three closely related

More information

Animal Defense against Predators. Ms. Levasseur Biology

Animal Defense against Predators. Ms. Levasseur Biology Animal Defense against Predators Ms. Levasseur Biology Animal Defense Against Predators Throughout millions of years of evolution, animals have evolved numerous ways of defending themselves against predators.

More information

PIGEON DISCRIMINATION OF PAINTINGS 1

PIGEON DISCRIMINATION OF PAINTINGS 1 PIGEON DISCRIMINATION OF PAINTINGS 1 Pigeon Discrimination of Paintings by Image Sharpness ANONYMOUS Psychology and 20th Century Literature August 8th, 2016 PIGEON DISCRIMINATION OF PAINTINGS 2 Pigeon

More information

This article is downloaded from.

This article is downloaded from. This article is downloaded from http://researchoutput.csu.edu.au It is the paper published as: Author: A. Wichman, L. Rogers and R. Freire Title: Visual lateralisation and development of spatial and social

More information

Mimicry Scientific Background

Mimicry Scientific Background Mimicry Scientific Background The adult Monarch butterfly advertises the fact that it is bitter tasting and toxic to its predators by its bright orange, black, and white wing color pattern. This pattern

More information

Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds

Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds Anim. Behav., 1997, 54, 1019 1025 Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds MARIAN STAMP DAWKINS & ALAN WOODINGTON Department of Zoology, University of Oxford (Received 16 October 1996;

More information

INDIVIDUAL IDENTIFICATION OF GREEN TURTLE (CHELONIA MYDAS) HATCHLINGS

INDIVIDUAL IDENTIFICATION OF GREEN TURTLE (CHELONIA MYDAS) HATCHLINGS INDIVIDUAL IDENTIFICATION OF GREEN TURTLE (CHELONIA MYDAS) HATCHLINGS Ellen Ariel, Loïse Corbrion, Laura Leleu and Jennifer Brand Report No. 15/55 Page i INDIVIDUAL IDENTIFICATION OF GREEN TURTLE (CHELONIA

More information

Management of bold wolves

Management of bold wolves Policy Support Statements of the Large Carnivore Initiative for Europe (LCIE). Policy support statements are intended to provide a short indication of what the LCIE regards as being good management practice

More information

Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens

Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens 1 Introduction: Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens In many species, social interactions among siblings and (or) between siblings and their parents during

More information

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia. PRESENTED BY KEN Yasukawa at the 2007 ABS Annual Meeting Education Workshop Burlington VT ANIMAL BEHAVIOR Humans have always been interested in animals and how they behave because animals are a source

More information

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting Penny Hawkins Research Animals Department, RSPCA, UK Helping animals through welfare science Aim: to provide practical information on refining

More information

ONTOGENY OF DEFENSE AND ADAPTIVE COLORATION IN LARVAE OF THE COMMA BUTTERFLY, POLYGONIA C-ALBUM (NYMPHALIDAE)

ONTOGENY OF DEFENSE AND ADAPTIVE COLORATION IN LARVAE OF THE COMMA BUTTERFLY, POLYGONIA C-ALBUM (NYMPHALIDAE) JOl1rnal of the Lepidopterists' Society.5.5(2), 2001, 69-73 ONTOGENY OF DEFENSE AND ADAPTIVE COLORATION IN LARVAE OF THE COMMA BUTTERFLY, POLYGONIA C-ALBUM (NYMPHALIDAE) SOREN NYLIN\ GAB luella GAMBERALE-STILLE,

More information

Mimicry and Defense. Protective Strategies 3/24/2015. Professor Donald McFarlane. Camouflage ( Cryptic coloration ) Diverse Coloration

Mimicry and Defense. Protective Strategies 3/24/2015. Professor Donald McFarlane. Camouflage ( Cryptic coloration ) Diverse Coloration Professor Donald McFarlane Mimicry and Defense Protective Strategies Camouflage ( Cryptic coloration ) Diverse Coloration Diversion Structures Startle Structures 2 1 Camouflage ( Cryptic coloration ) Minimize

More information

The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae)

The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae) June, 2002 Journal of Vector Ecology 39 The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae) W. Lawrence and L. D. Foil Department of Entomology, Louisiana

More information

CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015

CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015 CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015 The Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply study seeks to understand the sustainability impacts of three laying hen housing systems

More information

LAB. NATURAL SELECTION

LAB. NATURAL SELECTION Period Date LAB. NATURAL SELECTION This game was invented by G. Ledyard Stebbins, a pioneer in the evolution of plants. The purpose of the game is to illustrate the basic principles and some of the general

More information

Answers to Questions about Smarter Balanced 2017 Test Results. March 27, 2018

Answers to Questions about Smarter Balanced 2017 Test Results. March 27, 2018 Answers to Questions about Smarter Balanced Test Results March 27, 2018 Smarter Balanced Assessment Consortium, 2018 Table of Contents Table of Contents...1 Background...2 Jurisdictions included in Studies...2

More information

MITOCW MIT9_20F13_lec17.mp3

MITOCW MIT9_20F13_lec17.mp3 MITOCW MIT9_20F13_lec17.mp3 The following content is provided under a Creative Commons license. Your support will help MIT OpenCourseWare continue to offer high quality educational resources for free.

More information

Population differences in predation on Batesian mimics in allopatry with their model: selection against mimics is strongest when they are common

Population differences in predation on Batesian mimics in allopatry with their model: selection against mimics is strongest when they are common Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2007) 61:505 511 DOI 10.1007/s00265-006-0278-x ORIGINAL ARTICLE Population differences in predation on Batesian mimics in allopatry with their model: selection against mimics is strongest

More information

Sexy smells Featured scientist: Danielle Whittaker from Michigan State University

Sexy smells Featured scientist: Danielle Whittaker from Michigan State University Sexy smells Featured scientist: Danielle Whittaker from Michigan State University Research Background: Animals collect information about each other and the rest of the world using multiple senses, including

More information

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis I. P. JOHNSON and R. M. SIBLY Fourteen individually marked pairs o f Canada Geese were observedfrom January to April on their feeding grounds

More information

Variation in predator species abundance can cause variable selection pressure on warning signaling prey

Variation in predator species abundance can cause variable selection pressure on warning signaling prey Variation in predator species abundance can cause variable selection pressure on warning signaling prey Janne K. Valkonen 1, Ossi Nokelainen 1, Martti Niskanen 1, Janne Kilpimaa 1, Mats Björklund 2 & Johanna

More information

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour Jessica Vroonen Introduction Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour Introduction Lizards intra- and interspecific differences in colour Introduction Lizards

More information

Species must be adapted to their habitat.

Species must be adapted to their habitat. Species must be adapted to their habitat. Species must protect themselves from the heat, cold, move around, feed themselves, communicate and reproduce. These are the types of adaptations which we will

More information

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics OVERVIEW This activity serves as a supplement to the film The Origin of Species: The Beak of the Finch and provides students with the opportunity to develop

More information

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush Sample paper critique #2 The article by Hayes, Nahrung and Wilson 1 investigates the response of three rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush rat), Uromys

More information

Warning Signalling in European Vipers and Their Mimics

Warning Signalling in European Vipers and Their Mimics JYVÄSKYLÄ STUDIES IN BIOLOGICAL AND ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCE 283 Janne K. Valkonen Warning Signalling in European Vipers and Their Mimics Implications for Conservation of the Smooth Snake JYVÄSKYLÄ STUDIES

More information

NATURAL SELECTION SIMULATION

NATURAL SELECTION SIMULATION ANTHR 1-L BioAnthro Lab Name: NATURAL SELECTION SIMULATION INTRODUCTION Natural selection is an important process underlying the theory of evolution as proposed by Charles Darwin and Alfred Russell Wallace.

More information

ANS 490-A: Ewe Lamb stemperament and Effects on Maze Entry, Exit Order and Coping Styles When Exposed to Novel Stimulus

ANS 490-A: Ewe Lamb stemperament and Effects on Maze Entry, Exit Order and Coping Styles When Exposed to Novel Stimulus Animal Industry Report AS 663 ASL R3182 2017 ANS 490-A: Ewe Lamb stemperament and Effects on Maze Entry, Exit Order and Coping Styles When Exposed to Novel Stimulus Emily Strong Iowa State University Samaneh

More information

of ftphelocoma goeruixscens, the

of ftphelocoma goeruixscens, the the effect of aposematic coloration on thefood preferende of ftphelocoma goeruixscens, the floim^ Mary Bizabeth Hi The effect of aposematic coloration on the food preference of Aphelocoma coerulescens,

More information

Mosquitoes in Your Backyard Diversity, life cycles and management of backyard mosquitoes

Mosquitoes in Your Backyard Diversity, life cycles and management of backyard mosquitoes Mosquitoes in Your Backyard Diversity, life cycles and management of backyard mosquitoes Martha B. Reiskind, PhD & Colleen B. Grant, MS North Carolina State University, Department of Applied Ecology, Raleigh,

More information

Conflict-Related Aggression

Conflict-Related Aggression Conflict-Related Aggression and other problems In the past many cases of aggression towards owners and also a variety of other problem behaviours, such as lack of responsiveness to commands, excessive

More information

Higher National Unit Specification. General information for centres. Unit code: F3V4 34

Higher National Unit Specification. General information for centres. Unit code: F3V4 34 Higher National Unit Specification General information for centres Unit title: Dog Training Unit code: F3V4 34 Unit purpose: This Unit provides knowledge and understanding of how dogs learn and how this

More information

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns Demography and Populations Survivorship Demography is the study of fecundity and survival Four critical variables Age of first breeding Number of young fledged each year Juvenile survival Adult survival

More information

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS? Wilson Bull., 0(4), 989, pp. 599605 DO BROWNHEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF REDWINGED BLACKBIRDS? GORDON H. ORTANS, EIVIN RDSKAPT, AND LES D. BELETSKY AssrnAcr.We tested the hypothesis

More information

Monarchs: Metamorphosis, Migration, Mimicry and More

Monarchs: Metamorphosis, Migration, Mimicry and More Monarchs: Metamorphosis, Migration, Mimicry and More Middle School Life Science TEKS Sixth Grade: 6.12E, 6.12F Seventh Grade: 7.10A, 7.10B, 7.10C, 7.11A, 7.11B, 7.11C, 7.12A, 7.13A, 7.13B, 7.14A Eighth

More information

Basic Commands and Training

Basic Commands and Training Greyhounds: Basic Commands and Training Written by Susan McKeon, MAPDT, UK (01157) www.happyhoundstraining.co.uk Registered Charity Numbers 269688 & SC044047 Providing bright futures and loving homes Providing

More information

SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT

SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT The STSM applicant submits this report for approval to the STSM coordinator Action number: CA15134 STSM title: Investigating how feather peckers choose

More information

INDIA S SNAKE-MIMIC CATERPILLARS WORMTONGUE!

INDIA S SNAKE-MIMIC CATERPILLARS WORMTONGUE! 4 SCOOP! INDIA S SNAKE-MIMIC CATERPILLARS WORMTONGUE! THE PERFECT IMITATION OF A VENOMOUS SNAKE S HEAD - COMPLETE WITH STARING EYES AND A FLICKING TONGUE A close-up of a caterpillar of the Common Mormon

More information

Plating the PANAMAs of the Fourth Panama Carmine Narrow-Bar Stamps of the C.Z. Third Series

Plating the PANAMAs of the Fourth Panama Carmine Narrow-Bar Stamps of the C.Z. Third Series Plating the PANAMAs of the Fourth Panama Carmine Narrow-Bar Stamps of the C.Z. Third Series by Geoffrey Brewster The purpose of this work is to facilitate the plating of CZSG Nos. 12.Aa, 12.Ab, 13.A, 14.Aa,

More information

CHOOSING YOUR REPTILE LIGHTING AND HEATING

CHOOSING YOUR REPTILE LIGHTING AND HEATING CHOOSING YOUR REPTILE LIGHTING AND HEATING What lights do I need for my pet Bearded Dragon, Python, Gecko or other reptile, turtle or frog? Is specialised lighting and heating required for indoor reptile

More information

AMOXICILLIN AND CLAVULANIC ACID TABLETS Draft proposal for The International Pharmacopoeia (February 2018)

AMOXICILLIN AND CLAVULANIC ACID TABLETS Draft proposal for The International Pharmacopoeia (February 2018) February 2018 Draft for comment 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 AMOXICILLIN AND CLAVULANIC ACID TABLETS Draft

More information

The Development of Behavior

The Development of Behavior The Development of Behavior 0 people liked this 0 discussions READING ASSIGNMENT Read this assignment. Though you've already read the textbook reading assignment that accompanies this assignment, you may

More information

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring - 2011 Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey October 2011 1 Cover photograph: Egyptian vulture landing in Beypazarı dump site, photographed

More information

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research Terry J. Ord, Emília P. Martins Department of Biology, Indiana University Sidharth Thakur Computer Science Department, Indiana University

More information

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING J. exp. Biol. 180, 247-251 (1993) Printed in Great Britain The Company of Biologists Limited 1993 247 BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING AUD THESEN, JOHAN B. STEEN* and KJELL B. DØVING Division

More information

Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and Sociality

Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and Sociality International Journal of Poultry Science 9 (12): 1087-1091, 2010 ISSN 1682-8356 Asian Network for Scientific Information, 2010 Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and

More information

8/19/2013. Who eats herps? Topic 20: Predators. Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps?

8/19/2013. Who eats herps? Topic 20: Predators. Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Topic 20: Predators Variation in predators across taxa Variation in predators through ontogeny How do herps avoid being eaten? Introduction to the diversity of anti-predator defenses Many animals Depends

More information

Scientists use the scientific method to inquire WHY and HOW?

Scientists use the scientific method to inquire WHY and HOW? Scientists use the scientific method to inquire WHY and HOW? Science provides a method for generating explanations of events and patterns that we observe. Hypotheses ARE these explanations Hypotheses lead

More information

Species introductions can reveal the operation of natural

Species introductions can reveal the operation of natural Evolution of bird eggs in the absence of cuckoo parasitism David C. Lahti* Museum of Zoology and Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 Communicated

More information

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color Madeleine van der Heyden, Kimberly Debriansky, and Randall Clarke

More information

Name period date assigned date due date returned. Variation Lab

Name period date assigned date due date returned. Variation Lab Name period date assigned date due date returned Introduction: The Island Strawling bird lives on a remote island in the South Pacific. It feeds on strawberries and drinks from the freshwater lakes that

More information

Living Planet Report 2018

Living Planet Report 2018 Living Planet Report 2018 Technical Supplement: Living Planet Index Prepared by the Zoological Society of London Contents The Living Planet Index at a glance... 2 What is the Living Planet Index?... 2

More information

Spatial and reversal learning in congeneric lizards with different foraging strategies

Spatial and reversal learning in congeneric lizards with different foraging strategies ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1999, 57, 393 47 Article No. anbe.1998.17, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Spatial and reversal learning in congeneric lizards with different foraging strategies LAINY

More information

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS Wilson Bull., 97(2), 1985, pp. 183-190 BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS BRADLEY M. GOTTFRIED, KATHRYN ANDREWS, AND MICHAELA

More information

Do predator energy demands or previous exposure influence protection by aposematic coloration of prey?

Do predator energy demands or previous exposure influence protection by aposematic coloration of prey? Current Zoology, 2017, 63(3), 259 267 doi: 10.1093/cz/zow057 Advance Access Publication Date: 10 May 2016 Article Article Do predator energy demands or previous exposure influence protection by aposematic

More information

Tuning a nose to forage: Evidence for olfactory learning in a procellariiform seabird chicks

Tuning a nose to forage: Evidence for olfactory learning in a procellariiform seabird chicks St. John Fisher College Fisher Digital Publications Biology Faculty Publications Biology 1-7-2010 Tuning a nose to forage: Evidence for olfactory learning in a procellariiform seabird chicks Gregory B.

More information

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153) i Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN 978-1-927194-58-4, page 153) Activity 9: Intraspecific relationships extra questions

More information

1.5 C: Role of the Environment in Evolution Quiz

1.5 C: Role of the Environment in Evolution Quiz 1. Numbers of reported cases of bedbug infestations have been increasing over the past ten years in the United States. In an attempt to combat the infestations, people began using pesticides to kill the

More information

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc 1. The money in the kingdom of Florin consists of bills with the value written on the front, and pictures of members of the royal family on the back. To test the hypothesis that all of the Florinese $5

More information

Population Dynamics: Predator/Prey Teacher Version

Population Dynamics: Predator/Prey Teacher Version Population Dynamics: Predator/Prey Teacher Version In this lab students will simulate the population dynamics in the lives of bunnies and wolves. They will discover how both predator and prey interact

More information

Animal Behavior. Problem Area: Animal Health and Administering Veterinary Care. Corresponding E-unit(s). Danville, IL: CAERT, Inc.

Animal Behavior. Problem Area: Animal Health and Administering Veterinary Care. Corresponding E-unit(s). Danville, IL: CAERT, Inc. Animal Behavior Unit: Animal Science and the Industry Problem Area: Animal Health and Administering Veterinary Care Student Learning Objectives. Instruction in this lesson should result in students achieving

More information

Reversing Category Exclusivities in Infant Perceptual Categorization: Simulations and Data

Reversing Category Exclusivities in Infant Perceptual Categorization: Simulations and Data Reversing Category Exclusivities in Infant Perceptual Categorization: Simulations and Data Robert M. French, Martial Mermillod (rfrench, mmermillod@ulg.ac.be) Psychology Department, Université de Liège,

More information

A test of an antipredatory function of conspicuous plastron coloration in hatchling turtles

A test of an antipredatory function of conspicuous plastron coloration in hatchling turtles Evol Ecol (2017) 31:463 476 DOI 10.1007/s10682-017-9892-5 ORIGINAL PAPER A test of an antipredatory function of conspicuous plastron coloration in hatchling turtles Beth A. Reinke 1 Ryan Calsbeek 1 Devi

More information

A tail of two scorpions Featured scientists: Ashlee Rowe and Matt Rowe from University of Oklahoma

A tail of two scorpions Featured scientists: Ashlee Rowe and Matt Rowe from University of Oklahoma A tail of two scorpions Featured scientists: Ashlee Rowe and Matt Rowe from University of Oklahoma Animals have evolved many ways to defend themselves against predators. Many species use camouflage to

More information

5 State of the Turtles

5 State of the Turtles CHALLENGE 5 State of the Turtles In the previous Challenges, you altered several turtle properties (e.g., heading, color, etc.). These properties, called turtle variables or states, allow the turtles to

More information

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010. This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 08 June 2015, At: 08:36 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Shooting the poop Featured scientist: Martha Weiss from Georgetown University

Shooting the poop Featured scientist: Martha Weiss from Georgetown University Research Background: Shooting the poop Featured scientist: Martha Weiss from Georgetown University Imagine walking through a forest in the middle of summer. You can hear birds chirping, a slight breeze

More information

Teaching Assessment Lessons

Teaching Assessment Lessons DOG TRAINER PROFESSIONAL Lesson 19 Teaching Assessment Lessons The lessons presented here reflect the skills and concepts that are included in the KPA beginner class curriculum (which is provided to all

More information

Taming Shy and Feral Rabbits with Clicker Training. Andrea Bratt-Frick and Jean Silva

Taming Shy and Feral Rabbits with Clicker Training. Andrea Bratt-Frick and Jean Silva Taming Shy and Feral Rabbits with Clicker Training By The strategy in taming shy or feral rabbits is to associate all good things in life with you. All privileges, like time in the exercise pen, all toys,

More information

Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris

Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris Applied Animal Behaviour Science 84 (2003) 297 305 Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris Deborah L. Wells, Peter G. Hepper Canine Behaviour Centre, School of Psychology, Queen s University

More information

IMPROVEMENT OF SENSORY ODOUR INTENSITY SCALE USING 1-BUTANOL FOR ENVIRONMENTAL ODOUR EVALUATION

IMPROVEMENT OF SENSORY ODOUR INTENSITY SCALE USING 1-BUTANOL FOR ENVIRONMENTAL ODOUR EVALUATION Proceedings of the 14 th International Conference on Environmental Science and Technology Rhodes, Greece, 3-5 September 2015 IMPROVEMENT OF SENSORY ODOUR INTENSITY SCALE USING 1-BUTANOL FOR ENVIRONMENTAL

More information

EVALUATION OF EFFECTS OF A STRAIN, STOCKING DENSITY AND AGE ON BILATERAL SYMMETRY OF BROILER CHICKENS

EVALUATION OF EFFECTS OF A STRAIN, STOCKING DENSITY AND AGE ON BILATERAL SYMMETRY OF BROILER CHICKENS 2017 NPPC ISSN 1337-9984 EVALUATION OF EFFECTS OF A STRAIN, STOCKING DENSITY AND AGE ON BILATERAL SYMMETRY OF BROILER CHICKENS M. A. POPOOLA*, M. O. BOLARINWA, O. O. OJETOLA, O. C. OLADITI, O. P. KOLAWOLE

More information

ADAPTATION IN ANIMALS. 1. Which body feature of a frog MAINLY helps it to capture a flying insect? Ans

ADAPTATION IN ANIMALS. 1. Which body feature of a frog MAINLY helps it to capture a flying insect? Ans Name : Subject : Science Class : V Roll No. : Date : SECTION A Choose the correct alternative ADAPTATION IN ANIMALS 1. Which body feature of a frog MAINLY helps it to capture a flying insect? a. Long,

More information

INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR

INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR Photo courtesy: USDA What is behavior? Aggregate of responses to internal and external stimuli - Dictionary.com The action, reaction, or functioning of a system,

More information

Population Dynamics: Predator/Prey Teacher Version

Population Dynamics: Predator/Prey Teacher Version Population Dynamics: Predator/Prey Teacher Version In this lab students will simulate the population dynamics in the lives of bunnies and wolves. They will discover how both predator and prey interact

More information

Physical and social environment for sheep

Physical and social environment for sheep Physical and social environment for sheep Effects on spacing behaviour, social interactions and activity budgets in housed ewes PhD student: Grete H.M. Jørgensen Supervisors: Knut. E. Bøe, Inger Lise Andersen

More information

Field Development of the Sex Pheromone for the Western Avocado Leafroller, Amorbia cuneana

Field Development of the Sex Pheromone for the Western Avocado Leafroller, Amorbia cuneana California Avocado Society 1981 Yearbook 65: 143-151 Field Development of the Sex Pheromone for the Western Avocado Leafroller, Amorbia cuneana J. B. Bailey, M. P. Hoffman, L. M. McDonough Principal investigator,

More information

Housetraining Drs. Foster & Smith Educational Staff

Housetraining Drs. Foster & Smith Educational Staff Housetraining Drs. Foster & Smith Educational Staff Q. What are the best methods for housetraining a puppy? A. If your dog is going to live inside the home, and in America over 90% of our pets do, you

More information

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia) Luke Campillo and Aaron Claus IBS Animal Behavior Prof. Wisenden 6/25/2009 Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia) Abstract: The Song Sparrow

More information

MARY F. WILLSON RESULTS

MARY F. WILLSON RESULTS SEED SIZE PREFERENCE IN FINCHES S MARY F. WILLSON EED preferences of several finch species have been explored in the labora- tory (Willson, 1971; Willson and Harmeson, in press) using both wild and commercial

More information

Effects of Natural Selection

Effects of Natural Selection Effects of Natural Selection Lesson Plan for Secondary Science Teachers Created by Christine Taylor And Mark Urban University of Connecticut Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Funded by the

More information

STATUS SIGNALING IN DARK-EYED JUNCOS

STATUS SIGNALING IN DARK-EYED JUNCOS STATUS SIGNALING IN DARK-EYED JUNCOS ELLEN D. KETTERSON Department of Biology, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana 47401 USA ABSTR CT.--Rohwer (1975, 1977) has proposed that members of certain variably-plumaged

More information

Be Doggone Smart at Work

Be Doggone Smart at Work Be Doggone Smart at Work Safety training for dog bite prevention on the job No part of this demo may be copied or used for public presentation or training purposes. This is a free introductory demo containing

More information

ECOL /8/2019. Why do birds have colorful plumage? Today s Outline. Evolution of Animal Form & Function. 1. Functions of Colorful Plumage

ECOL /8/2019. Why do birds have colorful plumage? Today s Outline. Evolution of Animal Form & Function. 1. Functions of Colorful Plumage Today s Outline 1. Functions of Colorful Plumage Evolution of Animal Form & Function Dr Alex Badyaev Office hours: T 11 12, by apt BSW 416 Lecture 14 ECOL 3 3 0 Why do birds have colorful plumage? 2. Types

More information

P VASANTA KUMARI and JAMIL AHMAD KHAN Department of Zoology, Aligarh Muslim University, Aligarh

P VASANTA KUMARI and JAMIL AHMAD KHAN Department of Zoology, Aligarh Muslim University, Aligarh Proc. Indian Acad. Sci., Vol. 87 B, No.9, (Animal Sciences-J), September 1978, pp. 285-291, printed in 1ndia. Retrieval of young by lactating Indian gerbil, indica (Hardwicke) Tatera indica P VASANTA KUMARI

More information

Adaptation. Survival of the Fittest

Adaptation. Survival of the Fittest Adaptation Survival of the Fittest It s all about traits Acquired Traits Happen After Birth Scars Pierced Ears Learning a Skill Changing Appearance It s all about traits Inherited Traits Programmed at

More information

BIOL4. General Certificate of Education Advanced Level Examination June Unit 4 Populations and environment. Monday 13 June pm to 3.

BIOL4. General Certificate of Education Advanced Level Examination June Unit 4 Populations and environment. Monday 13 June pm to 3. Centre Number Surname Candidate Number For Examiner s Use Other Names Candidate Signature Examiner s Initials General Certificate of Education Advanced Level Examination June 2011 Question 1 2 Mark Biology

More information

Reports hom the Research Laboratories... of the Department of Psychiatry University of Minnesota

Reports hom the Research Laboratories... of the Department of Psychiatry University of Minnesota -1 Reports hom the Research Laboratories of the Department of Psychiatry University of Minnesota "Bubble-Nest Building and Visual Reinforcement in Siamese Fighting Fish (Betta splendensy' by JOHN GRABOWSK

More information

An experimental test of female choice relative to male structural coloration in eastern bluebirds

An experimental test of female choice relative to male structural coloration in eastern bluebirds Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2007) 61:623 630 DOI 10.1007/s00265-006-0292-z ORIGINAL ARTICLE An experimental test of female choice relative to male structural coloration in eastern bluebirds Mark Liu & Lynn Siefferman

More information

Wandering Garter Snake

Wandering Garter Snake Wandering Garter Snake General Status Sensitive Long and slender, typical of all garter snakes Body is brown, grey or greenish with checkered back Yellow or brown stripe running down its back When harassed

More information

Biology 2108 Laboratory Exercises: Variation in Natural Systems. LABORATORY 2 Evolution: Genetic Variation within Species

Biology 2108 Laboratory Exercises: Variation in Natural Systems. LABORATORY 2 Evolution: Genetic Variation within Species Biology 2108 Laboratory Exercises: Variation in Natural Systems Ed Bostick Don Davis Marcus C. Davis Joe Dirnberger Bill Ensign Ben Golden Lynelle Golden Paula Jackson Ron Matson R.C. Paul Pam Rhyne Gail

More information

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite The Inheritance of Egg Shell Color W. L. BLOW, C. H. BOSTIAN AND E.^W. GLAZENER North Carolina State College, Raleigh, N. C. ECONOMIC studies have shown definite consumer preference based on egg shell

More information

Exterior egg quality as affected by enrichment resources layout in furnished laying-hen cages

Exterior egg quality as affected by enrichment resources layout in furnished laying-hen cages Open Access Asian-Australas J Anim Sci Vol. 30, No. 10:1495-1499 October 2017 https://doi.org/10.5713/ajas.16.0794 pissn 1011-2367 eissn 1976-5517 Exterior egg quality as affected by enrichment resources

More information

Puppy and Dog Training Guide

Puppy and Dog Training Guide Puppy and Dog Training Guide Puppy and Dog Training Puppies are cute bundles of fun curious and looking for trouble. They have no idea what is and isn t allowed, so it s important to be patient with them.

More information

SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT

SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT The STSM applicant submits this report for approval to the STSM coordinator Action number: CA15134 Synergy for preventing damaging behaviour in group

More information

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota Hatchling Orientation During Dispersal from Nests Experimental analyses of an early life stage comparing orientation and dispersal patterns of hatchlings that emerge from nests close to and far from wetlands

More information

Animal Biodiversity. Teacher Resources - High School (Cycle 1) Biology Redpath Museum

Animal Biodiversity. Teacher Resources - High School (Cycle 1) Biology Redpath Museum Animal Biodiversity Teacher Resources - High School (Cycle 1) Biology Redpath Museum Ecology What defines a habitat? 1. Geographic Location The location of a habitat is determined by its latitude and its

More information