A color in the coat of a mammal is variously known as silvering, roan or

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "A color in the coat of a mammal is variously known as silvering, roan or"

Transcription

1 ON THE GENETICS OF SEVERAL TYPES OF SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG1 SEWALL WRIGHT Department of Zoology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois Received October 19, 1946 INTRODUCTION SPRINKLING of white or light colored hairs among ones of a darker A color in the coat of a mammal is variously known as silvering, roan or gray. At least four distinct patterns of this general sort are known in guinea pigs. The most familiar is one in which silvering, on the ground color determined by the rest of the genotype, is present at birth and does not change. A second type of stationary silvering is restricted to the blacks and browns and their modifications since it is due to the reduction of the yellow of the tortoiseshell pattern of intermingled yellow and dark pigmented hairs to white by certain genotypes. Third is a progressive type of silvering or grizzling, in which the color at birth is wholly that due to the rest of the genotype but scattered white hairs appear later and increase in number as the animal ages. Finally there is a retrogressive type which affects only intense browns. Scattered dark tipped pale brown hairs give a dingy appearance at birth which tends to be reduced or even to disappear in later pelages, expecially in females. The second type has long been understood. The others have been in need of further investigation because of paucity of published data or conflicting interpretations. STATIONARY SILVERING Description The common silvering of the guinea pig varies enormously in degree and character. In its weakest development there are merely a few light or white hairs, usually on the mammary region of the belly. In higher grades, the belly may be more or less uniformly silvered or there may be irregularly distributed blotches of white within silvered areas. Extensive silvering of the belly is correlated with the occurrence of patches of silvering on the sides and middle of the back. More rarely slight silvering of the back occurs without silvering of the belly. In the highest grades, the belly becomes almost white, usually sprinkled here and there with dark hairs or blotches, and the back becomes strongly silvered.' The head and feet show little or no silvering even in the highest grade, as in roan horses. Red hairs are more conspicuously diluted in silvered areas than are black ones. It is sometimes difficult to decide whether a tortoiseshell with apparently dilute yellow hairs, intermingled with intense black, but no white, is to be considered a low grade silver or not. Silvered self reds and yellows are also difficult to classify unless the silvering is strong. This investigation was aided by a grant from the Wallace C. and Clara A. Abbott Memorial Fund of the University of Chicago. GENETICS~~: 115 March 1947

2 116 SEWALL WRIGHT Previous Accounts CASTLE (1912) produced a strongly silvered strain from low grade silvers by selection but arrived at no factorial interpretation. WRIGHT (1917) stated (on the basis of a few crosses between a strongly silvered inbred strain, No. 34, and nonsilvers) that FI usually showed slight silvering on the belly but that there was no clear cut segregation in Fz. IBSEN (1932) tentatively attributed what seems to be the same pattern to an incompletely dominant gene, Ro (roan). He gave no data, however. PICTET and FERRERO (1940) have published extensive data on silvering which seems from the description and photographs to be identical with that considered here. They ascribe whitening of the belly (blanchiment du ventre, partiel ou total) to a dominant gene, Ti. Silvering of the back first appeared in their colony on a whitebellied animal. They found it to be separable, however, and describe it as behaving as a simple recessive (d) in crosses with nonsilvers, and capable of increase by selection. The effect of d is referred to in most places as restricted to the back but in one place (p. 83) it is stated that it may reach the ventral surface. The evidence for their two factor interpretation will be discussed later. Experiments The silvering of the following experiments traced, except where otherwise stated, to an inbred strain, No. 13, in which the average extent of colored areas was so small (about 2 percent of the coat, WRIGHT and CHASE 1936) and so nearly restricted to the head that silvering was rarely evident. Silvering of all grades appeared, however, in the descendants of crosses with a tortoiseshell strain. No analysis of the early segregating generations is practicable; partly because the young were not graded with sufficient care in this respect and partly because the large amount of white in many of them made classification highly uncertain. It is, however, important to give an idea of the range of variability among those descended wholly from animals with more than slight silvering and from crosses between these and inbred strain 13. In table I, a distinction is made between matings (A) in which both parents had more than slight silvering and (B) ones in which one or both parents had only slight, if any, silvering, though descended wholly from the former group. Class C consists of the crosses between either of these (strain 1-13) and strain 13. Among the offspring a distinction is made between those with 50 percent or more of color in the coat, including color on the posterior belly, and the rest in which classification is of little value. The former are classified on the basis of descriptive terms used in the records, such as no silver, very slight, medium, strong, and very strong. In most cases, however, medium and strong here mean merely that silvering was ascribed to the belly alone or to both belly and back, respectively. It is probable from later results that more careful examination would have raised most, if not all, of those called nonsilver to the category very slight silver. The most important conclusion from this table is that animals tracing exclusively to conspicuously silvered ancestors may range from ones with at

3 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG 117 least no easily recognized silvering to ones with very strong silvering. A series of experiments to explore the mode of inheritance was begun by mating medium, strong, or very strong silvers from strain 1-13 with nonsilvers TABLE I Distribzltion of grades of silver in strain I-13, from parents (A) bo?h of which were at least of medium grade, or (B) of which one or both were no more than slightly silvered but were derived from the preceding or (C) one parent was derived from (A) or (B), the other from strain (13), the original source of silvering in MORE THAN 50% COLOR 1W COAT LESS THAN 50% COLOR PARENTS NO MAT- INGS %OFYOUNGOFEACHGRADE - NO. NO % NO. SlL- YOUNG SIL- YOUNG. V.STR. VER VER % A I-13x B I-13x C 1-13XI Total from various sources. Grades of silver have been assigned at birth to back and belly separately to test the hypothesis of PICTET and FERRERO that silver in these areas is determined by independent genes. Grade o Grade I Grade 2 Grade 3 Grade 4 Grade 5 no silver trace of silvering (irregular dilution or few scattered white hairs) slight but unmistakable silvering in -a small area silvering over less than half the area strong silvering and blotching with white over more than half the area white predominates Doubtful grades were checked at weaning if possible. In some cases a trace of silvering was apparent at weaning which had not been observed at birth and in other cases an apparent trace at birth could not be detected later. Changes were rarely made in the higher grades. The principal results are given in table 2. The grades of back and belly are here averaged: averages 0.5 and 1.0 are called I, 1.5 and 2.0 are called 2, etc. The results in F1 indicate that silver is more nearly recessive than dominant. In five matings, no trace of silver was detected in 69 percent of the 39 young and only one individual was assigned grade two. Another F1 mating gave high grade silvers as well as nonsilvers and low grades. The nonsilver parent in this case, however, may well have been heterozygous since silvering has been rather widely distributed in the colony., Crossbreds showing no silver or only a trace have been backcrossed generation after generation to animals of 1-13 that were strongly silvered at least on

4 118 SEWALL WRIGHT the belly. If only one major factor is involved, 50 percent of the F1 type should appear in each generation in spite of the continued attempt at dilution. With two major factors only a little more than 25 percent should continue to be of grades o and r, while if multiple minor factors are involved backcrossing should soon produce a strain like 1-13 in spite of selection of one parent for low grade. The first of these possibilities is the one that was realized. There were TABLE 2 Distribution of grades of silvering from matings derivedfrom outcross of silvers of I-13 to miscellaneous nonsilvers..- PARENT (LOWER) PARENT (HIGHER) NO, GRADES OF SILVERING TOTAL % % MAT TNGS 0 I F, SiSi(Misc) o sisi (1-13) I F, Sisi (Misr) o sisi (1-13) 3.5 I 2 I 2 2 I IBC Sisi(FJ 0.1 sisi (1-13) 3-7 IO zbc Sisi(1BC) 0.5 sisi 11-13) 3.9 IO I BC Sisi(2BC) 0.5 sisi (1-13) BC Sisi(3BC) 0.0 sisi (I-13) 3.5 I z I BC Sisi (4BC) 0.5 sisi (1-13) 4. j I I 1 I I TotalBC Sisi 0.3 sisi IO (F# Sisi(F1) 0.0 Sisi(FJ (IBC)~ Sisi(1BC) 0.5 Sisi(1BC) IO z (zbc)2 Sisi(2Br) 0.0 Sisi(2BC) 1.0 I I I I I (3BCp Sisi(3BC) 0.4 Sisi(3BC) Total (BC)Z Sisi 0.4 Sisi 0.5 IO so (BCp sisi (BC) 2.1 sisi (BC) I BC sisi (BC) 3.0 sisi (1-13) Total sisi 2.6 sisi Si- (BCP 0.6 Sisi(BC) IO Sisi(BC)2 0.1 Sisi(BC) x (BC)2 Sisi(BC) 0.6 sisi (BC) ; percent of grades o and I among 109 young from the first backcross and 49.5 percent of these grades among the same number of young from later backcross generations. The hypothesis that silvering depends on an incomplete recessive (to be called si) has been confirmed by other tests. Backcross silvers of the higher grades (2-5) mated with 1-13 or with each other have given no nonsilvers and no more of grade I than can be accounted for from the distribution in 1-13 itself, and are thus indicated to be sisi. Matings between nonsilvered parents or ones with only a trace, whether from FI or the first to third backcross gave largely (80 percent) nonsilver or grade I, but 20 percent of grades z to 4, indicating that these matings were of type SisiXSisi. A few matings which do not fall into the preceding categories gave results easily interpreted as in harmony with the conclusion that there is only one major pair of alleles.

5 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG TABLE 3 Distribution of backcross progeny by sex and (ype of mating with respect to sex. 119 OFFSPRING 9 ( 3 3) MATINGS FEMALES MALES TOTAL 0- I I 2-5 The progeny of the backcross matings are classified in table 3 by sex of parents and of offspring. R(RD) means a mating of recessive (silver) female by a male from recessive mother and dominant father. The other possibilities, R(DR), (RD)R and (DR)R are to be interpreted similarly. Matings of type R(RD) and R(DR) test the possibility of partial sex-linkage on the preferred hypothesis that males are heterogametic while (RD)R and (DR)R are tests on the less probable hypothesis that females are heterogametic. The absence of significant differences in the distribution of grade classes (0-1) and (2-5) among male and female progenies indicate that there is no sex-linkage of any sort. It is evident that the range of silvering in heterozygotes overlaps both homozygotes. To get a more accurate idea of the amount of overlap than can be obtained merely by inspection of table 2, the 459 individuals in this table have been apportioned to genotypes and grades in as nearly exact Mendelian ratios as possible from each type of mating, and with a minimum of overlap. Table 4 shows the results. TABLE 4 Apportionment of individuals of table 2 to genotypes on the assumption of as nearly exact Mendelian ratios as possible and a minimum of overlap. NO. PROGENY PARENTS MAT- sisi Sisi sisi TOTAL INGS sisi x sisi 5 SisiXsisi 30 SisiXSisi sisix sisi 8 sisixsisi 2 9 Total % within genotypes Mean I IO ' I

6 I20 SEWALL WRIGHT There is no clear evidence that SiSi ever showed any silvering although in one mating, parents both called grade I, SiSi or Sisi, produced ten young all of grades o or I. The heterozygotes must be supposed to be distributed in varying proportions between grades o and I and only very rarely grade 2. In the total, 47 percent of Sisi had no detectable silvering and 53 percent had a trace. Genotype sisi is indicated to cover the range from a trace to very strong silvering with a mean at 3.0: medium silvering., Table 5 shows the relation between the grades of silvering of back and belly among the 218 young from the backcrosses. TABLE 5 Joint distribution of grades of silvering of back and belly in the backcross population. ~_ GRADE OF BELLY - - TOTAL IN TOTAL IN UPPER % o I I Grade of 3 I Back Total Back f Belly 1.96f k Correlation.So+.024 o.68f.os4 26 I The strong correlation,.go+.024, between back and belly, demonstrates again that there can not be random assortment of two major genes, acting separately in these two regions. The rather large number (63) with silvering of the belly but not of the back as compared with the small number (2) in which the opposite was true, seems merely to reflect an average difference in threshold. This difference might to some extent account for the dominance of ventral silvering and recessiveness of dorsal silvering, noted by PICTET and FERRERO, but only as different effects of the same gene, not of independent genes. The correlation between back and belly in silvers, homozygous in si but segregating in minor factors, may be estimated roughly by considering only the upper 50 percent of the backcross population (those in which the sum of dorsal and ventral grades is 3 or more, plus seven from the class graded 2 on the belly, o on the back). The correlation in this group is.68 k.054, which indicates that 68 percent of the variability in silvers of genotype sisi, but otherwise heterogeneous, is due to factors (nongenetic or minor genetic), that act alike on back and belly, leaving only 32 percent to factors that act more locally. That there is considerable nongenetic variability is indicated by the frequency of marked asymmetry and other irregularities on both back and belly and is, of course, to be expected of a character, the very nature of which implies different thresholds among hair follicles that all presumably have the

7 ~~ ~ ~ ~ SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG same genetic constitution. The efficacy of selection indicates, however, that minor genetic factors play some role. Varying degrees of silvering have appeared in a stock of reds and yellows. The character is manifested in these by a usually rather slight but apparently rather uniform dilution of the color of the back, associated with a sprinkling of white hairs which is less conspicuous than on a black ground, and with a marked reduction of color on the belly which appears light yellow, blotched yellow and white, or solid white, depending on the grade. This silvering was independent of strain 13 in origin. The grading within this stock has not been sufficiently accurate for presentation but strong silvering has seemed to behave on the whole as recessive. Crosses of strongly silvered reds with silvers of strains 1-13 or 13 have given silvered black-red tortoise shells of the following grades: I21 MATINGS 0 I TOTAL White bellied light redxsilver (1-13, 13) I8 From this result, there seems little doubt that the same recessive gene si is involved. Not all unexpectedly dilute reds and yellows are sisi, however. Two unexpectedly pale creams (ee ckc7 FF and ee ckca FF), from an experiment in which the amounts of yellow pigment in diverse genotypes were being determined, were mated with silver from These pale creams did not have bellies appreciably lighter than their backs. The results (included in F1 in table 2) indicate that these animals were SiSi. MATINGS 0 I TOTAL Pale creamxsilver (1-13) Discussion of previous interpretations The results obtained in these experiments agree with the earlier statements by the author (1917), IBSEN (1932) *and PICTET and FERRERO (1940) that silver may have at least some degree of dominance, manifested by silvering of the belly in the crossbreds. The gene, called si here, may be the same as that listed by IBSEN with the symbol Ro. It is obvious, however, that the gene is quite unusable as a dominant in the experiments described here, not only because about half of the heterozygotes show no silvering whatever, but those that are called silver have such a slight trace that their classification is often doubtful. The gene can be used as a recessive on the other hand with about the same degree of reliability as in the case of the spotting factor, s, by drawing an arbitrary line according to the expected ratio (cf. WRIGHT 1941). The relation of si to PICTET and FERRERO S V and d, if any, is a puzzling matter. It seems unlikely that two apparently identical series of variations should have nothing in common genetically yet it is certain that the present results can not be explained by separate genes for silvering of back and belly and on the other hand PICTET and FERRERO S published results appear to

8 I22 SEWALL WRIGHT substantiate their interpretation and certainly do not lend themselves readily to interpretation in terms of %,si. There are, however, certain difficulties in the interpretation. The original dorsal silver (which also had ventral white and was considered to be VV dd) was mated with a ventral white which had produced normals and was therefore assigned the formula VvDD. The progeny, 16 in number, were all ventral whites, presumably VvDd and VVDd. Two matings among these gave the following results. In the authors terminology VbZ means ventral white alone, Vbld is ventral white associated with dorsal silvering and arg is dorsal silvering alone. MATING V bl Vbld NORMAL erg TOTAL - - VbL(Fi)XVbl(FJ 17 IO VoDd X VvDd (9/16)V-D- (3/16) V-dd (3/16)uu D- (1/16)uvdd The most striking thing here is the absence of normals (vvd-) although the ratio as a whole is far from good. It should be said, however, that these matings gave much the poorest fit of any reported in the paper. They contribute about one-third of the total x2 referred to later. Six later matings are reported as between ventral whites without dorsal silvering, but with dorsally silvered young (necessarily VvDdX VvDd). Vbl Vbld NORMAL a7g Total - - Vb1 (VvDd) X Vbl (VvDd) I53 Here again there are no normals, vvd-, although this class should constitute 3/16 of the young from this type of mating. The numbers in the other classes are however in excellent agreement with expectation. The authors advance the hypothesis that the normal genotype is lethal when produced by VvDd XVvDd. This is supported by the smaller size of litter in this case (2.25) as compared with from 89 litters from vvddxvvdd (which produces the same kinds of normals). The difference almost exactly accounts for the missing normal young. According to this hypothesis, the union of a vd sperm with a VD egg, or of a VD sperm with either a vd or a VD egg gives lethal results but only if both parents are VvDd. Compare for example the results of the following matings (from which parents of the anomalous lethals were at least in part derived) which show that VvDd individuals of either sex produce the four expected types of germ cells in the expected I : I : I : I ratio and that two at least of the three alleged types of lethal unions give viable young when only one parent is VvDd. MALE FEMALE V bl Vbld NORMAL a7g TOTAL Vbl( VvDd) Xarg(vvdd) 17 I8 I8 I4 67 arg(vvdd)x Vbl(VvDd) Total The burden of proof on such an hypothesis seems so great that it would seem preferable to interpret the matings between Vbl parents cited above, as

9 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG 123 ones in which both were homozygous in a gene for silvering (sisi) which might manifest its action in any of the three forms VU, Vbld or arg according to minor factors, genetic or otherwise. Here, however, we must take cognizance of the closeness with which the observed results are matched by the calculated ones. The data are collected in tables 16 and 17 in PICTET and FERRERO S paper. A series of 48 matings, beginning with ones between extracted dorsal silvers (vvdd) and normals (DUD-) are reported in table 16 as giving only these two classes, in close agreement with expectation for every individual mating. Similarly close agreement is exhibited by the 47 matings (table 17) in which one or both parents had gene V. An analysis of the segregating progenies from these matings gives the following results : DEGREES OF FREEDOM X2 PROBABILITY ~ Table ,996 Table ,999,8 Total ,999,999>99 According to the descriptions, both whitening of the belly and dorsal silvering varied from mere traces to strong; and dorsal silvering (in the category arg) might invade the belly. It would seem that there must have been intergrading of all four categories and many doubtful cases. One can only conclude from the extraordinary goodness of fit that doubtful cases were resolved in favor of the expected ratios. It is possible to account for ~ICTET and FERRERO S results in terms of Si and si but this means little since normals may be either SiSi or Sisi, VbZ and arg either Sisi or sisi and even VbZd may occasionally be Sisi as well as sisi. A record of degree of silvering, rather than mere occurrence is necessary for an analysis. The possibility that the heterozygotes overlap both homozygotes can hardly be tested adequately without determining the resistance to dilution of the FI type by repeated backcrossing to pure recessives, supplemented by breeding tests of supposed recessive segregants. It may be concluded that the postulated independent genes for ventral (V) and dorsal (4 silvering cannot be accepted as demonstrated and that it is at least possible that the same incomplete recessive (si) may underlie all stationary silvering of the type which may be manifested on all ground colors. SYNTHETIC STATIONARY SILVERING In genotypes SSePeP or SSePe there is a sprinkling of yellow hairs on a ground of one of the darker colors in the tortoiseshell pattern (IBSEN rg~g), with more yellow on the average in SSePe than in SSePeP (CHASE 1939). To some extent in SsePeP or Ssepe and to a greater extent in ssepep and ssepe, yellow tends to occur in large areas resembling in form and location the white areas which also appear in these tricolors (WRIGHT(1917)~ ILJIN (1928), CHASE (1939)). There is, however, considerable sprinkling of yellow in the black areas and of black in the yellow areas. It has long been known that

10 124 SEWALL WRIGHT replacement of gene C by lower alleles tends to reduce the intensity of yellow disproportionately and in certain cases (crcr, crca, ckcyj, ckcafj, cdc ff, cdcaff) the reduction is to white (WRIGHT 1915, 1927). There may be considerable resemblance to the preceding type of stationary silvering as manifested in silvered blacks and browns, but not of course in silvered reds and yellows. PROGRESSIVE SILVERING Lambert s results (OR GRIZZLING) A type of silvering of the guinea pig that first appears after the first pelage and which progresses with age, like gray hair in man, and gray (as opposed to roan) in horses, seems to have been described only by LAMBERT (1935). It has been observed in my colony only in a few animals, kindly presented by DR. LAMBERT, and in descendants of these. The character consists of the occurrence of totally white hairs, scattered principally over the posterior region of the back but in extreme caser over all the back and sides and to a lesser extent on the head and belly. At birth the animals may be of any color, depending on the residual genotype. According to LAMBERT, 40 days is the earliest at which white hairs appear (in the posterior dorsal region). LAMBERT S principal results (substituting grizzled for his term silver) were as follows: MATING GRADE OF GRIZZLING NONE TRACE LOW MEDIUM HIGH TOTAL Grizzled Xgrizzled I 6 IO GrizzledXnongrizzled (outcross) I2 2 I4 Grizzled X heterozygote HeterozygoteX heterozygote I7 7 5 I 30 These results point clearly toward an incohpletely recessive major factor (LAMBERT S interpretation) although reports of breeding tests of the supposed segregants are needed to clinch the demonstration. Thus in a cross between inbred normal and polydactyl strains of guinea pigs an apparent one factor difference, from evidence more extensive than that above, was completely disproved on testing the supposed segregants (WRIGHT 1934). Accepting provisionally the one factor interpretation, it may be noted that the amount of overlap of the heterozygotes with both homozygotes is remarkably similar to that inferred earlier in this psper for stationary silvering. In both cases the situation is also very similar to that found for spotting (s) (WRIGHT and CHASE 1936). LAMBERT gives no symbol. T shall use gr (grizzling) to avoid confusion with stationary silvering (si) and to suggest its progressive character. Experiments My own data are not very extensive but confirm LAMBERT S interpretation as far as they go. The pure stock derived from his stock produced 54

11 ~ SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG 125 young which were saved beyond weaning. All of these became grizzled, some as early as two months. A few were ungrizzled at four months but became grizzled, usually of a high grade, later. Reciprocal crosses to ungrizzled animals of my colony gave 21 young which were saved to six months or more. Only one of these developed a trace of grizzling (first noted at nine months). These include a mating to a silver of stock At birth one of the young showed slight silvering of the belly, three showed traces on the belly, five showed no silvering and 13 others were born before careful grading began, but no silvering was recorded and presumably no more than a trace was present if any. Four of the young, including one with a trace of ventral silvering, were kept to ten or more months. None developed any dorsal grizzling. Thus there appears to be no relation between stationary silvering and progressive grizzling. A backcross to the pure grizzled stock gave I 2 nongrizzled (at six months or more) to eight grizzled (two slight, six stronger). Two of the nongrizzled were backcrossed again to pure grizzled and gave five nongrizzled (at six months or more) to two slightly grizzled. These results tend to indicate a normal gene that behaves as a unit in withstanding successive attempts at dilution by backcrossing to the pure recessive strain. Analysis of these progenies by sex of parents and offspring gives no indications of sex-linkage. Using R(DR) for recessive (grizzled) female by male from dominant female and recessive male and (DR)R for the reciprocal backcross, the results were as follows. TABLE 6 Backcross progeny classi$ed by character, sex and nature of the mating. MATING FEMALE MALE TOTAL - - -_ NON- NON- NON- GRIZZLED GRIZZLED GRIZZLED GRIZZLED GRIZZLED GRIZZLED R(DW (DWR I IO 4 Total I7 IO Matings between heterozygotes gave eight nongrizzled (at six months or more) to three grizzled. Matings between heterozygotes and pure nongrizzled gave 24 young all nongrizzled. Extracted grizzled mated inter se have given 14 that were grizzled at six months or later and three not grizzled when last examined (six months in two cases, nine months in the other). As three of those ultimately recorded as grizzled showed no grizzling at six, nine and ten months respectively, the nongrizzled might have become grizzled if they had lived long enough. It is obvious that the grizzling extracted from the outcrosses was not as strong as that from LAMBERT S selected stock. The increase in grizzling with age is shown in table 7 for the pure stock, for the segregants which ultimately become grizzled and for the descendants of extracted grizzled. Grade o is nongrizzled, I means a trace, and z slight, 3

12 126 SEWALL WRIGHT medium, 4 strong and 5 very strong grizzling. The nongrizzled are included in the averages. In conclusion, there seems little doubt that there is one major pair of alleles (Gr, gr) although more extensive tests of segregants would be desirable. The degree of effect is undoubtedly subject to minor factors. With the usual modi- TABLE 7 Increase in grizzling with age. The number of animals examined, the number nongrizzled and the average grade are shown by t-wo month periods to 17 months, for ages 18 to 23 months and for 24 tnonths and over. ~ AGE IN MONTHS IO number IO LAMBERT nongrizzled 4 I O Stock av. o number Segregants nongrizzled 3 2 I O av. o o _~ Extracted Strain number nongrizzled I2 3 2 I O 0 0 av fiers, grizzling is completely or almost completely recessive and even in homozygotes may not appear until the animal is nearly a year old. A residual heredity can be built up by selection that enables grizzling to appear at two months or even less and to become very strong in time and which enables a trace of the character to appear in heterozygotes. RETROGRESSIVE SILVERING OR DINGINESS OF BROWN IBSEN (1932) included in his list of variations of the guinea pig a white tipping for which he suggested an incompletely dominant factor (W). W causes the distal half of all chocolate (b) hairs, on the back especially, to become white, provided the animal carries C, the factor for full color (red is present). It has no effect in cr (absence of red) animals. The effect on black hair (B) is very slight; homozygotes have a few white tipped hairs on the head, while heterozygotes do not show any white tipping. Red hair (CCee) is entirely unaffected by W. There is also a sex-limited effect in that nearly all females gradually lose their white tipping while males tend to retain it. No data were given. A variation with some of the above characteristics has been present in browns (chocolates) of my colony and has been referred to briefly in a previous paper (WRIGHT 1942). EATON (1943) has described a similar condition as occurring in browns of the stock of the U. S. BUREAU OF ANIMAL INDUSTRY

13 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG 127 (from which my stock split off in 1926) and as following an irregular mode of inheritance. There are a number of respects in which the condition in my stock differs from IBSEN S account while agreeing more nearly with EATON S description. Not all brown hairs are affected, even on the head, except in the more extreme cases. There is instead a sprinkling of light colored hairs among ones of full intensity. Moreover brown seems never to be reduced to white in any part of the hair in my stock, even in the highest grades, nor is the lightest portion of the hairs at the tip. Some two or three mm. at the tip of the hair is of full or nearly full intensity. Below this, some hairs merely show a band in which brown is reduced to a color comparable to that of a very pale brown (E bbcdcdppff) followed by less dilute brown to the base. Other hairs are uniformly pale brown except for the tip. In the most extreme grades all hairs are pale brown with dark tips. According to EATON dilution is more extreme in brown agoutis than in browns and this dilution occurs only below the yellow subterminal band of the agouti pattern. The term white tipping obviously does not apply to the condition in these stocks. The term dinginess gives the general impression given by these browns in contrast with intense browns and will be used here for this type of variation. It was noted in the previous paper that this condition could not be transferred to browns with ckck (ck is a higher allele of albinism than c referred to by IBSEN) or with pp, extreme diluter of brown and sepia but could be transferred to browns withjf (i.e. E-bbCCPPjfi. Replacement of F by f has no appreciable effect on browns or sepias in the presence of P although it removes these colors in combination with pp. The paradoxical effect of dinginess in reducing brown only when it would otherwise be most intense made further study of its effects of interest. Experiments Strong dinginess of brown first appeared in some segregants from a cross made by DR. E. C. COLIN between the same black-red-white tricolor strain No. 13 that was the source of stationary silvering and a stock (C) which carried brown. DR. COLIN began experiments with these in He developed a strongly dingy strain and mated one of these with a pink eyed dilute brown tricolor (ssepepaabbckckffpp) from a strain ( 3) that was the most inbred strain of browns in the colony at the time. FI consisted of intense browns with no dinginess. Absence of dinginess persisted through three successive back crosses to the dingy stock in more than 50 percent of the young (total 61 non-dingy, 19 slightly dingy and 30 strongly dingy). Extracted dingies, mated inter se gave four nondingy, 18 slightly dingy and 94 strongly dingy. These results indicate that nondingy differs from dingy primarily ih one major dominant gene. From later results, however, it appears probable now that this dominant gene was the well known recessive p, introduced in the original mating. A stock (S) of browns from DR. COLIN S experiments was carried on for a number of years before an attempt at semiquantitative analysis was made by

14 ~. 128 SEWALL WRIGHT means of grades. Grade o is complete absence of dinginess. In grade I, there is a trace of dinginess, restricted to scattered hairs on the cheeks and perhaps the nape. In grade 2, there is unmistakable dinginess of scattered hairs, restricted to head and nape. In grade 3, scattered dingy hairs are found on the back but substantial nondingy areas are also present. In grade 4, all parts of the back (as well as the head) are sprinkled with dingy hairs and there may be some dinginess on the belly. Grade 5 was adopted later for cases in which practically all hairs are affected. Many animals of this stock, especially in the earlier generations, had only small areas of color on a white ground due to ss, derived as in the case of the silver stock, from strain 13. Grading of these presented less uncertainty than in the case of silver since dinginess can be graded from observation on the head, the commonest location of colored spots in extreme piebalds in contrast with silver, which cannot be graded satisfactorily in the absence of color on the posterior belly. There is no indication that the dinginess of colored spots is at all affected by genes S, s. Nearly all animals have been graded at birth. A second grade has usually been given when possible at about 30 days, when low grade dinginess is sometimes more conspicuous than at birth. Following are the averages of live born and still born intense browns at birth and of those that reached weaning, from matings that trace wholly to animals of grades 3 and 4 in the stock S referred to above and similar averages from the progeny of animals of grades 3 to j that segregated from outcrosses (SP, SK, SA) to other stocks referred to later. In both cases, matings that produced dilutes (ckck etc.) are excluded. All were probably bbc-ppff, but undoubtedly some individuals were heterozygous for albino alleles (Cck, Cc" etc.). (Table 8.) Descendants of high grade dingies TABLE 8 Mean grade5 of dinginess at birth among live born and still born young, and at weaning, from the high grade stock S and from matings between high grades derivingfrom outcrosses. TIME OF GRADING STOCK s EXPERIMENTS SP, SK, SA ~- NO. MEAN SE NO. MEAN SE -~~ Birth (live born) k.09 Birth (still born) k & days oof k.09 The grades of still born young are significantly lower than those of the live born young, possibly because of difficulties in grading in some cases (short hair, wetness, etc.). It has seemed best to exclude them from the data. The correlation between grade at birth and that at weaning was.7 j 5.03 in series S and.8j k.02 in the later group. The difference between the grades was not significant in the former but is in the latter. The grades at weaning are undoubtedly more reliable than those at birth

15 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG 129 but it has not seemed desirable for the purposes of this paper to exclude the grades of live born young when no later grade was given. The grade in all later tables is thus the last grade up to weaning of the live born young. There was segregation of E and ep in these experiments. The average grades of young without red spotting (largely E) and with red spotting (epep) are as follows in the two groups. STOCK s EXPERIMENTS SP, SK, SA - ~ _ - NO. MEAN SE NO. MEAN SE Brown Brown-red IO I f f f.I; It is obvious that there are no consistent differences and no distinction will be made later. The full distribution of grades in these two groups is given below by sex. TABLE 9 Distribution of grades by sex in stock S and among offspring of high grades deri-jed from outcrosses. PERCENTAGE - - -_ 0 I NO. MEAN SE f.07 Stock k.08 S Total oof.06 SP,SK,SA f.11 (parents 3-j) 0 o f.11 Total f.08 The males are slightly higher in grade in both stocks but the difference is of somewhat doubtful significance. If both stocks are combined the difference (.191 f.092) is 2.1 times its standard error. The wide variability of stock S suggests great genetic heterogeneity. The parents in the $3 matings in which both were graded (308 young) ranged from grade I to 4. The correlation between father and offspring was +.280, between mother and offspring +.086, giving an average of There was a low correlation between the parents, +.212, due to a considerable number of brother-sister matings rather than to intentional assortative mating. On the simplest hypothesis (no dominance, no interaction effects, a constant tendency to inbreeding) the correlation between parent and offspring is ( 1/2)h2( 1+m) =.183 where h2 is degree of determination by heredity and m is the correlation between the genotypes of parents. This indicates less than 37 percent determination by heredity. The correlation between parents (often littermates) may include a nongenetic component. The nongenetic variance includes that

16 130 SEWALL WRIGHT due to inconsistencies of grading, but the scale is such a coarse one that this can hardly be the major component. Mated animals have been graded at the time of birth of each litter. Table IO shows the averages for males and females of each initial grade. The males are distinctly less dingy at four or five months than at first but rise slightly in TABLE IO Gvades of dinginess at successive ages of males and females oj each initial grade. _- -~ AGE IN MONTHS _~ - - ~~- ~ IO no. av. no. av. no. 3 av. no. av. no. av. I I IO I IO IO I II 6 3 IO no av no j IO av no Q av no av no av the next pelage and thereafter remain about the same or, in the case of high grades, decline slightly. The females lose most of their dinginess in the first six or eight months but thereafter change little on the average. The grand averages of all grades after six months bring out the difference between the sexes in loss of dinginess. A number of characters of the guinea pig, most notably the occurrence of polydactyly and the amount of color in the spotting pattern have been shown to be greatly affected by the age of the mother (less polydactyly and more

17 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG 131 white among the progeny of mature than of immature females) (WRIGHT 1926). It is of some interest to see whether dinginess belongs in this group, especially as in this case the females lose their dinginess as they become mature which, on a Lamarckian view might be supposed to have a corresponding effect on the offspring. The average grades according to age of mother in the high grade derivatives of stock S and of experiments SP, SK and SA are shown in table 11. TABLE TI Relation of age of mother to average grade of dinginess of ojspring in Stock S and from matings between high grades in experiments SP, SK and SA. - AGE OF MOTHER (MONTHS) STOCK s -_ EXPERIMEKTS SP, SK, SA NO. MEAN SE NO. MEAN SE 3- IO f f.I3 6-82?.go+.II k.IS ,.IO , f f.22 There are no consistent differences among the averages. In stock S, the distributions yield x2 = 9,4, n = 9, Probability, In the other group x2= 20.3, n= 12, Probability, Interactions with known color factors Dinginess is not expressed in the white areas of the piebald pattern, due to the incomplete recessive s, but, as noted earlier, seems to be as strongly expressed in the brown spots of piebalds as in the corresponding areas of self browns. Similarly dinginess is not expressed in self yellows (ee) or in the yellow hairs of tortoiseshells (epep, e*e) but seems to be equally expressible in brown hairs of tortoiseshells and of self browns (E-). Dinginess occurs in the brown of brown agoutis (A-) and according to EATON is more strongly developed than in nonagouti browns (aa). In my experiments, there have not been a sufficient number of litters with segregation of brown agoutis and bruwns for an adequate comparison. The brown factor b is necessary, at least for more than a trace of dinginess. According to IBSEN, traces of white tipping may be present in blacks (B). I have not observed anything in black littermates of dingy browns that could be said with confidence to be the same condition but such litters have not been produced in adequate numbers. There is also, as noted, some doubt as to whether IBSEN S white tipping is the same as the dinginess of brown in my colony. The remaining three major series of alleles are concerned with the intensity of black, brown and yellow. Their interaction with each other in nondingy browns are shown in table 12 in which the amount of brown pigment ex-

18 ~ 132 SEWALL WRIGHT tractible in genotype (S E aabb) C-P- is taken as IOO (WRIGHT 1942). The albino series (C, ck, cd, cr, ca) is represented by the three alleles that have been studied most intensively in connection with dinginess. Replacement of ck by cd or cr gives somewhat smaller imounts of brown. C and P appear to be completely dominant and replacement of F byf in the presence of P appears to have no effect in nondingy browns. F is not completely dominant over f in pale browns, genotype bbc-ffpp having only about 24 percent as much brown as bbc-p- instead of 30 percent as in bbc-ffpp. The very pale yellow or cream of bbc,ffpp seems to contain no brown. TABLE 12 The appearance and amount of brown pigment relative to that with C-P- in combinations of C, ck, c"; F, f and P, p. (F, ffp- FF PP If PP C- Brown (100) Pale Brown (30) Pale Cream (0) CkCk Brown (96) Pale Brown (25) White (0) CkP Brown (83) Pale Brown (15) White (0) caca White (0) White (0) White (0) The effects of these genes on black (B) are somewhat parallel to those on brown. Replacement of either C or F by lower alleles reduces the intensity of yellow. Thus ckck can easily be distinguished from C, and ff from F in tortoiseshells though not in self browns. Replacement of B by b or of P by p has no effect on intensity of yellow. The observation that the occasional brown-yellow tortoiseshells(epepbbckckp- FF) and pink eyed pale browns (bbc-ppff), that segregated out in early generations of stock S, never showed the dinginess which was usually present in their brown-red littermates (epepc-p-ff), led to experiments (SK, SI') intended to test these apparent interaction effects more thoroughly. Another experiment (SF) was made to test whether dinginess could be introduced into brown-yellow tortoiseshells that owed their dilution of yellow toff instead of to ckck. As the interpretation of the results in the last case turned out to involve fewer complications these data will be considered first. Effect of F, f High grade dingies of stock S were mated reciprocally with brown-yellow tortoiseshells of a strain (F) in which at this time all animals were homozygous CC'PP. Most of the young (table 13) showed low grades of dinginess (average I.S+O.I). There were no significant differences by direction of cross or sex of young (averages for 16(DR) 9 's 1.6, for 25(DR) 3 's 1.5. for 12 (RD 0 's 1.2 and for 3(RD) 3's 1.3). Backcrosses to stock F gave 41 young of which 24 had red spots (Ff), 16 had dilute yellow spots (ff) and one had no yellow spots and therefore might either be Ff or ff. Only seven of the 24 known to be Ff had any dinginess (average for Ff0.4Lo.1) and none of the 16 known to be ffwere dingy,

19 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG I33 The significant reduction of the average for Ff (1.5k.10 to ) on backcrossing to stock F shows that the latter must have introduced an independent factor or factors that inhibit dinginess. Results in later generations show that there is, in fact, no difficulty in obtaining Ff of grade 4. Brown-reds from Ffxff in which both parents were half to three-quarter blood of stock S average 2.57rtr.13. This is significantly lower than FF of stock S but is higher than the average for Brown-reds from FfXFf and about the same TABLE 13 The distribution of grades of dinginess in the offspring of matings involving only segregation of F, f among known factors. The ancestry (stock S, or F, F1, $ blood stock S, etc.) is indicated in parentheses and is followed by the average grade. DINGIER LESS DINGY MAT- GENO- PARENT PABENT INGS TYPE 0 I NO. AV. SE FF (S) 3.7 ff (F) Ff ,.IO Ff (RI) 2.2 jf (F) IFF k.13 I I ? (ff 16 FF (SI 4.0 Ff(FJ F- I I *.37 Ff (FI,(2) S) 2.3 F- (ca (2) S) F- I 7 I k.29 FF(ca;S) 3.4 fj(ca(a)s) Ff k.18 FF (S) 3.6 fj (cu(f)s) Ff IS Ff k.13 Ff ((i)-(q)s) 2.8 jf((i)-($)s) ? ff k.13 F k.12 Ff ((B)-(f)S) 2.6 Ff((+)-(Q)S ? (ff IO I k I8 ff I k.c8 ff ((a)-(2)s) 1.6 ff ((i)-(+)s) _. amount of ancestry of stock S although about one-third of these should be FF. The data give no reliable evidence for any difference between FF and Ff where the rest of the genotype is the same. The results in the later generations show that it is also possible to introduce dinginess into ff. But while a few have been given grade 4, it is clear that the average is systematically lower than for Ff. In the case of matings of type FfXff the young known (by their dilute yellow spotting) to be ff are lower than those known (by their red spotting) to be Ff by In the case of matings FfXFf, the brown-yellows (fj) are lower than the brown-reds (Ff or FF) by 1.07 f.23. Those without yellow spotting and thus unclassifiable are intermediate in both cases. The average for young fromffxff (extracted) is very low (1.43 rt.08) in view of the smount of S ancestry and thus confirms the effect of ff indicated by the preceding results. The data from the backcrosses of F1 to stocks F and S are not adequate for a decision on whether stock F differs from S by one incompletely recessive gene (other thanf) or by multiple factors although the fact that the upper i:

20 I34 SEWALL WRIGHT 50 percent of Ff in the backcross to stock F is below F1 in average, while the lower 50 percent in the backcross to stock S is above F1, suggests more than one factor. The small numbers are, however, compatible with the one factor interpretation. It is noteworthy that the antagonistic effect of ff on dinginess is the first clear effect off on dark pigmentation in the presence of P. It is also noteworthy that this effect, which is in the direction of intensification of brown, is in the opposite direction to the elimination of brown by ff in the combination ffpp. Eject of P, p Dingy browns of stock S were mated with pale browns (C-ppFF and ckckppff) from various sources as the first step in an attempt to introduce dinginess into this color. The young in F1 (intense brown) consisted of 40 nondingy and only three with a trace of dinginess. In outcrosses of stock S to browns (nondingy or nearly so) from similar sources but known to be Pp, half the young showed some dinginess and two were of grade 4. Fa from the first cross also included high grade dingies as well as ones of low grade and nondingies. The pink-eyed pale brown-red segregants (C-pp) lacked all trace of dinginess as did a few brown-yellow (ckcckp-) and pale brown-yellow (ckckpp) segregants. Table 14 gives these results and those in later generations, except that the few animals shown to be ckckp or ckckpp by their yellow spotting are omitted. None of these showed any dinginess. The results in F1 and Fz show that the pale brown parent must have introduced a gene or genes that tend to suppress dinginess, that there is segregation of high grade dinginess in Fz and that dinginess is at least difficult to introduce into pale brown. The complete absence of dinginess in the pp segregants of F2 continued into later generations even when the amount of ancestry of stock S was increased to three-fourths or more. The attempt to obtain pale browns from high grade dingy parents, was, however, a failure. The brown young from matings that produced pale browns at all were moreover predominantly nondingy or of low grade. With one exception, matings in which either parent was of high grade (3, 4) produced no pale brown young. Matings in which either parent was pp produce only low grade dingies (and pale browns) except for one individual called grade 3. Nearly all matings in these experiments between animals of grades o or I, or between such animals and pale brown produced some pale browns. All of the results point to the conclusion that p strongly reduces dinginess even in heterozygotes. Only three animals of grade 3 and none of grade 4 have yet been found which were Pp. One of these is shown in the table as coming from a class of matings of type PPXPP. The second came from the same class but was born dead and so does not appear in the table. The third came from a mating of type PpXPp and was proved by a breeding test to be Pp. All of these were about three-fourths blood of stock S. Linkage of p with a suppressor of dinginess is an alternative interpretation. The fact that p from all sources seems to have this effect and that nothing

21 ~ SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG suggesting crossingover has occurred in the course of the experiments, indicates that the effect is inherent in p rather than linked. This conclusion was unexpected for two reasons. First there has been no indication from other experiments that P ever falls short of complete dominance over p even when other factors tend to reduce black or brown pigmentation to such a point that slight differences between PP and Pp should be easily TABLE 14 Distribution of grades of dinginess among the offspring of matings involving p but not f. FI was largely from CCPPXckckpp. Some h er matings produced yellow spotted young (ckckp-, ckckpp). These young are omitted. Ancestry and ar:rage grade of parents are indiceted. PP? and Pp? refer to animals which may have been either PP or Pp by ancestr.y and descendants but are jueythably assigned a genotype by grade. These were ale ($) blood or more of stock S. I35 PARENT NO. GRADE OF OFFSPRING PARENT (BIGHER MAT- pp (LOWER GRADE) GRADE) lngs NO. P- pp (SI 34 PP (Misc IO o pp (3) 3. I Pp (Misc.) o PP VI) 0.2 PP (Fd 0.2 IO II PP cca,s, 2.9 PP (( o PP ((3)s) 1.0 PP ((W) PP ((4)s) 1.4 PP ((3)s) PP (S, (3)s) 3.1 PP ((m,(2)s) Pp (ca (3)s) 2.3 Pp ((W, 2)s) Pp (ca (2)s) 1.3 Pp (ca (t)s) IO I 89 PP (S, ( f ) S 3.2 Pp ((Q)S,(f)S) o PPI 3.5 PP? I EP? 3.6 PP? I 3 9 IO PP? 2.0 PPI PP? 3.8 PP o PP? 3.6 Pp? o PP? 2.0 Pp? P- 0.5 PP P- 1.0 Pp? 0.0 I 0 2 I I 4 detected if present. Secondly, Pp gives a more intense brown than either p p or PP. In extreme dinginess of grade 5, PP may be even paler, though more irregular, than pp. Eject of C series The few brown-yellow tortoiseshells (ckck) that segregated out in experiment S completely lacked dinginess and often appeared to be more intense in the brown parts of their coats than their dingy brown-red littermates. This called to mind IBSEN S observation that white tipping could not be introduced into browns of genotype crcr. As ck is the closest of the albino alleles to C in its effects on color, an experiment (SK) was started with the purpose of introducing dinginess into ckck browns if possible. Table 15 gives the data froin this experiment excluding all matings in which either parent was definitely

22 136 SEWALL WRIGHT known from its ancestry or descendants to be Pp. Unfortunately this excludes all of FI and most of the early matings as the effect of Pp on dinginess was not anticipated. Some of the matjngs included may involve Pp and this may account for some of the young of low grade. The results resemble those with PI p in that no clear case of dinginess has been observed in the recessive segregants] here brown-yellow tortoiseshells (ckck). In two or three cases there was some suggestion of dinginess on the cheeks but not enough to be sure that this was not a manifestation of the tor- TABLE ij Distribution oj grades oj dinginess among offsprkg ojmatings involving C, ck but not F, j. Matings (including all of FJ in which either parent was known to he either Pp or p p are excluded. Matings oj type CckXCck are divided into a group in wh;ch both parents were of high grade (3, 4) and one in which one parent was oj grade L and the oiher z or 3. In all cases both parents are 50% or more oj stock S. The average grades of higher and lower parents in the matings in each group are shown. ~ PARENT (HIGHER GRADE) PARENT (LOWER GRADE) NO. GRADE OF OFFSPRlNG MAT- GENO. --_- INGS TYPE 0 I NO. AV. cc 3.9 CkCk 0 Cck 2.6 CkCk 0 CCk 2.2 CCk 2.0 Cck 3.8 Cck I I I ' I I toiseshell pattern. The results differ from those with P, p in that high grade parents often turned out to be heterozygous (Cck) as shown by the production of brown-yellow young. In such cases, moreover, the brown-red young were predominantly of high grade. Again matings between a ckck parent and either CC or Cck produced many high grade dingies. These results suggest that Cck does not differ from CC in relation to dinginess. The average grades for the brown-red young are, however, rather low for the amount of ancestry of strain S (one-half to three-fourths). Moreover the average grade of brown-reds from matings of type CckXCck which should include 33 percent homozygotes is higher (2.90) than the average from matings which can not produce homozygotes (2.20). This comparison does not, however, have the validity of a comparison between classes of young from the same matings. It has not been possible to obtain a decisive answer to this question by more detailed analysis of these data. In another experiment (SA) an attempt was made to produce albinos (c"c") capable of transmitting dinginess. Table 16 gives a condensed account of the results. Again no matings are included in which either parent was definitely

23 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG '37 known to be other than PP from ancestry or descendants. Unfortunately the available brown eared albinos (EEaabbcacaP-FF) all had p in their recent ancestry and some of them may well have been Pp and again this could be responsible for some of the low grade young. The average lor f (stock S by the foundation albinos) was low (1.36) and included none of grade 4. In later generations, however, with more ancestry of stock S, young were obtained from matings of type CCXcaca and CcaXcaca TABLE 16 Distribution of grades of dinginess among offspring of matings involving C, but not F, f. Parents known to be Pp or pp are excluded. In all except the first row both parents are 50% or more of stock S. The average grade of higher and lower parent in the matings of each group are shown. PARENT PARENT NO. GRADE OF c OFFSPRING (HIGHER (LOWER MAT- GRADE) GRADE) INGS e- c- CC (S) 3.4 caca (EBA) 5 o cc 3.4 c"c" CcQ 2.6 c*c" 8 44 I CC' 2.1 CCQ IO CC(S) 4.0 Cc" NO. AV. that were of grade 4. Thus Cca, like Cck, can not have as great a suppressive effect on dinginess as does Pp. The rather low averages throughout in comparison with stock S suggest, however, that Cca may have some effect but the average is no higher in the progenies that include CC than in those that do not. No systematic attempt has been made to introduce dinginess into browns with other combinations of albino alleles. The following have, however, segregated out of matings in progenies that contained red spotted dingies (C-): 12 ckcr, 24 ckca, 14 cdca, 6 ctcr, 3 crca. The young in the first three of these classes were identifiable by cream spotting (and by ancestry), the last two by white spotting or silvering of the synthetic type (and by ancestry). No trace of dinginess was observed in the brown of any of these. It seems safe to conclude that gene C is necessary for the expression of dinginess. Joint effects of P, p and the C series Genes ck and ca and to a less extend cd and cr were present in experiment SP and it is possible that some of the suppression of dinginess attributed to Pp may have been due to joint effects of Pp and heterozygosis in the C-series. An enumeration (table 17) of known double heterozygotes yields the following in which cz here refers to cases in which it was merely known that a lower albino allele was present. It is, noteworthy that 82 percent of the known double heterozygotes were completely lacking in dinginess and none of the rest showed more than a trace. Thirty-six of these were from the first cross between stock S and unrelated ani-

24 138 SEWALL WRIGHT mals and might owe their low grade to such a factor or factors as was indicated to be present in stock F. The remaining 21, however, had S ancestry, usually 50 percent or more, on both sides. TABLE 17 I)istribution of grades among individirals known to be Pp and also heterozygotes of C wifh alower allele. F,(S X M ISC.) GRADE BOTH PARENTS WITH s ANCESTRY GRADE 0 I 0 I Total Eject of age on different genotypes The average for all grades after six months of age was found for each mated animal. Table 18 shows the relation between initial grade and the average for each genotype and sex after six months. Genotype C-P-FF includes all animals known to be carrying C and P and not known from ancestry or descendants to carry p or any lower allele of C (though heterozygosis may be suspected because of grade in some cases). The initial grades of these mated animals agree fairly well with those based on the progeny in the preceding discussion. The grades after six months show the reduction previously referred to, but otherwise tend to parallel the grades at weaning. It is, however, noteworthy that CckP-FF and CcaP-FF in both males and females show a much greater loss of dinginess than does CCPPFj although the initial grades differ but little. These adult grades give more reliable evidence for effects of Cck and Cca than obtained from the initial grades. The very low grades of CCPPfj, C-PpFF confirm the previous conclusions and the exceptionally low grade of Cc PpFF in males supports the view that Cc and Pp act cumulatively. The effects of unrecognized Pp (in P-) and of residual factors can be largely eliminated by confining attention to animals that were of grades 3, 4 or 5 initially. This is done in table 19. The group C-PPFF is subdivided into those from stock S, experiment SP, and experiments SK or SA. The initial grades mean nothing here because of the selection, which is more drastic in CCPPFf and Cc PPFF than for C-PPFF. The grades after six months bring out clearly the tendency of Cc PPFF to lose dinginess more than does C-PPFF from the same experiments. It appears indeed that C-PPFF from SK and SA has been selected to an even higher level of dinginess than in

25 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG 139 TABLE 18 Average grade after 6 months of age o j animals of indicated genotypes and each initio1 grade. The class 3 C-P-FF initial grade 4 includes 2 of initial grade 5 which averaged 3.70 after 6 months. - - INITIAL GRADE TOTAL GENOTYPE 0 I INI- AFTER - - TIAL 6 NO. AV. NO. AV. NO. AV. NO. AV. NO. AV. NO. GRADE MONTHS d C-P-FF I d CCPPFf I C-PPff d CckP-FF I d CPP-FF C-PpFF dcckppff CPPpFF f f I f IO f I f I f.19 0 C-P-FF CCPPFf C-I?Pff CckP-FF I CPP-FF C-PPFF Cc'PpFF CPPpFF I f f , f o.4of.09 I If orfr.04 stock S. The difference between C- and Cc"in males in experiments SK, SA alone is Using all males of genotype C-PPFF the difference is and in femsles , highly significant in spite of the great reductionin grade in all females. Genotype Ff of initial grade 3 or 4 does not, on the other hand, differ significantly from FF after six months, either in males or females. TABLE 19 Average grade after 6 months of age of animals of the indicated genotypes and of grade 3 or more initially. The group C-PPFF is divided into 3 classes according to source. GENOTYPE NO. INITIAL GRADE AFTER 6 MONTHS d C-PPFF(S) d C-PPFF(SP) d C-PPFF(SK, SA) d CCPPFf(SF) d CczPPFF(SK, SA) e? C-PPFF(S) 0 C-PPFF(SP) 0 C-PPFF(SK, SA) 0 CCPPFf(SF) 0 CczPPFF(SK, SA) I1 PI k f.51 I

26 140 SEWALL WRIGHT The only evidence for an effect of Ffis in the unselected initial grades and here the undoubted presence of unfavorable residual heredity in many Fj and the likelihood that Pp is present in some of those called C-P-FF make comparisons unreliable. Devqlopmental Factors To some extent at least dinginess tends to develop most in the regions of the body (head and back as opposed to belly) and of individual hairs (subterminal region) in which brown would otherwise be most intense. Apparently there is a certain optimum in the complex of factors of all sorts that control the intensity of brown. Either excess or defect in this complex results in less than maximum intensity. The actual amount of brown pigment in dingy hair has not yet, however, been investigated. SUMMARY Four types of silvering of the coat of the guinea pig are discussed. There is a stationary type in which white or light colored hairs are mingled with those of the color determined by the rest of the genotype. It is usually restricted to the belly in lower grades but in higher grades appears also on the back. The headhd legs are least affected. Silvering depends in the main on a single incompletely recessive autosomal gene (si) but varies continuously over a wide range because of minor factors, genetic and non-genetic. There is a syntheticatype in which the tortoiseshell pattern of mingled dark and yellow hairs is reduced to a pattern, sometimes simulating the preceding, by action of genotypes which reduce yellow disproportionately. The genetics of this type is well known. There is a progressive type of silvering or grizzling which appears only after the first pelage. White hairs appear first on the posterior back but become sprinkled later over the entire back, head, and belly, among hairs of the color determined by the rest of the genotype. This depends on an incompletely recessive autosomal gene (grj, in confirmation of LAMBERT. There is a retrogressive type of silvering or dinginess which is manifested only in otherwise intense browns (bb C P). In the lowest grade there are scattered hairs on cheeks and nape in which there is subterminal banding with pale brown. In higher grades, such hairs are sprinkled over the whole head and back, and in the highest practically all hairs are affected. Ginginess is reduced with age, especially in females. At least four series of alleles are involved. One or more autosomal factors, again incompletely recessive, are necessary for any dinginess. If these are favorable, all known series of alleles, that affect intensity of brown in any combination, tend to modify dinginess, but in the opposite direction from that which would be expected from their ordinary effects. CCPPF- is necessary for the highest grade of dinginess. Cc ( =Cck, Ccd, Ccr or Cca) reduces dinginess, at least in mature animals. No dinginess appears in combinations lacking C(ckck, etc.). Pp reduces dinginess more than does Cc and acts cumulatively with the latter. There is no dinginess in pale browns (pp). It is uncertain whether Ff affects dinginess, but ff reduces it considera-

27 SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG 141 bly. To some extent at least dinginess tends to appear most in the region of the body and of the hairs in which brown would otherwise be most intense. The factors that affect dinginess give good illustrations of the principle that gene replacements that increase development of a character in certain combinations may reduce development in others. LITERATURE CITED CASTLE, W. E., 1912 The inconstancy of unit characters. Amer. Nat. 46: CHASE, H. B., 1939 Studies on the tricolor pattern of the guinea pig. Genetics 24: EATON, 0. N., 1943 Silvering in a strain of guinea pigs. J. Hered. 34: IBSEN, H. L., 1919 Tricolor inheritance. IV. The triple allelomorphic series in guinea pigs. Genetics 4: Modifying factors in guinea pigs. Proc. 6th Int. Congress. Genetics 2: ILJIN, N. A., 1928 The distribution and inheritance of white spots in guinea pigs (Russian). Trans. Lab. Exp. Biol. Zoopark, Moscow 4: LAMBERT, W. V., 1935 Silver guinea pigs. J. Hered. 26: PICTET, A. and A. FERRERO, 1940 Recherches sur l h6rcdit6 de la dilution et du blanchiment du pelage dans le genie Cavia. Genetica 22: WRIGHT, S., 1915 The albino series of allelomorphs in guinea pigs. Amer. Nat. 49: Color inheritance in mammals. V. The guinea pig. J. Hered. 8: Effects of age of parents on characteristics of the guinea pig. Amer. Nat. 60: The effects in combination of the major color-factors of the guinea pig. Genetics 12: The results of crosses between inbred strains of guinea pigs, differing in number of digits. Genetics 19: Tests for linkage in the guinea pig. Genetics 26: The physiological genetics of coat color of the guinea pig. Biol. Symp. 6: WRIGHT, S., and H. B. CHASE, 1936 On the genetics of the spotted pattern of the guinea pig. Genetics 21:

SEWALL WRIGHT. Received July 7, 1958

SEWALL WRIGHT. Received July 7, 1958 ON THE GENETICS OF SILVERING IN THE GUINEA PIG WITH ESPECIAL REFERENCE TO INTERACTION AND LINKAGEv2 THE SEWALL WRIGHT Department of Genetics, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin Received July 7,

More information

LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE'

LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE' LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE' HORACE W. FELDMAN Bussey Inslitutim, Harvard Univwsity, Forest Hills, Boston, Massachusetts Received June 4, 1924 Present concepts of some phenomena of

More information

I the BUSSEY INSTITUTION of HARVARD UNIVERSITY, it was found that

I the BUSSEY INSTITUTION of HARVARD UNIVERSITY, it was found that THE RELATION OF ALBINISM TO BODY SIZE IN MICE W. E. CASTLE Division of Genetics, University of Calijornia, Berkeley, California Received January 24, 1938 N PREVIOUS studies made in cooperation with former

More information

I yellow, a great assortment of shades of red and yellow being known. The

I yellow, a great assortment of shades of red and yellow being known. The INHERITANCE OF BULB COLOR IN THE ONION A. E. CLARKE, H. A. JONES, AND T. M. LITTLE' U. S. Department oj Agrudture, Bdtsville, Maryland Received February 17, 1944 N THE onion the color of the bulb ranges

More information

Breeding Icelandic Sheepdog article for ISIC 2012 Wilma Roem

Breeding Icelandic Sheepdog article for ISIC 2012 Wilma Roem Breeding Icelandic Sheepdog article for ISIC 2012 Wilma Roem Icelandic Sheepdog breeders should have two high priority objectives: The survival of the breed and the health of the breed. In this article

More information

THE FACTORS OF THE ALBINO SERIES OF GUINEA-PIGS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON BLACK AND YELLOW PIGMENTATION

THE FACTORS OF THE ALBINO SERIES OF GUINEA-PIGS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON BLACK AND YELLOW PIGMENTATION THE FACTORS OF THE ALBINO SERIES OF GUINEA-PIGS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON BLACK AND YELLOW PIGMENTATION SEWALL WRIGHT Bureau of Animal Industry, U. S. Defartment of Agriculture, Washington, D. C. Received January

More information

TRICOLOR IIVHERITANCE TORTOISESHELL CATS'

TRICOLOR IIVHERITANCE TORTOISESHELL CATS' TRICOLOR IIVHERITANCE. 111. TORTOISESHELL CATS' HEMAS L. IBSEN Uiiiversity of Wisconsin, Madison, Wkconsin [Received June 6, 19161 DONCASTER has published several papers dealing with the inheritance of

More information

A FAMILY OF SPOTTED Q. I. SIMPSON AND W. E. CASTLE1

A FAMILY OF SPOTTED Q. I. SIMPSON AND W. E. CASTLE1 A FAMILY OF SPOTTED NEGROES Q. I. SIMPSON AND W. E. CASTLE1 IT is the purpose of this note to put on record an interesting variation in human skin color which made its appearance as a mutation or sport

More information

WILLIAM H. EYSTER University of Maine, Orono, Mahe. Received December 15, 1924 TABLE OF CONTENTS

WILLIAM H. EYSTER University of Maine, Orono, Mahe. Received December 15, 1924 TABLE OF CONTENTS MOSAIC PERICARP IN MAIZE WILLIAM H. EYSTER University of Maine, Orono, Mahe Received December 15, 1924 TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE INTRODUCTION.... 179 Previous study of mosaic pericarp.... 179 Material a&

More information

T heritance in mice begun some years ago at the Bussey Institution of

T heritance in mice begun some years ago at the Bussey Institution of INFLUENCE OF CERTAIN COLOR MUTATIONS ON BODY SIZE IN MICE, RATS, AND RABBITS W. E. CASTLE University of California, Berkeley, California' Received October 4, I 940 INTRODUCTION HIS paper is a further contribution

More information

W. E. CASTLE. Received, July 21, 1950

W. E. CASTLE. Received, July 21, 1950 VARIATION IN THE HOODED PATTERN OF RATS, AND A NEW ALLELE OF HOODED* W. E. CASTLE Division of Genetics, University of California, Berkeley, California Received, July 21, 1950 HE earliest recorded studies

More information

Mendelian Genetics SI

Mendelian Genetics SI Name Mendelian Genetics SI Date 1. In sheep, eye color is controlled by a single gene with two alleles. When a homozygous brown-eyed sheep is crossed with a homozygous green-eyed sheep, blue-eyed offspring

More information

Biology 164 Laboratory

Biology 164 Laboratory Biology 164 Laboratory CATLAB: Computer Model for Inheritance of Coat and Tail Characteristics in Domestic Cats (Based on simulation developed by Judith Kinnear, University of Sydney, NSW, Australia) Introduction

More information

GENETICS PRACTICE 1: BASIC MENDELIAN GENETICS

GENETICS PRACTICE 1: BASIC MENDELIAN GENETICS Period Date GENETICS PRACTICE 1: BASIC MENDELIAN GENETICS Solve these genetics problems. Be sure to complete the Punnett square to show how you derived your solution. 1. In humans the allele for albinism

More information

Studying Gene Frequencies in a Population of Domestic Cats

Studying Gene Frequencies in a Population of Domestic Cats Studying Gene Frequencies in a Population of Domestic Cats Linda K. Ellis Department of Biology Monmouth University Edison Hall, 400 Cedar Avenue, W. Long Branch, NJ 07764 USA lellis@monmouth.edu Description:

More information

Mendelian Genetics 1

Mendelian Genetics 1 Mendelian Genetics 1 Genetic Terminology Trait - any characteristic that can be passed from parent to offspring Heredity - passing of traits from parent to offspring Genetics - study of heredity 2 Gregor

More information

THE ASSOCIATION OF SIZE DIFFERENCES WITH SEED-COAT PATTERN AND PIGMENTA- TION IN PHASEOLUS VULGARIS

THE ASSOCIATION OF SIZE DIFFERENCES WITH SEED-COAT PATTERN AND PIGMENTA- TION IN PHASEOLUS VULGARIS THE ASSOCIATION OF SIZE DIFFERENCES WITH SEED-COAT PATTERN AND PIGMENTA- TION IN PHASEOLUS VULGARIS KARL SAX Maine A gricuttural Experiment Station, Orono, Maine Received May 26, 1923 TARTW nm rmwrvwrc

More information

THE MASKING OF SEPIA BY WHITE, TWO RECESSIVE

THE MASKING OF SEPIA BY WHITE, TWO RECESSIVE Eye-Colors in Drosophila 261 THE MASKING OF SEPIA BY WHITE, TWO RECESSIVE EYE-COLORS IN DROSOPHILA Floyd T. Romberger, Jr., Purdue University During* the course of a discussion on the dilution effects

More information

Biology 201 (Genetics) Exam #1 120 points 22 September 2006

Biology 201 (Genetics) Exam #1 120 points 22 September 2006 Name KEY Section Biology 201 (Genetics) Exam #1 120 points 22 September 2006 Read the question carefully before answering. Think before you write. You will have up to 50 minutes to take this exam. After

More information

Mendelian Genetics Problem Set

Mendelian Genetics Problem Set Mendelian Genetics Problem Set Name: Biology 105 Principles of Biology Fall 2003 These problem sets are due at the beginning of your lab class the week of 11/10/03 Before beginning the assigned problem

More information

INHERITANCE OF BODY WEIGHT IN DOMESTIC FOWL. Single Comb White Leghorn breeds of fowl and in their hybrids.

INHERITANCE OF BODY WEIGHT IN DOMESTIC FOWL. Single Comb White Leghorn breeds of fowl and in their hybrids. 440 GENETICS: N. F. WATERS PROC. N. A. S. and genetical behavior of this form is not incompatible with the segmental interchange theory of circle formation in Oenothera. Summary.-It is impossible for the

More information

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens? Problem 1 A rooster with gray feathers is mated with a hen of the same phenotype. Among their offspring, 15 chicks are gray, 6 are black, and 8 are white. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance

More information

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens? Problem 1 A rooster with gray feathers is mated with a hen of the same phenotype. Among their offspring, 15 chicks are gray, 6 are black, and 8 are white. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance

More information

17 Inherited change Exam-style questions. AQA Biology

17 Inherited change Exam-style questions. AQA Biology 1 Two genes in a mouse interact to control three possible coat colours: grey, black and brown. The two genes are located on separate chromosomes. Each gene has two alleles: A is dominant to a and B is

More information

Genetics. Labrador Retrievers as a Model System to Study Inheritance of Hair Color. Contents of this Section

Genetics. Labrador Retrievers as a Model System to Study Inheritance of Hair Color. Contents of this Section Genetics Labrador Retrievers as a Model System to Study Inheritance of Hair Color Contents of this Section Unlike humans, who usually have only one child at a time, and rarely manage more than a dozen

More information

1. For each genotype, indicate whether it is heterozygous (HE) or homozygous (HO) Ii Jj kk Ll

1. For each genotype, indicate whether it is heterozygous (HE) or homozygous (HO) Ii Jj kk Ll Simple Genetics Practice Problems 1. For each genotype, indicate whether it is heterozygous (HE) or homozygous (HO) AA Bb Cc Dd Ee ff GG HH Ii Jj kk Ll Mm nn OO Pp 2. For each of the genotypes below, determine

More information

Basic color/pattern genetics. Heather R Roberts 3 November 2007

Basic color/pattern genetics. Heather R Roberts 3 November 2007 Basic color/pattern genetics Heather R Roberts 3 November 2007 Today s Outline 1) Review of Mendelian Genetics 2) Review of Extensions 3) Mutation 4) Coloration and pattern Alleles Homozygous having the

More information

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens? Problem 1 A rooster with gray feathers is mated with a hen of the same phenotype. Among their offspring, 15 chicks are gray, 6 are black, and 8 are white. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance

More information

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite The Inheritance of Egg Shell Color W. L. BLOW, C. H. BOSTIAN AND E.^W. GLAZENER North Carolina State College, Raleigh, N. C. ECONOMIC studies have shown definite consumer preference based on egg shell

More information

Question 3 (30 points)

Question 3 (30 points) Question 3 (30 points) You hope to use your hard-won 7.014 knowledge to make some extra cash over the summer, so you adopt two Chinchillas to start a Chinchilla breeding business. Your Chinchillas are

More information

1 - Black 2 Gold (Light) 3 - Gold. 4 - Gold (Rich Red) 5 - Black and Tan (Light gold) 6 - Black and Tan

1 - Black 2 Gold (Light) 3 - Gold. 4 - Gold (Rich Red) 5 - Black and Tan (Light gold) 6 - Black and Tan 1 - Black 2 Gold (Light) 3 - Gold 4 - Gold (Rich Red) 5 - Black and Tan (Light gold) 6 - Black and Tan 7 - Black and Tan (Rich Red) 8 - Blue/Grey 9 - Blue/Grey and Tan 10 - Chocolate/Brown 11 - Chocolate/Brown

More information

Sections 2.1. and 2.2. (Single gene inheritance, The chromosomal basis of single-gene inheritance patterns)

Sections 2.1. and 2.2. (Single gene inheritance, The chromosomal basis of single-gene inheritance patterns) Chapter 2 Single-Gene Inheritance MULTIPLE-CHOICE QUESTIONS Sections 2.1. and 2.2. (Single gene inheritance, The chromosomal basis of single-gene inheritance patterns) 1. If a plant of genotype A/a is

More information

Coat Colour. Lakeland Terrier

Coat Colour. Lakeland Terrier A Discussion of Coat Colour Genetics in the Lakeland Terrier Chapter 6 Blue By Ron Punter Blue: G gene is produced by an extra gene modifying the black pigment, it is not an allele on the same series as

More information

The purpose of this lab was to examine inheritance patters in cats through a

The purpose of this lab was to examine inheritance patters in cats through a Abstract The purpose of this lab was to examine inheritance patters in cats through a computer program called Catlab. Two specific questions were asked. What is the inheritance mechanism for a black verses

More information

No tail (Manx) is a dominant trait and its allele is represented by M The presence of a tail is recessive and its allele is represented by m

No tail (Manx) is a dominant trait and its allele is represented by M The presence of a tail is recessive and its allele is represented by m Lab #4: Extensions to Mendelian Genetics Exercise #1 In this exercise you will be working with the Manx phenotype. This phenotype involves the presence or absence of a tail. The Manx phenotype is controlled

More information

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics Objectives In today s lab you will explore some of the simpler principles of Mendelian genetics using a computer program called CATLAB. By the end of this

More information

P to be a heritable characteristic, although in some instances the mode of

P to be a heritable characteristic, although in some instances the mode of INHERITANCE OF POLYDACTYLISM IN THE FOWL' D. C. WARREN Kansas Agricultural Experiment Station, Manhattan, Kansas Received October 21, 1943 OLYDACTYLISM, where studied in vertebrates, has usually been shown

More information

Text Reference, Campbell v.8, chapter 14 MENDELIAN GENETICS SINGLE TRAIT CROSS LAW OF SEGREGATION:

Text Reference, Campbell v.8, chapter 14 MENDELIAN GENETICS SINGLE TRAIT CROSS LAW OF SEGREGATION: AP BIOLOGY Text Reference, Campbell v.8, chapter 14 ACTIVITY 1.20 NAME DATE HOUR MENDELIAN GENETICS SINGLE TRAIT CROSS LAW OF SEGREGATION: TWO TRAIT CROSS LAW OF INDEPENDENT ASSORTMENT LAWS OF PROBABILITY

More information

What is Genetics? Genetics is the scientific study of heredity

What is Genetics? Genetics is the scientific study of heredity What is Genetics? Genetics is the scientific study of heredity What is a Trait? A trait is a specific characteristic that varies from one individual to another. Examples: Brown hair, blue eyes, tall, curly

More information

The Inheritance of Coat Colour in the Cardigan Welsh Corgi by Ken Linacre

The Inheritance of Coat Colour in the Cardigan Welsh Corgi by Ken Linacre The Inheritance of Coat Colour in the Cardigan Welsh Corgi by Ken Linacre In a working dog, colour is undoubtedly of secondary importance to construction, but the wide range of colours found in the Cardigan

More information

The genetics and development of fused and supernumerary molars in the rice rat

The genetics and development of fused and supernumerary molars in the rice rat /. Embryol. exp. Morph. Vol. 26, 1, pp. 99-109, 1971 99 Printed in Great Britain The genetics and development of fused and supernumerary molars in the rice rat By J. A. SOFAER 1 AND J. H. SHAW 2 From the

More information

Today: Mendel s Technique: What Mendel Observes: Mendelian Genetics: Consider this. Mendelian Genetics and Problems (In-Class 6)

Today: Mendel s Technique: What Mendel Observes: Mendelian Genetics: Consider this. Mendelian Genetics and Problems (In-Class 6) Today: Mendelian Genetics and Problems (In-Class 6) Mendelian Genetics: Consider this. 8 million possible chromosome combinations in each egg, and each sperm = >70 trillion possibilities! How are we able

More information

UNIT 6 Genes and Inheritance sciencepeek.com

UNIT 6 Genes and Inheritance sciencepeek.com Part 1 - Inheritance of Genes Name Date Period 1. Fill in the charts below on the inheritance of genes. 2. In a diploid cell, there are copies of each chromosome present. 3. Each human diploid cell has

More information

A was analyzed recently in two papers by the author (GERSTEL 1943: 1945a).

A was analyzed recently in two papers by the author (GERSTEL 1943: 1945a). INHERITANCE IN NICOTIANA TABACUM. XXI. THE MECHANISM OF CHROMOSOME SUBSTITUTION D. U. GERSTEL Division of Gendics, University of California, Berkeley Received January 3, 14 CYTOGENETIC basis for virus

More information

13. Cell division is. assortment. telophase. cytokinesis.

13. Cell division is. assortment. telophase. cytokinesis. Sample Examination Questions for Exam 1 Material Biology 3300 / Dr. Jerald Hendrix Warning! These questions are posted solely to provide examples of past test questions. There is no guarantee that any

More information

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING.

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING. MIDTERM EXAM 1 100 points total (6 questions) 8 pages PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING. PLEASE NOTE: YOU MUST ANSWER QUESTIONS 1-4 AND EITHER QUESTION 5 OR

More information

Genetics Problems. Character Dominant Recessive

Genetics Problems. Character Dominant Recessive Genetics Problems 1. A rooster with gray feathers is mated with a hen of the same phenotype. Among their offspring, 15 chicks are gray, 6 are black, and 8 are white. What is the simplest explanation for

More information

Genetics. What s Genetics? An organism s heredity is the set of characteristics it receives from its parents.

Genetics. What s Genetics? An organism s heredity is the set of characteristics it receives from its parents. Genetics Why don t you look exactly like your parents? Pull How are traits passed to the next generation? Pull What s Genetics? An organism s heredity is the set of characteristics it receives from its

More information

AP Biology Genetics Practice Alternative Modes of Inheritance

AP Biology Genetics Practice Alternative Modes of Inheritance AP Biology Genetics Practice Alternative Modes of Inheritance Name: Blk: Please put all answers on a separate sheet of paper and SHOW ALL WORK! 1. In snapdragons red flower color (R) is incompletely dominant

More information

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics Objectives In today s lab you will explore some of the simpler principles of Mendelian genetics using a computer program called CATLAB. By the end of this

More information

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below.

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below. IDTER EXA 1 100 points total (6 questions) Problem 1. (20 points) In this pedigree, colorblindness is represented by horizontal hatching, and is determined by an X-linked recessive gene (g); the dominant

More information

Sample Size Adapted from Schmidt, et al Life All Around Us.

Sample Size Adapted from Schmidt, et al Life All Around Us. Lab 9, Biol-1, C. Briggs, revised Spring 2018 Sample Size Adapted from Schmidt, et al. 2006. Life All Around Us. Name: Lab day of week: Objectives Observe the benefits of large sample sizes. Instructions

More information

Bew *Blue-Eyed White* Surface color: Pure White Undercolor: Pure White

Bew *Blue-Eyed White* Surface color: Pure White Undercolor: Pure White Self Group (Non-Agouti) The self group consists of black, blue, chocolate, and lilac. Also REW and BEW fall into the self group but have to be bred a certain way. Self is to have the same color over the

More information

Biology 100. ALE #8. Mendelian Genetics and Inheritance Practice Problems

Biology 100. ALE #8. Mendelian Genetics and Inheritance Practice Problems Biology 100 Instructor: K. Marr Name Lab Section Group No. Quarter ALE #8. Mendelian Genetics and Inheritance Practice Problems Answer the following questions neatly and fully in the spaces provided. References:

More information

Blue is the New Black How genes can influence appearance.

Blue is the New Black How genes can influence appearance. Blue is the New Black How genes can influence appearance. Backstory Humans have selectively bred plants and animals for thousands of years in order to create variations most useful to our purposes. This

More information

If you take the time to follow the directions below, you will be able to solve most genetics problems.

If you take the time to follow the directions below, you will be able to solve most genetics problems. Genetics Worksheet Part 1 Introduction: 1. Describe the genotypes given (use your notes). The first two are already done. A. DD homozygous, dominant D. ss B. Dd _heterozygous E. Yy C. dd F. WW 2. In humans,

More information

Incomplete Dominance and Codominance

Incomplete Dominance and Codominance Incomplete Dominance and Codominance Name Define incomplete dominance Incomplete dominance can be remembered in the form of Red flower X white flower = pink flower The trick is to recognize when you are

More information

Mendelian Inheritance Practice Problems

Mendelian Inheritance Practice Problems Name: Period: Mendelian Inheritance Practice Problems Team Problem 1 2. 3. Team Problem 2 2. Team Problem 3 Team Problem 4 Mendelian Inheritance Monohybrid Practice Problems In cats, long hair is recessive

More information

GCCF REGISTRATION POLICY FOR BRITISH SHORTHAIR CATS. Final Document V.1.7 (rev 5 March, 18)

GCCF REGISTRATION POLICY FOR BRITISH SHORTHAIR CATS. Final Document V.1.7 (rev 5 March, 18) GCCF REGISTRATION POLICY FOR BRITISH SHORTHAIR CATS Final Document V.1.7 (rev 5 March, 18) 1 Table of Contents British Registration Policy How to use this Document... 3 The Registers Explained... 5 Registration

More information

Basic Terminology and Eyeband Colors

Basic Terminology and Eyeband Colors Color Genetics of the Dwarf Hotot Amy Hinkle, M.S. Printed in the 2011 ADHRC Guidebook, Free to use and distribute WITH ATTRIBUTION: Hinkle, Amy. "Color Genetics of the Dwarf Hotot." American Dwarf Hotot

More information

The Rest of the Story. Fine Points of Mendelian Genetics. Alleles don t necessarily come in two forms only! The Rest of the Story 3/9/11

The Rest of the Story. Fine Points of Mendelian Genetics. Alleles don t necessarily come in two forms only! The Rest of the Story 3/9/11 Fine Points of Mendelian Genetics illustrated copiously, primarily with examples of Felis domesticus plagiarized from web pages too numerous to count The Rest of the Story Mendel announced his findings

More information

The genetic factors under consideration in the present study include black (+) vs. red (y), a sex-linked pair of alternatives manifesting

The genetic factors under consideration in the present study include black (+) vs. red (y), a sex-linked pair of alternatives manifesting GENE FREQUENCES N BOSTON'S CATS NEL B. TODD* The Biological Laboratories, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 218 Received 29.Vi.6 1. NTRODUCTON THREE previous papers have appeared on gene frequencies

More information

Complex Patterns of Inheritance Puzzle Stations Station #1: Multiple alleles, blood types

Complex Patterns of Inheritance Puzzle Stations Station #1: Multiple alleles, blood types Station #1: Multiple alleles, blood types (Remember, the possible multiple alleles for blood are written as I A, I B, i, with types A and B being codominant, and O being recessive.) 1. A man with blood

More information

Making Babies Lab 1. FACE SHAPE. 2. CHIN SHAPE: Next Three Flips. Very Prominent (VV, Vv) Less Prominent (vv)

Making Babies Lab 1. FACE SHAPE. 2. CHIN SHAPE: Next Three Flips. Very Prominent (VV, Vv) Less Prominent (vv) Making Babies Lab What would your baby look like if both you and your classmate (who will simulate your spouse) have one dominant gene and one recessive gene for each of the facial features illustrated

More information

B- indicates dominant phenotype

B- indicates dominant phenotype BIO 208 Genetics 2011 1 Applied Human Genetics Pedigree Analysis Monohybrid Cross Dihybrid Cross Chi Square Analysis Probability Epistasis I. Applied Human Genetics/Single Gene Traits The classical study

More information

Mendel s Laws: Their Application to Solving Genetics Problem

Mendel s Laws: Their Application to Solving Genetics Problem Solving Genetics Problems Page 1 Mendel s Laws: Their Application to Solving Genetics Problem Objectives This lab activity is designed to teach students how to solve classic genetics problems using Mendel

More information

Genetics Practice Problems. 1. For each genotype, indicate whether it is heterozygous (HE) or homozygous (HO) AA Bb Cc Dd.

Genetics Practice Problems. 1. For each genotype, indicate whether it is heterozygous (HE) or homozygous (HO) AA Bb Cc Dd. Name Period Genetics Practice Problems 1. For each genotype, indicate whether it is heterozygous (HE) or homozygous (HO) AA Bb Cc Dd Ee ff GG HH Ii Jj kk Ll Mm nn OO Pp 2. For each of the genotypes below,

More information

Heredity and Genetics Notes- Enriched

Heredity and Genetics Notes- Enriched Heredity and Genetics Notes- Enriched Def: Law of Segregation or independent assortment Def: Ex: BB Bb bb Dominance and recessive Traits Traits Stem length Seed shape Seed colour Seed coat colour Pod shape

More information

Seed color is either. that Studies Heredity. = Any Characteristic that can be passed from parents to offspring

Seed color is either. that Studies Heredity. = Any Characteristic that can be passed from parents to offspring Class Notes Genetic Definitions Trait = Any Characteristic that can be passed from parents to offspring Heredity The passing of traits from parent to offspring - Blood Type - Color of our Hair - Round

More information

Cardigan Welsh Corgi Coat Color Genetics. a report to the Cardigan Welsh Corgi Club of America

Cardigan Welsh Corgi Coat Color Genetics. a report to the Cardigan Welsh Corgi Club of America Cardigan Welsh Corgi Coat Color Genetics a report to the Cardigan Welsh Corgi Club of America Introduction April 3, 2005 Sheila Schmtuz Over the past year, Sue Buxton, as the Health Representative for

More information

MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS 1. Mendel verified true-breeding pea plants for certain traits before undertaking his experiments. The term true-breeding refers to: A. genetically pure lines. B. organisms that

More information

W. E. CASTLE C. C. LITTLE. Castle, W. E., and C. C. Little On a modified Mendelian ratio among yellow mice. Science, N.S., 32:

W. E. CASTLE C. C. LITTLE. Castle, W. E., and C. C. Little On a modified Mendelian ratio among yellow mice. Science, N.S., 32: ON A MODIFIED MENDELIAN RATIO AMONG YELLOW MICE. W. E. CASTLE C. C. LITTLE BUSSEY INSTITUTION, HARVARD UNIVERSITY Castle, W. E., and C. C. Little. 1910. On a modified Mendelian ratio among yellow mice.

More information

Sex-linked/incomplete dominance/codominance quiz

Sex-linked/incomplete dominance/codominance quiz 1. What is the difference between genotype and phenotype? a. Genotype is the physical characteristics; phenotype is the genetic make-up. b. Genotype is the genetic make-up; phenotype is the physical characteristics.

More information

Genetics Worksheet. Name

Genetics Worksheet. Name Genetics Worksheet Name Section A: Vocabulary 1. Identify if the alleles are homozygous (Ho) or heterozygous (He). a. DD b. Ee c. tt d. Hh 2. For each genotype below, determine the phenotype. a. Purple

More information

GCCF REGISTRATION POLICY FOR BRITISH SHORTHAIR CATS. Proposal for Revision 2012

GCCF REGISTRATION POLICY FOR BRITISH SHORTHAIR CATS. Proposal for Revision 2012 GCCF REGISTRATION POLICY FOR BRITISH SHORTHAIR CATS Proposal for Revision 2012 Outcrossing As a working group, the BAC have recently spent time preparing the British Shorthair Breeding Policy. Therefore,

More information

Mendel s Laws of Inheritance

Mendel s Laws of Inheritance Mendel s Laws of Inheritance From his work on the inheritance of phenotypic traits in peas, Mendel formulated a number of ideas about the inheritance of characters. These were later given formal recognition

More information

RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS.

RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS. RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS. Published on the AWEBSA webpage with the kind permission of the author: Robert Manvell. Please visit his page and view photos of

More information

Selfs. What am I? Tan Pattern. Solids

Selfs. What am I? Tan Pattern. Solids Agoutis Selfs Marked Tan Pattern Solids What am I? 1 Agoutis Selfs Beige Black Chocolate Dilute Agouti Golden Agouti Tan Pattern Broken Color Black Tan Dalmatian Chocolate Tan Silver Agouti Solids Cream

More information

Different versions of a single gene are called allleles, and one can be dominant over the other(s).

Different versions of a single gene are called allleles, and one can be dominant over the other(s). Answer KEY 1 Different versions of a single gene are called allleles, and one can be dominant over the other(s). 2 Describe genotype and phenotype in your own words. A genotype is the genetic makeup of

More information

Selfs. What am I? Tan Pattern. Solids

Selfs. What am I? Tan Pattern. Solids Note: Page numbers referenced in this document are to the ARBA Standard of Perfection. s Selfs Marked Tan Pattern s What am I? 1 s Selfs Beige Black Dilute Golden Tan Pattern Broken Color Black Tan Dalmatian

More information

Name: Project RECEIVED: Project DUE: Project is worth total points

Name: Project RECEIVED: Project DUE: Project is worth total points Name: Project RECEIVED: _ Project DUE: Project is worth total points Our third quarter project will be based on the concepts of iosis and Genetics. During this project we will specifically look at the

More information

Biology 3201 Sex Linked Review Mr.Gillam Name:

Biology 3201 Sex Linked Review Mr.Gillam Name: Biology 3201 Sex Linked Review Mr.Gillam Name: A female has the chromosomes XX, while a male has the chromosomes XY. In sex-linked inheritance the genes are carried on the chromosome and as a rule and

More information

Soap Opera Genetics Genetics to Resolve Family Arguments 1

Soap Opera Genetics Genetics to Resolve Family Arguments 1 Soap Opera Genetics Genetics to Resolve Family Arguments 1 I. How could our baby be an albino? Tiffany and Joe have just had a baby and are very surprised to learn that their baby is albino with very pale

More information

ON COMMERCIAL poultry farms during

ON COMMERCIAL poultry farms during Effect of Date of Hatch on Weight F. P. JEFFREY Department of Poultry Husbandry, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey (Presented at annual meeting June, 1940; received for publication May 23,

More information

TOPIC 8: PUNNETT SQUARES

TOPIC 8: PUNNETT SQUARES Page 1 TOPIC 8: PUNNETT SQUARES PUNNETT SQUARES 8.1: Definition A Punnett square is a device to help you predict the possible genotypes of the offspring if you know the genotypes of the parents. Because

More information

Breeding Bunnies. Purpose: To model the changes in gene frequency over several generations. 50 orange beads 50 purple beads 1 paper bag 3 cups

Breeding Bunnies. Purpose: To model the changes in gene frequency over several generations. 50 orange beads 50 purple beads 1 paper bag 3 cups Breeding Bunnies 1 Name Breeding Bunnies Background Information: Sometimes the frequency of changes in a population over a period of time. This means that how often you will see a particular trait will

More information

Determining the Inheritance Patterns of Purple Eye, Lobe Eye, and Yellow Body Traits of. Drosophilia Flies. Introduction

Determining the Inheritance Patterns of Purple Eye, Lobe Eye, and Yellow Body Traits of. Drosophilia Flies. Introduction Karen Jacques and Audrey Puleio Mrs. Lajoie Honors Biology April 30, 2012 Determining the Inheritance Patterns of Purple Eye, Lobe Eye, and Yellow Body Traits of Drosophilia Flies Introduction This experiment

More information

Cow Exercise 1 Answer Key

Cow Exercise 1 Answer Key Name Cow Exercise 1 Key Goal In this exercise, you will use StarGenetics, a software tool that simulates mating experiments, to analyze the nature and mode of inheritance of specific genetic traits. Learning

More information

Worksheet for Morgan/Carter Laboratory #9 Mendelian Genetics II: Drosophila

Worksheet for Morgan/Carter Laboratory #9 Mendelian Genetics II: Drosophila Worksheet for Morgan/Carter Laboratory #9 Mendelian Genetics II: Drosophila Ex. 9-1: ESTABLISHING THE ENZYME REACTION CONTROLS Propose a hypothesis about AO activity in flies from vial 1a and flies from

More information

Mendelian Genetics and Punnett Squares 5/07 Integrated Science 2 Redwood High School Name: Period:

Mendelian Genetics and Punnett Squares 5/07 Integrated Science 2 Redwood High School Name: Period: Mendelian Genetics and Punnett Squares 5/07 Integrated Science 2 Redwood High School Name: Period: Background Monohybrid crosses are crosses in which only one characteristic/trait is considered. For example,

More information

F same general plumage color pattern but differ in details of color are recognized,

F same general plumage color pattern but differ in details of color are recognized, A TRIPLE-ALLELE SERIES AND PLUMAGE COLOR IN TURKEYS V. S. ASMUNDSON L%sivn of Podtry Husbandry, University of California, Davis Received January 3, 1945 IVE subspecies of the wild turkey (Meleagris gallopavo)

More information

Bell Ringer. Which features do you have that match your mother? Your father? Which of the following features do you have?

Bell Ringer. Which features do you have that match your mother? Your father? Which of the following features do you have? Bell Ringer Which features do you have that match your mother? Your father? Which of the following features do you have? Widow s Peak? Ability to roll your tongue? Attached earlobes? Simple Genetics Exploring

More information

Human Genetics: Create-a-Person

Human Genetics: Create-a-Person Human Genetics: Create-a-Person Have you ever wondered why people look so different? Even close relatives don t look exactly alike. This happens because a large variety of traits exist in the human population

More information

Genetics Extra Practice Show all work!

Genetics Extra Practice Show all work! Name: # Date: Per: Genetics Extra Practice Show all work! Monohybrids 1. A cross between two pea plants hybird for a single trait produces 60 offspring. Approximately how many of the offspring would be

More information

The Pigeon Genetics Newsletter

The Pigeon Genetics Newsletter The Pigeon Genetics Newsletter News, Views, and Comments. Editor: R J Rodgers, Nova Scotia, Canada Co-Editor: Jith Peter, Palakkad, India March 2016, Volume 4, page 1 Section # (1) Beginner Text &Photos:

More information

DO NOT WRITE ON THIS TEST Unit 6 Assessment Genetics Objective 3.2.2

DO NOT WRITE ON THIS TEST Unit 6 Assessment Genetics Objective 3.2.2 DO NOT WRITE ON THIS TEST Unit 6 Assessment Objective 3.2.2 Vocabulary Matching + 1 point each 1. dominant 2. recessive 3. genotype 4. phenotype 5. heterozygous 6. homozygous 7. incomplete dominance 8.

More information

Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD

Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD Glossary Gene = A piece of DNA that provides the 'recipe' for an enzyme or a protein. Gene locus = The position of a gene on a chromosome.

More information

Mendelian Genetics Part 4: Dihybrid Cross

Mendelian Genetics Part 4: Dihybrid Cross Mendelian Genetics Part 4: Dihybrid Cross Name Terms and Explanations Explain the following terms and concepts, using both a diagram and an explanation in sentences or statements: Monohybrid cross Meiosis

More information

Study of genes and traits and how they are passed on.

Study of genes and traits and how they are passed on. Mendel Single Trait Experiments _ Genetics _ Biology.mp4 Heredity Meet the Super Cow [www.keepvid Study of genes and traits and how they are passed on. Law of Segregation Alleles pairs separate during

More information

1 st Type basic vocabulary and setting up Punnett Squares:

1 st Type basic vocabulary and setting up Punnett Squares: Genetics Punnett Square Review Questions Work booklet Name: There are several types of questions that involve the use of Punnett Squares in this unit. Here s the break down or summary of those problems.

More information