Herpetology Notes, volume 9: (2016) (published online on 05 September 2016) Luis Alberto Rueda Solano 1, * and Karen M.

Similar documents
First record of visual displays in Scinax cardosoi (Anura: Hylidae)

Rhinella marina (Cane Toad or Crapaud)

CONSERVATION ACTIONS FOR NATIVE AND THREATENED AMPHIBIANS OF COLOMBIAN CARIBBEAN REGION

Objectives: Outline: Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles. Characteristics of Amphibians. Types and Numbers of Amphibians

Doug Scull s SCIENCE & NATURE

B-Division Herpetology Test. By: Brooke Diamond

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Outline. Identifying Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles

Range extension of the critically endangered true poison-dart frog, Phyllobates terribilis (Anura: Dendrobatidae), in western Colombia

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF CTENOPHORUS CAUDICINCTUS (AGAMIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

Piggy s Herpetology Test

2019 Herpetology (B/C)

A.13 BLAINVILLE S HORNED LIZARD (PHRYNOSOMA BLAINVILLII)

SALAMANDERS. Helpful Hints: What is a Salamander: Physical Characteristics:

Reptile Identification Guide

JoJoKeKe s Herpetology Exam

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

4/8/10. Introduction to Exotics. Exotic Fish and Invertebrates Exotic Reptiles Exotic Amphibians

Station 1 1. (3 points) Identification: Station 2 6. (3 points) Identification:

Plestiodon (=Eumeces) fasciatus Family Scincidae

A Comparison of morphological differences between Gymnophthalmus spp. in Dominica, West Indies

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

Northern Copperhead Updated: April 8, 2018

Byall, C., H. M. Smith, and D. Chiszar Response of Brown Tree Snakes (Boiga

08 AMPHIBIANS & REPTILES (B) AND HERPETOLOGY (C) TRAINING HANDOUT By Karen L. Lancour

PEREGRINE FALCON HABITAT MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES ONTARIO MINISTRY OF NATURAL RESOURCES

8/19/2013. Who eats herps? Topic 20: Predators. Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps?

Bobcat Interpretive Guide

8/19/2013. What is a community? Topic 21: Communities. What is a community? What are some examples of a herp species assemblage? What is a community?

Agalychnis callidryas Red-Eyed Tree Frog, Red-Eyed Leaf Frog, Gaudy Leaf Frog, Rana Arborícola, Rana Calzonudo

Characteristics of a Reptile. Vertebrate animals Lungs Scaly skin Amniotic egg

Unit 19.3: Amphibians

Provision of egg-laying sites for captive breeding of the endangered Fijian ground frog Platymantis vitianus

WHAT ARE HERPTILES? WHICH IS WHICH? 1. Vertebrates are animals that have 2. Complete the following chart of vertebrate groups: EGGS LAID WHERE?

REPTILES & AMPHIBIANS

4 Many species of mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians and fish 940L. Source 1 Habitats

Animal Form and Function. Amphibians. United by several distinguishing apomorphies within the Vertebrata

Reptiles and amphibian behaviour

The Red-Eyed Tree Frog By Joy Cowley, Nic Bishop

A.13 BLAINVILLE S HORNED LIZARD (PHRYNOSOMA BLAINVILLII)

FIELD GUIDE TO NORTH AMERICAN MAMMALS Northern Short tailed Shrew (Blarina brevicauda)

Squamates of Connecticut

Conservation of the Magdalena River Turtle in the Sinú River, Colombia

The platypus lives in streams, ponds, and rivers in Australia. It closes its eyes under water and uses its bill to dig in the mud to find its food.

NAME: DATE: SECTION:

Captains Tryouts Herpetology Key. John P. Stevens High School. Rishabh Rout & Cindy Xu. Points: 114

Big Cat Rescue Presents. Tigrina or Oncilla

Herpetology, Third Edition: An Introductory Biology Of Amphibians And Reptiles By Laurie J. Vitt, Janalee P. Caldwell

Suitable age group: 10 and older These printable lessons will be added to as time goes along. (Solutions to questions are not provided)

By Dennis A. Thoney, Ph.D.

Megascops choliba (Tropical Screech Owl)

8/19/2013. Topic 14: Body support & locomotion. What structures are used for locomotion? What structures are used for locomotion?

Amphibians and Reptiles Division B

Most amphibians begin life as aquatic organisms and then live on land as adults.

Coccyzus minor (Mangrove Cuckoo)

Reptiles Notes. Compiled by the Davidson College Herpetology Laboratory

Active Searching: As a fauna survey technique.

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

Writing: Lesson 23. Today the students will practice planning for informative/explanatory prompts in response to text they read.

Vertebrates. Vertebrates are animals that have a backbone and an endoskeleton.

Announcements. Results: due today at 5pm for weekend feedback, otherwise due at Monday at 9am

This Coloring Book has been adapted for the Wildlife of the Table Rocks

Just Frogs. Just Frogs is published by Bookpx, LLC. Copyright 2011 Bookpx, LLC. All photography Copyright 2011 Nature s Eyes, Inc

A new species of torrent toad (Genus Silent Valley, S. India

Cnemidophorus lemniscatus (Rainbow Whiptail)

Dipsas trinitatis (Trinidad Snail-eating Snake)

Nesting in the Gladiator Frog, Hypsiboas boans (Anura: Hylidae), in Trinidad and Tobago

Time of Day. Teacher Lesson Plan Nocturnal Animals Pre-Visit Lesson. Overview

Draw a line from the names below to the animals they match. Red Fox. Wild Turkey. Wood Duck. White-tailed Deer. Black Bear

The tailed frog has been found from sea level to near timberline ( m; Province of BC 1999).

Current Status of Amphibian Populations. Amphibian biology - characteristics making

THE KOMODO DRAGON. endangered species L ARCHE PHOTOGRAPHIQUE CHARACTERISTICS. Animal Phylum. Kingdom

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

DO NOW: Invertebrate POP Quiz. Sit Quietly and clear off your desk/table of everything EXCEPT and blank piece of white lined paper and a pen/pencil.

I LOVE MY DRAGONS! Dragons of Sydney Harbour Factsheet Kids Version

Soraya Wijntuin, CI-SURINAME

Field Herpetology Final Guide

There are 35 phyla of animals These phyla can be classified into two groups (vertebrates or invertebrates) based on external and internal physical

Addressing the Wallacean Shortfall for small vertebrates in the Western Ghats across space

WildlifeCampus Advanced Snakes & Reptiles 1. Vipers and Adders

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Talks generally last minutes and take place in one of our classrooms.

THE risk of predation has pervasive lethal and nonlethal

Phylum Chordata. Fish, Amphibians, Reptiles

Cub Scout Den Meeting Outline

Endangered and Endemic Species of India (8 Marks)

Writing: Lesson 31. Today the students will be learning how to write more advanced middle paragraphs using a variety of elaborative techniques.

Acorn Ecology Certificate Course Self-Study Tutorial. British Reptile & Amphibian ID ( and a bit about surveying too!)

ZooTrek : Adaptations. Grades 6 8

35 phyla of animals These phyla can be classified into two groups (vertebrates or invertebrates) based on external

CHOOSING YOUR REPTILE LIGHTING AND HEATING

Title of Project: Distribution of the Collared Lizard, Crotophytus collaris, in the Arkansas River Valley and Ouachita Mountains

KS3 Adaptation. KS3 Adaptation. Adaptation dominoes Trail

Biology. Slide 1of 50. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

PETITION TO LIST THE Virgin Islands Coqui (Eleutherodactylus schwartzi)

Like mother, like daughter: inheritance of nest-site

Frogs, Toads, And Treefrogs (Complete Pet Owner's Manual) By Patricia Bartlett, R.D. Bartlett

THE CHILDREN S ZOO. Scavenger Hunt GRADES K-3

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema

Transcription:

Herpetology Notes, volume 9: 191-195 (2016) (published online on 05 September 2016) Foraging behavior with possible use of substrate-borne vibrational cues for prey localization in Atelopus laetissimus (Ruiz-Carranza, Ardila-Robayo and Hernández-Camacho, 1994) Luis Alberto Rueda Solano 1, * and Karen M. Warkentin 2,3 The use of substrate-borne vibrational cues during foraging is well documented in arthropods (Cocroft and Rodríguez, 2005; Hill, 2009; Cocroft et al., 2014), especially arachnids (Barth et al., 1988; Brownell and Farley, 1979; Hill, 2009). However, vibration-cued foraging is insufficiently studied in terrestrial vertebrates (Hill, 2009). Herpetologically, it has only been tested in snakes (Young and Morain, 2002; Shine et al., 2004) and one lizard species (Hetherington, 1989). Amphibians are the terrestrial vertebrates most sensitive to vibrations (Hill, 2008), but the use of substrate-borne vibrational cues as a source of information has been reported in only a few species in limited contexts (Narins, 1990; Lewis et al., 2001; Hill, 2009; 2008; Cocroft et al., 2014). In predator-prey interactions, the use of vibrational cues is known from the escape-hatching behaviour of Agalychnis callidryas embryos. Vibrations produced during snake attacks on egg clutches induce embryo hatching (Warkentin, 2005). Toe twitching behaviour in frogs has been hypothesized to function in foraging by generating vibrations that elicit prey movement or by serving as a visual lure (Hagman and Shine, 2008; Sloggett and Zeilstra, 2008). Nevertheless, to our knowledge, there are no reports of the use of vibrational cues for prey detection or localization during foraging, in any amphibian species. Atelopus laetissimus (Ruíz-Carranza et al., 1994) is an endemic harlequin frog from the wet montane forest of the Sierra Nevada de Santa Marta (SNSM), Colombia (Fig. 1). This species, like almost all harlequin frogs (Atelopus spp.), is threatened with extinction (La Marca et al., 2005; IUCN, 2015). Nonetheless, Atelopus laetissimus, A. nahumae and A. carrikeri populations in wet mountain forests and paramos of the SNSM survive with good conservation status (Rueda-Solano et al., 2016) (Fig. 1). Unlike most Atelopus, A. laetissimus is highly nocturnal, and A. nahumae is also active at night (Rueda-Solano et al., 2016). At night both species can be found perched on leaves along high-altitude mountain streams (Granda-Rodríguez et al., 2008; LARS pers. obs.), and both species use their nocturnal perch for foraging (Rueda Solano et al., in prep.) (Fig. 2). During the day, it is very rare to see Atelopus laetissimus, and 1 Facultad de Ciencias Básicas, Universidad del Magdalena, Carrera 32 No 22-08 C.P. No. 470004, Santa Marta D.T.C.H. - Colombia. 2 Department of Biology, Boston University, 5 Cummington Mall, Boston, MA, 02215, U.S.A. 3 Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Apartado Postal 0843-03092, Panamá, República de Panamá. * Corresponding author e-mail: biologoluisrueda@gmail.com Figure 1. Sierra Nevada de Santa Marta, north of Colombia, A. laetissimus locality (Δ); Atelopus nahumae locality (x); and A. carrikeri locality ( ).

192 Luis Alberto Rueda Solano & Karen M. Warkentin Figure 2. Individuals of Atelopus laetissimus (A. and B.) and Atelopus nahumae (C. and D.) perched on leaves in nocturnal foraging activity; Sierra Nevada de Santa Marta, Colombia. Photographs by Luis Alberto Rueda Solano. the few observations are all on terrestrial microhabitats such as leaf litter or rocks along streams. Thus, it appears that they climb onto plants only at night (LARS pers. obs.). In early May 2009, during the dry to rainy season transition at quebrada San Lorenzo (11 06 N 74 03 W, 2100 m, San Lorenzo Serrania, upper basin of Gaira river drainage; Fig. 1), the first author (LARS) observed the typical nocturnal foraging behavior of Atelopus laetissimus (Fig. 3A). Observations and video recordings were made using red light, at 20:00 h. The first focal A. laetissimus (male, 4.5 g, 4 cm SVL) was found perched on a Melastomataceae leaf about 0.5 m above the ground, immediately next to the stream. He had an active and alert body position with eyes open and limbs moderately separated (Fig. 3A), and there were several other male individuals with similar behaviour nearby. The initial focal individual was observed staying immobile for almost ten minutes without disturbance. After that, a simple behavioural assay was performed: LARS used a small twig to touch the edge of the leaf on which the frog was perched, moving it against the leaf to generate a vibrational and visual stimulus (Fig. 3B). There were no insects visible on the twig or on the plant (Fig. 3B). The twig-leaf contact elicited the frog s attention. It oriented towards the stimulus (Fig. 3C) and, within a few seconds, struck with its tongue at the twig-leaf contact point (Fig. 3D). The frog then resumed its alert position (Fig. 3E). LARS then repeated the stimulus on another leaf of the same plant, adjacent to the frog (Fig. 3F). Like the first time, this elicited orientation, movement towards the stimulus (Fig. 3G) and then an attack on the twig-leaf contact point (Fig. 3H). Following both attacks, the frog resumed its alert position (Fig. 3I). This trial was made with two individuals more along 50 m of the San Lorenzo stream with similar outcomes. In early May 2015, additional modified behavioural assays were performed with another, geographically distinct, Atelopus laetissimus population, in the middle basin of the Ancho River at 900 m (SNSM), La Guajira

Foraging behavior in Atelopus laetissimus 193 Figure 3. Video sequence showing possible use of vibrational cues for prey localization by foraging Atelopus laetissimus, San Lorenzo, Colombia. (A) Male Atelopus laetissimus on a leaf in nocturnal foraging position; (B, F) generating vibrational and visual stimulus by touching leaf with twig, white arrows indicates the twig-leaf contact point; (C, G) frog attention to stimulus (head orientation and movement toward it); (D, H) attacks on the exact position of the stimulus, yellow arrows indicate stimulus/ attack position; (E, I) active foraging posture following attacks. Still frames from video recorded by Luis Alberto Rueda Solano. Colombia. These observations were also made using red light, at 20:00 h. Males in this population are smaller (about 3 cm SVL) than the San Lorenzo population. In addition, unlike the San Lorenzo frogs, which were entirely immobile when unstimulated, the La Guajira frogs moved repeatedly, assuming unusual body postures on the leaf during the initial observation periods (Fig. 4A). They stretched their fore- and hindlimbs while lifting up their chin (Fig. 4B). For the behavioural assays, we tested two frogs and used three variations of the stimulus: visual + vibrational (touching the leaf edge with a twig), visual only (moving twig near leaf edge, without touching it), and vibrational only (touching twig to the underside of the leaf, out of sight of the frog). We tested all stimuli systematically; first visual + vibrational stimulus; second was visual only; and last one was vibrational only. Each stimulus was presented for five minutes per trial, with a three-minute wait between stimuli. With the vibrational + visual stimulus, the frogs showed similar behavioural responses to the San Lorenzo population. They were attracted by the stimulus, moving slowly over the leaf to the twig-leaf contact point. With the visual-only stimulus, the frogs showed no evident behavioural response; they just stayed immobile on the leaf with their limbs stretched and chin raised. With the vibration-only stimulus, the frogs showed head orientation towards the stimulus and slowly moved towards the twig-leaf contact point. In addition, they displayed toe-twitching behaviour, tapping on the leaf while the stimulus was presented and for a few seconds after it ended. The toe twitching was only displayed in response to the vibration-only stimulus, when the point of twig-leaf contact was under the leaf. We did not see this behaviour in other contexts. To date, Atelopus have only been reported to use nocturnal perches on vegetation for resting (Lindquist et al., 2007). Our observations reveal that A. laetissimus and A. nahume also use such perches for foraging. Moreover, our initial, simple behavioural assays of male Atelopus laetissimus suggest these frogs may use vibrational cues transmitted through plants for prey localization, as part

194 Luis Alberto Rueda Solano & Karen M. Warkentin Figure 4. Male Atelopus laetissimus in nocturnal foraging posture, La Guajira, Colombia. (A) Profile, showing raised chin and extended limbs. (B). Ventral view, showing lateral extension of forelimbs and raised chin. Still frames from video recorded by Luis Alberto Rueda Solano. of their nocturnal foraging behaviour. However, we do not reject that other visual and/or auditory cues may also be used. Thus, a rigorous experimental test of the roles of vibration and other sensory modalities as foraging cues would be worthwhile. Most Atelopus species are diurnally active (Lötters, 1996), suggesting that A. laetissimus probably has relatively poor night vision. This could make the use of non-visual sensory modalities for foraging at night more useful. Moreover, most Atelopus lack a tympanum and middle ear structures, presumably reducing their sensitivity to airborne sound (Lindquist et al., 1998). It would be interesting to assess the impact of this morphology on vibrational sensitivity, if vibrational cues are important for foraging. Acknowledgments. We are grateful to the Conservation Leadership Program for the financial support of the Atelopus Project Colombia (Project ID: 02177014), and to the administrators of Parques Nacionales Naturales of Colombia Territorial Caribe and indigenous communities for their logistic support of our field trips. Thanks to A. Rocha, L. Mejia, L. Jimenez, J. Perez, J. Eguis, T. Mejia, J. Rueda, J.S. Mendoza and F. Vargas-Salinas for their field-work support in Ancho River. Magdalena University supported LARS scientific visit to the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Gamboa, Panamá, in 2015, to work with KMW. KMW was supported by Boston University, the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, and the National Science Foundation (IOS-1354072). Literature Cited Barth, F. G., Bleckmann, H., Bohnenberger, J., Seyfarth, E.A. (1988): Spiders of the genus Cupiennius Simon 1891 (Araneae, Ctenidae) - II. On the vibratory environment of a wandering spider. Oecologia 77(2): 194-201. Brownell, P., Farley, R.D. (1979): Prey-localizing behaviour of the nocturnal desert scorpion, Paruroctonus mesaensis: Orientation to substrate vibrations. Animal Behaviour 27: 185-193. Cocroft, R.B., Gogala, M., Hill, P.S.M., Wessel, A. (Eds.) (2014): Studying Vibrational Communication (Vol. 3). Heidelberg: Springer Cocroft, R.B., Rodríguez, R.L. (2005): The behavioral ecology of insect vibrational communication. BioScience 55(4): 323-334. Granda-Rodríguez, H.D., del Portillo-Mozo, A., Renjifo, J.M. (2008): Uso de hábitat en Atelopus laetissimus (Anura: Bufonidae) en una localidad de la Sierra Nevada de Santa Marta, Colombia. Herpetotropicos 4(2): 87-93. Hagman, M., Shine, R. (2008): Deceptive digits: the functional significance of toe waving by cannibalistic cane toads, Chaunus marinus. Animal Behaviour 75(1): 123-131. Hetherington, T.E. (1989): Use of vibratory cues for detection of insect prey by the sandswimming lizard Scincus scincus. Animal Behaviour 37: 290-297. Hill, P.S.M. (2008): Vibrational Communication in Animals. Cambridge, Massachusetts, London, England: Harvard University Press. Hill, P.S.M. (2009): How do animals use substrate-borne vibrations as an information source? Naturwissenschaften 96(12): 1355-1371. IUCN. (2016): The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2016-1. Available at: http://www.iucnredlist.org. Last accessed on 11 April 2016.

Foraging behavior in Atelopus laetissimus 195 La Marca, E., Lips K.R., Lötters, S., Puschendorf, R., Ibáñez, R., Rueda-Almonacid, J.V., Schulte, R., Marty, C., Castro, F., Manzanilla-Puppo, J., García-Pérez, J.E., Toral, E., Bolaños, F., Chaves, G., Pounds, J.A., Young, B.E. (2005): Catastrophic population declines and extinctions in Neotropical harlequin frogs (Bufonidae: Atelopus). Biotropica 37:190-201. Lewis, E.R., Narins, P.M., Cortopassi, K.A., Yamada, W.M., Poinar, E.H., Moore, S.W., Yu, X. (2001): Do Male White-Lipped Frogs Use Seismic Signals for Intraspecific Communication?. American Zoologist 41(5): 1185-1199. Lindquist, E.D., Hetherington, T.E., Volman, S.F. (1998): Biomechanical and neurophysiological studies on audition in eared and earless harlequin frogs (Atelopus). Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 183(2): 265-271. Lindquist, E.D., Sapoznick, S.A., Rodriguez, E.J.G., Johantgen, P.B., Criswell, J.M. (2007): Nocturnal position in the Panamanian Golden Frog, Atelopus zeteki (Anura, Bufonidae), with notes on fluorescent pigment tracking. Phyllomedusa 6(1): 37-44. Lötters, S. (1996): The Neotropical Toad Genus Atelopus: Checklist - Biology - Distribution. Zülpicher Str. 79 Köln Germany: M.Vences & F. Claw Verlags GbR. Narins, P.M. (1990): Seismic Communication in Anuran Amphibians. BioScience 40(4): 268-274. Rueda-Solano, L.A., Flechas, S.V., Galvis-Aparicio, M., Rocha- Usuga, A.A., Rincón-Barón, E.J., Cuadrado-Peña, B., Franke- Ante, R. (2016): Epidemiological surveillance and amphibian assemblage status at the Estación Experimental de San Lorenzo, Sierra Nevada de Santa Marta, Colombia. Amphibian & Reptile Conservation 10(1) [General Section]: 7 19 (e114). Ruíz-Carranza, P.M., Ardila-Robayo, M.C., Hernández-Camacho, J.I. (1994): Tres nuevas especies de Atelopus A.M.C. Duméril & Bibron 1841 (Amphibia: Bufonidae) de la Sierra Nevada de Santa Marta, Colombia. Revista de La Academia Colombiana de Ciencias Exactas, Físicas Y Naturales 19(72): 153-163 Shine, R., Brown, G.P., Elphick, M.J. (2004): Field experiments on foraging in free-ranging water snakes Enhydris polylepis (Homalopsinae). Animal Behaviour 68: 1313-1324. Sloggett, J.J., Zeilstra, I. (2008): Waving or tapping? Vibrational stimuli and the general function of toe twitching in frogs and toads (Amphibia: Anura). Animal Behaviour 76(5): 1-4. Warkentin, K.M. (2005): How do embryos assess risk? Vibrational cues in predator-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrogs. Animal Behaviour 70(1): 59-71. Young, B.A., Morain, M. (2002): The use of ground-borne vibrations for prey localization in the Saharan sand vipers (Cerastes). The Journal of Experimental Biology 205: 661-665. Accepted by Iris Starnberger