Open Letter to Canine Research Scientists by Linda Michaels, M.A. Psychology

Similar documents
The Kennel Club has long campaigned for a ban on the use and sale of electric shock collars in Scotland.

To choke or not to choke How positive reinforcement has affected the use of choke collars in dog training

Behavior Modification Why Punishment Should Be Avoided

Position statements. Updated May, 2013

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY INVESTIGATION REPORT. For KITCHENER WATERLOO HUMANE SOCIETY

Biting Beth Bradley All Bites are Not Created Equal Teaching Puppies Bite Inhibition

L A N G U A G E THE LANGUAGE OF ADVOCACY

New Student Registration (page 1 of 5)

The Kennel Club/Scottish Kennel Club campaign to prohibit the use of electric shock collars in Scotland

ORGANIZATIONS THAT DO NOT ENDORSE BREED SPECIFIC LEGISLATION

Dogs Trust Pawlicy Document

Strategy 2020 Final Report March 2017

Deconstructing the Growl:

The Evidence For Positive Reinforcement Training By Pippa Mattinson

Nicole Wilde. June 20 & 21, Proudly Presents. Radisson Hotel & Conference Center Ave NW. Edmonton, Alberta

Waiting for a Forever Home

Battersea response to the Public Audit and Post-legislative Scrutiny Committee s call for evidence on the Control of Dogs (Scotland) Act 2010

Learning How to Deal with Your Fearful Dog

Dangerous Dogs and Safeguarding Children Contents

Our. for all political parties ahead of the 2016 Welsh Assembly election.

Long-term Effects of Early Environments on the Behavior and Welfare of Dogs

Building Responsible Pet Ownership Communities The Calgary Model. Thursday, October 22, 15

Golden Rule Training

TRAINING & BEHAVIOR QUESTIONNAIRE

AGGRESSION (CATS) DIAGNOSING AND TREATING

Generalization by George Hickox

What this guide covers

SP6B NUISANCE BARKING IN DOGS AND THE WELFARE IMPLICATIONS OF DIFFERENT TREATMENT MODALITIES

XII. LEGISLATIVE POLICY STATEMENTS

Cats Protection our strategy and plans

Animal Behavior and Animal Welfare

Dog Training Collar Introduction

Timing is Everything By Deborah Palman

DECLARATION of the First Conference on Animal Welfare in the Baltic Region RESPONSIBLE OWNERSHIP 5 to 6 May, 2011, Vilnius, Lithuania

International Shelter Programme for Applied Canine Studies (ISPACS)

Big Bend Community Based Care Policy & Procedure

Michigan Pet Fund Alliance's Getting to the Goal Conference

Aggression in Dogs Overview Basics

The Right Steps Class Prerequisites & Class Details:

Dog Training Workshop

Toward a Common Swine Industry Audit

Name: Address: Dog s Name: Spayed/Neutered. Yes No. How long have you had the dog? Where was the dog acquired?

Aggression Social Aggression to Unfamiliar Dogs

Potential controls or prohibition of electronic training aids in Scotland

It s a Zoo Out There: Understanding Animals on Campus

BARRY COUNTY DOG PROJECT NOTEBOOK JUNIORS THIRD YEAR

Character Education CITIZENSHIP

Critical appraisal Randomised controlled trial questions

Table of Contents. Foreward 13 Introduction 15 Acknowledgements 17. Chapter 1: Modern Training Fundamentals 19

Conflict-Related Aggression

TOWN OF GORHAM ANIMAL CONTROL ORDINANCE

State of Mind. Bite Work. Snapshot. Understanding the Different Drives. K9 Kylo s Approach to School Safety. Conditioning the ON and OFF Switch

RHETORIC 49. A Born Killer? Leah Johnson

Animal Control Law Village of Bergen Local Law Number 2 of 2018

Humane Society of Berks County Animal Statistics & Reporting. A summary of the HSBC Pet Evaluation Matrix (PEM)

Guidelines for the Use of Nonhuman Animals in Behavioral Projects in Schools (K-12)

Expert Panel Addresses New Hidden Camera Investigation

Guide Dogs Puppy Development and Advice Leaflet. No.6 Recall and Free Running

ASSOCIATION OF THE BAR OF THE CITY OF NEW YORK COMMITTEE ON LEGAL ISSUES PERTAINING TO ANIMALS. Proposed City Council Ordinance: Sec.

WHY A BAN IS THE ONLY OPTION FOR THIRD PARTY PUPPY SALES

CIVIL GRAND JURY FINDINGS, RECOMMENDATIONS, AND RESPONSES TO FINDINGS AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Table of Contents Operating Guide Training Guide

Caring for people caring for animals since 1980

Your Dog s Evaluation Result: Separation Anxiety

Autism Service Dog Information Package:

TRAINING PRINCIPLES Teaching Basic Skills

Understanding your dog's behaviour will help you prevent and reduce behaviour problems.

Course Pack Introduction to Canine Anatomy and Physiology

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression Getting Started Safety and Management

W hat's I n Yo u r To 0 I box? Training Equipment - Pros and Cons

What is BQA s purpose? To ensure all consumers that all cattle are raised in a responsible manner ensuring safe, wholesome and healthy beef.

Jim Reynolds DVM, MPVM

Dog Bite Prevention Handout written by Steph Callahan

Our Philosophy. Playing for Life! A Shelter s Training Program featuring Canine Play Groups presented by

Hiking Shoes OR, HOW EQUIPMENT MATTERS

MEDICAL CENTER POLICY NO.

University Council on Animal Care

Behavior Modification Reinforcement and Rewards

Adoption Contract. I, (print name) (also referred to herein as Client ) residing at. Cell Phone #: Home Phone #:

LIVING IN A MULTI- DOG HOUSEHOLD

Patriot Assistance Dogs

Dr Leanne Lawrence. General Practitioner Auckland

I. PURPOSE POLICY STATEMENT

Outdoor Bark Control

Animal Welfare Policy

Joint Committee on Health and Children Meeting 19 th November Opening Statement by Ms Jennifer Dowler, CEO Irish Dogs for the Disabled

American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior: Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals

Sociology of Dogs. Educating Humans on Dog Welfare

Pre-Consultation Questionnaire

European trends in animal welfare policies and research and their potential implications for US Agriculture

Puppy Behavior and Training Handling and Food Bowl Exercises

The Dog and Cat Management Board. Policy and Procedure for the training of dogs subject to a dangerous dog order

DCSD HR DEPARTMENT THERAPY ANIMAL POLICY

Information Guide. Do you know dog law?

U N D E R S TA N D I N G O U R C A N I N E C O M PA N I O N S ( ADVANCED DIPLOMA ) DISTANCE LEARNING

Bluewater District School Board ADMINISTRATIVE PROCEDURE AP 6809-D Students Page 1. Guide Dog/Service Animal/Companion Dog (Use of in Schools)

ABOUT THE KENNEL CLUB AND EUKANUBA DISCOVER DOGS. WE ARE: The UK s largest organisation dedicated to the health and welfare of dogs.

Clicker Training Guide


KCAI Scheme Online Assessments: Criteria

Transcription:

Open Letter to Canine Research Scientists by Linda Michaels, M.A. Psychology Linda Michaels calls on canine research scientists to lead the way on the ethical treatment of companion animals It would require a long list to delineate the benefits of companion animal canine research conferences and live streams. Admirably, these conference bring canine researchers into the mainstream of the canine applied practices fields, i.e., dog training. Many pet parents and trainers take careful note of scientific positions and plan to incorporate the lessons-learned into their practice. The researchers benefit in kind from relationships forged with dog trainers and pet parents. Through these forums, canine research scientists may reap benefits that include research dollars, personal financial rewards in the form of speaker fees and opportunities for book sales, name and brand exposure. Although the preponderance of materials presented at dog behavior conferences often favour a force-free approach, notably shock collar snake aversion training and emergency recall are of particular concern. Critical empirically based data and a clear position on ethics are not always forthcoming or apparent in these areas. To be clear, no one is calling for a ban on scientific discussions of devices designed to cause pain, or of positive punishment, as has been suggested by some speakers. However, rationalising or justifying the use of such devices and methods is another matter. Positions against the use of shock and positive punishment with companion pets are not necessarily based in a misunderstanding of science and the misapplication of the principles of learning, as is hinted at by some presenters., nor are such positions necessarily emotion-based and blind to science. Indeed, the opposition to their use may well be based in a sophisticated academic grasp of the mechanisms at work and a perfectly understandable desire to promote animal welfare in a civilized world. Taking a Stand Voices that take a clear stand against pain-based collar devices and the crude manner in which positive punishment and aversive control methods are often used in dog training deserve to be heard in public forums. In addition to being the more ethical choice, non-aversive methods are not only safer but more effective with longer-lasting effects. Progressive zoos nationwide have moved away entirely from using positive punishment and aversive methods when working with large and even

potentially dangerous animals. Certainly, competent scientists, behavioral consultants and dog trainers can manage our pet dogs. The so-called balanced trainer who practices a mix of training methods is a contradiction of terms. Alternating rewards with aversives sends mixed and contradictory messages to our dogs. In one canine research conference, a comparison of electro-convulsive therapy (ECT) used to treat depression in humans and shocking a dog to decrease an undesireable behavior was put forth as a viable analogy. However, it was not mentioned that ECT is conducted on a patient who is UNCONSCIOUS. ECT is not a treatment used to punish and decrease the frequency of a behavior. These are not similar applications of shock. Regarding the use of shock on human beings, one school in Massachusetts currently treats developmentally disabled students with shock. The shock method of behavior modification has been likened to torture and its use is being investigated by the US Department of Justice as well as the United Nations (Associated Press, 2014). I think what has happened is that this has gone on for this long because this is a population who cannot adequately speak for themselves, said Dr. Karen Weigle, a clinical psychologist affiliated with the University of New Hampshire. Who will speak for the dogs? Responsible leadership in science is the future of pet animal training. Shock and Public Safety: Beyond B.F. Skinner Dog bites are a serious and growing public safety issue in the US. It is well-documented in scientific literature that inflicting pain is frequently a cause of aggressive behavior. According to an article in the Journal of Veterinary Behavior, Good Trainers: How to Identify One (Overall et al, 2006), investigators place shock collars, prong collars and choke collars at the top of the list of equipment that causes anxiety, fear and arousal which often contributes to an increase in aggression. Although shock may cause a decrease in the frequency of the behavior of snake seeking, for example, it may also increase aggression and/or cause learned helplessness. Moreover, administration of shock often has serious unintended, classically conditioned side-effects. Pioneering psychologist Martin E. Seligman s groundbreaking studies on learned helplessness indicate that inescapable shock may well produce immobility after the shock is removed (Seligman & Maier, 1967). Bite redirect incidents resulting from the direct administration of shock are not uncommon. Polsky (2000) meanwhile describes severe attacks on humans by dogs who were being trained or maintained on an electronic pet containment system. Science has come a long way since B. F. Skinner s radical behaviorism of the 1950s. An understanding of the rudimentary principles of reward and punishment is necessary for any serious student of behavior, however, the correspondingly important classic Misbehavior of

Organisms (Breland & Breland, 1961) authored by Skinner s own graduate students, states that there are definite weaknesses in the philosophy underlying these techniques. A recent well-attended canine behavior conference included an emotion-provoking photograph of a dog licking the head of a rattlesnake. Alongside it was a photograph of a dog running loose in the street chasing a car. Following that was a photo of a dog lying dead or seriously injured in the street. The caption read: Sometimes punishment can be merciful. This sounds more like religion than science. No empirical data on the efficacy of snake aversion training was offered nor was evidence demonstrating recall efficacy using punishment/aversive control provided. Snake Breaking The use of shock collars in dog training has become the standard for snake aversion training despite the lack of evidence for its efficacy. This oversight has led to inaccurate conclusions regarding the benefits of positive punishment by pet parents, trainers and some scientists as well. Empirical evidence is requisite to the scientific method. An assumption of efficacy regarding shock snake aversion training has far-reaching and possibly dangerous consequences. Dr. Karen Overall PhD VMD MA DiplACVB tells us (2007), There are no scientific studies on whether shock teaches dogs to avoid snakes, in part because the population data on the range of normal canine responses to snakes are lacking completely. Snake aversion training has not been shown to be either a valid or reliable method of snake deterrence (see also Pages 32-34). Shock snake-aversion training seeks to instill the flight response. However, the fight or flight response is often erratic and unpredictable, i.e., the dog could freeze, the dog could fight, or the dog could easily panic and get bitten by the snake. Science must ask and seek to answer the obvious questions: Does shock training do what it purports to do? What are the side-effects? On whose scale would the benefits outweigh the risks and cost to the dog and, moreover, to public safety? Peer Pressure in the Scientific Community Is it possible that some scientists may be conforming to the philosophy of the all-inclusive big tent dog training organizations who often sponsor canine research and dog training conferences? With the advent of the Pet Professional Guild (PPG) for the first time, professionals in our industry have a clear option to affiliate with an international organization whose member subscribe to: No Shock, No Pain, No Choke, No Fear, No Physical Force, No Physical Molding, No Compulsion Based Methods employed to train or care for a pet (2015). I entreat scientists to consider the impact their presentations may have on their audience and,

moreover, on our companion animals when speaking from such a scholarly platform. It is surely the responsibility of canine researchers to carefully examine the topics they present and to avoid presenting opinions and positions as science that may reflect their own personal bias and prejudice. Is it not the responsibility of scientists to debunk myths? Is it not the responsibility of scientists to attempt to disprove a null hypothesis rather than assuming that a treatment has a desired effect? Animal Abuse in Dog Training Shock collar training is a serious animal welfare concern in the US. Not infrequently animal abuse masquerades as dog training. In Why Shock is Not Behavior Modification (Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 2007), principal investigator Dr. Karen Overall states, The use of shock is not treatment for pets with behavioral concerns. Wales has banned shock collars as other countries have done and an active movement is presently calling for a ban of shock collars in England, Scotland and Canada. In 2004, The Kennel Club, London, published a list of countries supporting a ban. The website BanShockCollars.ca lists numerous scientific studies, many with full text available, that demonstrate the deleterious effects of using these devices on companion pets. A study that used both behavioral and physiological measures, compared the impact on dogs where shock collars were used during training to those where no shock was used during training. This study found behavioral evidence that the welfare of dogs was negatively impacted even when training was conducted by professional trainers using relatively benign protocols advised by shock collar advocates. The Department for Environmental Food and Rural Affairs, UK (2010). How long will corporate agendas and personal interests in the US override animal welfare and principles of Do No Harm? Myth: Shock vs. Snake Bite The alternatives of either shocking a dog or getting bitten by a snake is an overly simplistic false dichotomy and a fallacy put forth by shock proponens meant to foment fear in caring pet parents. So how can we prevent snake bites to our dogs? Critical thinking mandates that we investigate other options. A presumption of supposed life-saving benefits is unwarranted and unsupported by science. We ought to protect our dogs against dangerous and venomous snakes as we protect ourselves and the other members of our family. Would we allow our two-year old child to go romping about in the fields during snake season?

Myth: Shock vs. Euthanasia How can we reduce euthanasia for dogs presenting with behavior problems? A premise with the alternatives of either shocking a dog or having him euthanized is another false dichotomy commonly posed by shock proponents. The use of shock does not bring dogs back from the brink of euthanasia; instead, it may send them there, says Overall (2007). In actual fact, punitive methods or devices can cause an increase in anxiety which may lead to further behavioral problems, often causing pet parents to relinquish their animals to overflowing shelters. Barring neurological damage or deficits, I have yet to see a case in my behavioral consulting practice (including wolfdogs) that could not be treated adequately using non-aversive methods. Indeed, so-called red zone dogs are in need of non-aversive training as much if not more than others. Future Research In an effort to protect our companion pets from snakebites, there are less invasive and more humane alternatives on the horizon than administering shock that ought to be explored by the scientific community. The use of service dog training methods may work without the inherent psychological and medically injurious effects that can result from shock devices. Service dogs are trusted to guide blind people across busy streets, undistracted by prey animals. A standard behavior chain of Detect, Recall to handler, and Alert may be more reliable, predictable and humane for the dogs as well as for the snakes currently used in shock snake-breaking. In addition, a survey using the extensive Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ) database already in progress, or a separately designed survey, exploring the co-relational relationship between shock training and bites would be of practical value to science, animal welfare advocates, trainers and the public. It is the job of researchers to fairly and honestly share thorough and ethically sound information. We need to move forward with sensible standardization of the dog training industry with skill competency requirements, consumer transparency in advertising and professional accountability based squarely on humane treatment. We should seek to strengthen our welfare laws in the US. It is imperative that scientists consider their words carefully: Our governmental representatives will look closely at what scientists have to say about shock. How can the average pet parent be expected to make a well-informed decision with regard to behavior modification tools when so many dog trainers and manufacturers of shock products misrepresent the true nature of their effects?

There is simply no place for shock, prong, choke or any other aversive devices in companion animal research or training. I believe that, in the end, animal welfare and ethics will finally bring about a sea change in dog training that is already long over-due in the US. It is incumbent upon scientists researching canine behavior and those with advanced degrees in animal behavior to take a firm, clear and unwavering stance on these issues. Surely, it is understood that science cannot take a stand... but scientists can. I urge canine research scientists to take a leadership position on the ethical treatment of companion animals. This article cites a previous piece of work published by the same author in the BARKS from the Guild Magazine. Jan/Feb 2015. Linda Michaels M.A. (Hons Experimental Psychology) specializes in the psychological aspects of dog behavior, socialization, treatments and training and owns and operates Linda Michaels, M.A. Dog Training. Linda is a Licensed Fear/Aggression/ Reactivity Consultant, Certified Veterinary Assistant, Behavior Advisor for the Wolf Education Project, Professional Canine Trainer Accredited, Advisory Board member of Art for Barks, and founder of the Positive Pet Professionals of San Diego network. You may reach her through: DogPsychologistOnCall.com Live Links/References Associated Press (2014). Skin shocks used at Massachusetts school draw FDA investigation. Ban Shock Collars (2015). BanShockCollars.ca. Brammeier, S., Brennan, J., Brown, S., Bryant, D., Calnon, D., Cole Stenson, T., Seksel, K. (2006). Good trainers: How to identify one and why this is important to your practice of veterinary medicine. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 1, 47-52 Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 16, 681-684. Defra, Department for Environment Food and Rural Affairs (2010). Research Project Final Report. Kennel Club, London. Electric Shock Collars: Countries supporting a ban. Michaels, L., (2015, Jan/Feb). An open letter to research scientists. BARKS from the Guild, 10,

58-60. Overall, K. L., (2007). Why electric shock is not behavior modification. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 2, 1-4. Overall, K. L., (2007). Considerations for shock and training collars: Concerns from and for the working dog community. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 2, 103-107. Pet Professional Guild (2015). Retrieved from http://www.petprofessionalguild.com/ Polsky, R. P., (2000). Can aggression in dogs be elicited through the use of electronic pet containment systems. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 3, 345-357. Seligman, M.E.P., & Maier, S.F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology 74: 1 9.