FREQUENCY OF ENGAGEMENT WITH DIFFERENT MATERIALS BY GROWING RABBITS

Similar documents
Online Journal of Veterinary Research

ABSTRACTS OF THE WELFARE AND ETHOLOGY SECTION PRESENTED DURING THE 8 th WORLD RABBIT CONGRESS

Housing of growing rabbits in individual, bicellular and collective cages: growth performance, carcass traits and meat quality

CODE OF PRACTICE FOR THE CARE AND HANDLING OF RABBITS: REVIEW OF SCIENTIFIC RESEARCH ON PRIORITY ISSUES

Original scientific article Izvirni znanstveni prispevek

Convegno ASIC th WRC: Inviati speciali in Cina. 30 settembre 2016, Padova

Committee on Agriculture and Rural Development WORKING DOCUMENT. on minimum standards for the protection of farm rabbits

Minimum Requirements for the Keeping of Domestic Animals. 11 Cattle. Animal Protection Ordinance

Rabbit Scenario: Laboratory vs. Fancier

Meat Rabbit Scenario. Prepared by: S. Sosnowik & E. Patterson-Kane, edited by J. Siegford

Session Ethology and Welfare

EFFECT OF HANDLING IN PRE-WEANING RABBITS

Session Reproduction

Best Practice in the Breeder House

The 1999 EU Hens Directive bans the conventional battery cage from 2012.

Welfare Assessment of Farmed Rabbits Housed in Indoor and Outdoor Cages

Growth and Mortality of Suckling Rabbits

BROILER PRODUCTION GUIDE

New Housing Conditions: Improving the Welfare of Experimental Animals

How To... Why the correct whole-house brooding set-up is important?

Angora Welfare. Quality Assessment Programme

4-H Club. "To Make The Best Better" ADDRESS

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt

REARING LAYING HENS IN A BARN SYSTEM WITHOUT BEAK TRIMMING: THE RONDEEL EXAMPLE

Gas emissions according to different pig housing systems

AMENDMENTS EN United in diversity EN. European Parliament Draft report Stefan Eck. PE v01-00

The effects of feeding and housing on the behaviour of the laboratory rabbit

There are very serious welfare issues in the breeding and intensive rearing of meat chickens:

FOOD AND AGRICULTURE ORGANIZATION OF THE UNITED NATIONS Rome, 1983

Best Practice in the Breeder House

EGG production of turkeys is not important

RABBITS. Code of practice for keeping rabbits in Western Australia ISBN

Provision of additional walls in the resting area the effects on. resting behaviour and social interactions in goats

CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015

TECHNICAL NOTE: RABBIT MEAT PRODUCTION UNDER A SMALL SCALE PRODUCTION SYSTEM AS A SOURCE OF ANIMAL PROTEIN IN A RURAL AREA OF MEXICO.

Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and Sociality

RABBIT BEHAVIOUR QUESTIONNAIRE

Regulating Animal Welfare in the EU.the EU.

Unit D: Egg Production. Lesson 4: Producing Layers

Overview of some of the latest development and new achievement of rabbit science research in the E.U.

Age, lighting treatment, feed allocation and feed form influence broiler breeder feeding time

Comparative Evaluation of the Egg Production Performance Indicators of Hy-Line Hybrid Kept in Traditional Cage System versus the Enriched Cages One

The EFSA Journal (2005) 267, 1-31, The Impact of the current housing and husbandry systems on the health and welfare of farmed domestic rabbits

SOCIOMETRIC INVESTIGATIONS IN GROUPS OF WILD AND DOMESTIC RABBITS WITH ONE BUCK AND TWO OR THREE DOES

MARKET RABBIT PROJECT BOOK

Recommended Resources: The following resources may be useful in teaching

Lameness in Irish pigs. Laura Boyle Teagasc Moorepark

Exterior egg quality as affected by enrichment resources layout in furnished laying-hen cages

MAXIMIZING FERTILITY WITH ROOSTER MANAGEMENT. Jeanna L. Wilson University of Georgia

Physical and social environment for sheep

HEALTH, PRODUCTIVITY AND BEHAVIOUR OF CONVENTIONAL TURKEY BREEDS UNDER ECOLOGICAL OUTDOOR REARING CONDITIONS

The welfare of ducks during foie gras production

Daily Animal Health Monitoring Program

Policy #30: Environmental Enrichment at WSU

Dimethyl anthranilate based repellents affect cage pecking and feather condition of laying hens

Approved Cat Enrichment Items

Poultry Housing Tips Monitoring Broiler Distribution Through Water Consumption Volume 14 Number 6 July, 2002

Behaviour of Hens in Cages

Emissions of NH 3 and greenhouse gases from pig houses: Influencing factors and mitigation techniques

HEALTH AND BODY CONDITION OF RABBIT DOES ON COMMERCIAL FARMS

Estelar CHAPTER-6 RAISING AND PRODUCTION OF POULTRY BIRDS

Market Trends influencing the UK egg sector

MANAGING AVIARY SYSTEMS TO ACHIEVE OPTIMAL RESULTS. TOPICS:

STRATEGY FOR DEVELOPING RABBIT MEAT PRODUCTION IN ALGERIA : CREATION AND SELECTION OF A SYNTHETIC STRAIN

The welfare of laying hens

AVIAN HUSBANDRY (POULTRY HATCHING AND CHICKS)

The Benefits of Floor Feeding (for Optimal Uniformity)

ROSS TECH 07/46 Managing the Ross 708 Parent Stock Female

NATURA CAGE-FREE. Modern aviary system for barn and free range egg production

ENVIRONMENT, WELL-BEING, AND BEHAVIOR

Unit 3 Sustainability and interdependence Sub Topic 3.4: Animal welfare

PEOPLE AND FARM ANIMALS

FRENZ. World Leading Poultry Layer Standard

Animal Care & Selection

RSPCA (Victoria) Farm animal welfare The next 5 years

DEVELOPMENT OF THE CAECAL MICROFLORA OF NEWBORN RABBITS DURING THE FIRST TEN DAYS AFTER BIRTH. Guba S. u. 40, Hungary ABSTRACT

Rabbit Adoption Policy

Group housing of sows. SCOFCAH Laurence Bonafos Unit G3

Fact Sheet 6. Breeding Cages

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

Leg and Foot Disorders in Domestic Fowl

Companion Animal Behaviour Referrals Claire Hargrave BSc (Hons), MSc, PGCE, C Sci, C Chem, MRSC, DAS (CABC), CCAB

AUGERMATIC. The feeding system for successful poultry growing

Relationship between hen age, body weight, laying rate, egg weight and rearing system

POULTRY MANAGEMENT IN EAST AFRICA (GUIDELINES FOR REARING CHICKEN)

MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES PARENT STOCK

1 of 9 7/1/10 2:08 PM

Livestock: Transport and Markets Student Activities

AT THE ARRIVAL TO THE SLAUGHTERHOUSE

CONCLUSIONS AND SUGGESTIONS AFTER EEP-VCF STAFF VISIT AT THE POZNAN ZOO

BQA RECERTIFICATION TRAINING Administered by Pennsylvania Beef Quality Assurance

CAT QUESTIONNAIRE. This will help determine the possible cause(s), prognosis and management plan for their behaviour problem(s).

Intermediate Osceola County 4-H Rabbit & Cavy Project Record Book

Comparative Medicine Vol 67, No 5 Copyright 2017 October 2017 by the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science Pages

HUSBANDRY STANDARDS FOR CAPTIVE BEARS IN HO CHI MINH CITY

A cage for the ferret

ANIMAL WELFARE (COMMERCIAL POULTRY) REGULATIONS 2008

Livestock - Definition

NCC Poultry Welfare Guidelines: The reasons behind

The Road to Capacity for Care (C4C): What it truly means to provide the best care & services for all animals (& people!) in your community

Transcription:

Ethology and Welfare FREQUENCY OF ENGAGEMENT WITH DIFFERENT MATERIALS BY GROWING RABBITS Wagner C.*, Weirich C., Hoy St. Department of Animal Breeding and Genetics, Justus Liebig University, Bismarckstraße 16, D-35390 Gießen, Germany *Corresponding author: caroline.wagner@agrar.uni-giessen.de ABSTRACT The frequency of engagement with different materials as environmental enrichment by growing rabbits in cages with plastic slatted floor was studied in two experiments using a total of 108 rabbits (ZIKA hybrids). In each experiment, 54 rabbits were kept in four group-cages with different groupsizes (8, 8, 16 and 22) at the same stocking density (625 cm²/rabbit). The cages were equipped with nipple drinkers and feeders. Feed and water were available ad libitum. The duration of the photoperiod was 16 hours. In the first experiment (D1), pellets were fed ad libitum from a conventional feeder installed outside the cage. In the second experiment (D2), another feeding system which gives the rabbit the possibility to take crude-fiber ad libitum was additionally located in the middle of every cage. Three different occupational tools were installed in the cages: an iron-chain with a wooden gnawing stick (WGS) for the two groups with eight rabbits (A, B), one iron-chain with a plastic gnawing stick (PGS) placed in the group with 16 rabbits (C) and two iron chains connected by a horizontal iron stick (ICS) for the group with 22 rabbits (D). In both experiments, the same kinds of occupational tools were located in the same cages. The frequency of occupation was studied by 24 hours video-observation with infrared cameras at the beginning, the middle and the end of the fattening period (eight weeks). Continuous ethological analyses were performed with the program Observer (Noldus). Additionally, the time sampling method with an interval of five minutes during three periods a day (08:00-10:00; 19:00-21:00; 01:00-05:00) was used to register the number of rabbits in each group showing the following behavioural patterns: eating, drinking, lying, hocking, hopping, own body licking, erecting, engagement with the materials, miscellaneous. In D1, the frequency of occupation was the highest in cages A and B with WGS followed by cage C with PGS and cage D with ICS. In D2, there were no significant differences between the cages, but the frequency of occupation with the different tools in all cages was significantly lower than in D1. Also the results of the time sampling method showed a significant higher frequency of occupation in D1 compared with D2. In the second experiment, the rabbits of all cages were more often observed eating total (hopper with pellets plus feeder for crude-fiber) than in D1 (only pellets from the hopper). Experiment 2 shows that a feeding system which gives the rabbits the possibility to satisfy the demand for crude-fiberintake is a tool in the housing system that allows the rabbits to express a species-specific behaviour. Key words: Growing rabbit, Frequency of occupation, Gnawing sticks, Environmental enrichment, Feeding system. INTRODUCTION Group housing systems are more acceptable than single cage systems as they allow the rabbit to socialize and to show a large variety of behavioural patterns. The negative aspects of group housing cages are the levels of aggression and the high frequency of scratching, sneezing and head shaking (Podberscek et al., 1991). In intensive systems, growing rabbits are usually kept on wire-grid (Dal Bosco et al., 2001; Orova et al., 2004; Morisse et al., 1999; Morisse and Maurice, 1994) but housing in litterless cages represents a very barren environment (Hansen and Berthelsen, 2000). By contrast environmental enrichment in group-cages with growing rabbits has no negative impact on the 1263

9 th World Rabbit Congress June 10-13, 2008 Verona Italy performance and causes fewer stereotypies (Verga et al., 2004). Also, welfare friendly pens with a plastic platform, a hiding box and gnawing material have no negative influence on the performance of growing rabbits (Maertens et al., 2004). The kind of occupational tool and the modality of occupation provide to the rabbit can have different outcomes (Jordan et al., 2006). For example, the type of wood in gnawing sticks can influence the duration of feed intake (Jordan et al., 2006). The wooden stick provided as gnawing material should be hung from the rood of the box, as otherwise there is a risk of it becoming contaminated with faeces (Hoy and Verga, 2006). To reduce abnormal behaviour, hay in a bottle was the most effective occupational tool compared with grass-cubes, a box and different gnawing sticks (Lidfors, 1997). Rabbits, kept in an enriched cage system with shelter and raised height, had better welfare than rabbits kept in a conventional cage system (Hansen and Berthelsen, 2000). The aim of this study was to investigate the frequency of occupation of growing rabbits kept in groups in a conventional cage system with plastic slatted floor and provided with different materials for environmental enrichment. Animals and environmental enrichment MATERIALS AND METHODS The investigation was carried out on the research-station Oberer Hardthof of the University of Gießen, Germany. A total number of 108 rabbits (ZIKA hybrids) were kept in standard group-cages with plastic slatted floor. In each round 54 rabbits were housed in four group-cages with different group sizes (8, 8, 16 and 22) but always using the same stocking density (625 cm²/rabbit) during the growing period, from 35 to 91 days of age. The cages were equipped with nipple drinkers and feeders. Feed and water were available ad libitum; the duration of the photoperiod was 16 hours. In the first round (D1), pellets were fed ad lib by a conventional feeder installed outside the cage. In the second round (D2), another feeding system which gives the rabbit the possibility to take crude-fiber ad libitum was additionally located in the middle of every cage. Three different occupational tools were installed in the cages: an iron-chain with a wooden gnawing stick (WGS) for the two groups with eight rabbits (A, B), one iron-chain with a plastic gnawing stick (PGS) placed in the group with 16 rabbits (C) and two iron chains, connected by a horizontal iron stick. (ICS) for the group with 22 rabbits (D) (Figure 1). In both rounds the same kinds of occupation tools were located in the same cages. WGS in A and B PGS in C ICS in D Behaviour 1264 Figure 1: Different occupation tools installed in different cages The behaviour was video-recorded at the beginning, the middle and the end of the fattening period (24 hours each period), using three cameras connected to a VCR tape recorder. Each cage was observed for 72 hours in total per fattening period. The behaviour of the rabbits in all cages was recorded at the same time. Two methods of observation were used. With the help of THE OBSERVER program (Noldus, NL), the frequency of occupation with the tools was continuously observed during 24 hours. Additionally, the time sampling method with an interval of five minutes during three periods a day

Ethology and Welfare (08:00-10:00; 19:00-21:00; 01:00-05:00) was used to register the number of rabbits in each group showing the following behavioural patterns: eating, drinking, lying, hocking, hopping, own body licking, erecting, engagement with the material, miscellaneous. Because the group size was somewhat different the frequency of behavioural patterns was calculated per rabbit and 24 hours. Statistical analysis Statistical data analysis was conducted using pairwise t-student-test and Student-Newman-Keuls multiple test to compare means between groups and rounds (SPSS 12.0 for Windows). RESULTS AND DISCUSSION In D1, the round without the crude-fiber-feeding-system, there was a significant difference between the cages concerning the frequency of occupation with the different materials. The frequency of occupation was the highest in cages A and B with the wooden gnawing sticks WGS (62.43%; 66.33% respectively on average per rabbit and 24 hours) followed by cage C with PGS (34.32%) and cage D with ICS (24.15%) (Figure 2). Differences between the averages of the groups A, B and C, D = P<0.05 Figure 2: Frequency of occupation with different tools (WGS, PGS or ICS) per rabbit in 24 hours, D1 In D2, there were no significant differences among the cages or groups concerning the frequency of use of the different tools. Yet, these frequencies in all cages were significantly lower in D2 than in D1 (Figure 3) perhaps because the rabbits had the possibility to spend more time with the occupation with crude-fiber from the new feeder. Also, the results of the time sampling method showed a significantly lower frequency of occupation with the different materials in D2 compared with D1. 1265

9 th World Rabbit Congress June 10-13, 2008 Verona Italy Differences between average of D1 and average of D2 = P<0.05 No significant differences between the groups A, B, C and D in D2 Figure 3: Frequency of occupation with different materials (average per round/rabbit in 24 hours) in D1 and D2 Furthermore, the results of the time sampling method showed that there was a significant difference between rounds D1 and D2 concerning the frequency of eating. In the second round the rabbits in all cages were more often found sitting at the new feeder (for crude-fiber) and at the hopper (for pellets) (= total eating ) than in D1 where they had only the possibility to eat pellets from the hopper. In both rounds, there was no difference among the four cages regarding the frequency of eating in 24 hours (Figure 4). All differences between D1 and D2 = P<0.05 Figure 4: Frequency of eating (as percent per rabbit in 24 hours) in D1 and D2 No significant differences between the groups A, B, C and D in D1 and D2 CONCLUSIONS The wooden gnawing stick was used most frequently but the effect of the tool cannot be separated from the effect of group size. 1266

Ethology and Welfare The frequency of engagement with different materials is much lower (P<0.05) if the rabbits have the possibility to eat crude-fiber ad libitum from a new feeding system. The frequency of total eating (pellets plus crude-fiber) is significantly higher if the rabbits have access to pellets in a traditional feeder and crude-fiber in a new feeding system (round D2) compared to only pellets from a hopper (round D1). A feeding system which gives the rabbits the possibility to satisfy the demand of crude-fiber intake is at the same time a tool in the housing system that allows the rabbits to express a species-specific behaviour. REFERENCES Dal Bosco A., Castellini C., Mugnai C. 2001. Rearing rabbits in a wire net floor or straw litter: behaviour, growth and meat qualitative traits. Livestock Production Science, 75, 2, 149-156. Hansen L.T., Berthelsen H. 2000. The effect of environmental enrichment on the behaviour of caged rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculuc). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 68, 163-178. Hoy St., Verga M. 2006. Welfare indicators. Recent Advances in Rabbit Sciences, 71-74. Jordan D., Gorjanc G., Kermauner A., Stuhec I. 2006. Growing rabbits behaviour in individual wire cages enriched with gnawing sticks of types of wood. KTBL-Schrift, 448. Jordan D., Luzi F., Verga M., Stuhec I. 2006. Environmental enrichment in growing rabbits. In: Maertens L., Coudert P. (Eds.), Recent Advances in Rabbit Sciences, ILVO, Melle, Belgium, 113-119. Lidfors L. 1997. Behavioural effects of environmental enrichment for individually caged rabbits. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 52, 157-169. Orova Z., Szendrı Zs., Matics Zs., Radnai I., Biro-Nemeth E. 2004. Free choice of growing rabbits between deep litter and wire net floor in pens. In: Proc. 8 th World Rabbit Congress, 2004 September, Puebla, Mexico, 1263-1265. Podberscek A.L., Blackshaw J.K., Beattie A.W. 1991. The behaviour of group penned and individually caged laboratory rabbits. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 28, 353-363. Maertens L., Tuyttens F., Poucke van E. 2004. Group housing of broiler rabbits : performances in enriched vs barren pens. In: Proc. 8 th World Rabbit Congress, 2004 September, Puebla, Mexico, 1247-1250. Morisse J.P., Maurice R. 1994. Welfare and the intensive production of rabbits. Rev. Sci. Tech., 13 (1), 131-152. Morisse J.P., Boilletot E., Martrenchar A. 1999. Preference testing in intensively kept meat production rabbits for straw or wire grid floor. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 64, 71-80. Verga M., Zingarelli I., Heinzl E., Ferrante V., Martino P.A., Luzi F. 2004. Effect of housing and environmental enrichment on performance and behaviour in fattening rabbits. In: Proc. 8 th World Rabbit Congress, 2004 September, Puebla, Mexico, 2005. 1267

9 th World Rabbit Congress June 10-13, 2008 Verona Italy 1268