The medial bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BSTm) is

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The medial bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BSTm) is"

Transcription

1 Valence-sensitive neurons exhibit divergent functional profiles in gregarious and asocial species James L. Goodson* and Yiwei Wang Department of Psychology, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, CA Edited by Bruce S. McEwen, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY, and approved September 21, 2006 (received for review July 24, 2006) The medial bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BSTm) influences both social approach and social aversion, suggesting that this structure may play an important role in generating motivational and behavioral differences between gregarious and asocial species. However, no specific neurons have been identified within the BSTm that influence species-typical levels of sociality or that mediate approach and avoidance. Using five songbird species that differ selectively in their species-typical group sizes, we now demonstrate that vasotocin-immunoreactive (VT-ir) neurons of the BSTm exhibit very different immediate early gene responses to same-sex stimuli in gregarious and asocial species. Exposure to a same-sex conspecific increases VT-Fos colocalization in gregarious species while decreasing colocalization in relatively asocial species. We additionally demonstrate that these neurons are selectively activated by social stimuli that normally elicit affiliation (positively valenced social stimuli) but not by stimuli that elicit aversion (negatively valenced social stimuli). Constitutive Fos activity of the VT-ir neurons is also significantly greater in the gregarious species, and the two most social species express significantly more VT-ir neurons. These findings demonstrate that the properties of valence-sensitive neurons evolve in relation to sociality and indicate that gregarious species accentuate positive stimulus properties during social interactions. amygdala sociality vasopressin vasotocin The medial bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BSTm) is anatomically and functionally conserved in the land vertebrate classes (1 3), and in virtually all species, a subpopulation of BSTm neurons produce vasotocin (VT) or its mammalian homologue vasopressin (VP) (4, 5) (see Fig. 1). These neurons project to multiple areas of the basal forebrain [e.g., ventral pallidum and lateral septum (5, 6)], where VT VP influences a variety of social behaviors, sometimes in a potent and highly species-specific manner (4, 7, 8). For instance, differential expression of V 1a receptors in the ventral pallidum is both necessary and sufficient to account for species differences in monogamous pair-bonding (9), and V 1a receptors in the lateral septum are both necessary and sufficient for the display of social recognition in mice (10). However, despite the vast literature on VT VP functions, receptor distributions, sexual dimorphisms, and steroid sensitivities (4, 5, 7, 8), almost nothing is known about BSTm VT VP neuronal activity as it relates to stimulus parameters, social context, and species-specific aspects of social structure. Indeed, to our knowledge, the only relevant data come from monogamous male voles, in which VP mrna increases in the BSTm after cohabitation with a female (11). The present experiments were therefore conducted to test the hypotheses that (i) VT-immunoreactive (VT-ir) neurons in the BSTm are responsive to stimuli that elicit affiliation, and (ii) responses of these neurons evolve in relation to sociality. These hypotheses were tested by immunofluorescently double-labeling for VT and the immediate early gene protein Fos (an inducible marker of neural activity) after a variety of social interactions in males and females of five estrildid finch species that differ selectively in their species-typical group sizes (12, 13). These include two species that independently evolved a relatively asocial, territorial social structure (the melba finch, Pytilia melba, and the violet-eared waxbill, Uraeginthus granatina); two species that independently evolved a highly colonial social structure (the spice finch, Lonchura punctulata, and the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata); and an intermediately gregarious species (the Angolan blue waxbill, Uraeginthus angolensis, which is a congener of the territorial violet-eared waxbill). The mean group size of the territorial species is two (i.e., male female pair). Group sizes for the Angolan blue waxbill range from 8 40 and are 100 for the two highly gregarious, colonial species. These species are closely matched in other aspects of behavior and ecology: All are monogamous (typically pair-bonding for life), exhibit biparental care, breed semiopportunistically dependent on rainfall, and inhabit semiarid and or grassland scrub habitat. The species differences in sociality are stable across seasons and contexts (for evolutionary considerations, see ref. 13; also see refs ). In the present experiments, subjects were separated from stimulus animals by a wire barrier (this was modified in the final experiment), and tests were conducted outside of the normal housing environment. This paradigm successfully limits the performance of overt social behaviors, even in territorial subjects (12). Thus, Fos induction that is observed in this paradigm should primarily reflect motivational and or perceptual processes, not motoric responses to the stimulus. Results In our first experiment, males and females of the highly gregarious zebra finch were exposed to a same-sex conspecific through the wire barrier or to a control condition (wire barrier only). This simple manipulation produced a large increase in the percentage of VT-ir neurons that coexpressed Fos-ir nuclei [Fig. 2; two-way ANOVA, sex condition: F(6,1,1) ; P 0.002]. No sex differences were observed [F(6,1,1) 1.701; P 0.234]. We subsequently repeated this experiment in the other four other species. Although no female melba finches could be committed to this study (collections in Africa and subsequent breeding have yielded few females), two-way ANOVAs (condition sex) revealed no main effect of sex for the violet-eared waxbill [F(6,1,1) 0.317; P 0.594], Angolan blue waxbill [F(10,1,1) 0.292; P 0.601], or spice finch [F(10,1,1) 0.070; P 0.797]. A three-way ANOVA including all three of these species likewise showed a clear lack of sex differences [F(25,1,1,2) 0.795; P 0.381]. We therefore pooled data from males and females for a two-way ANOVA (species condition) that included the melba finch (Fig. 3A). These analyses revealed a significant main effect of species [F(36,1,3) 4.356; P 0.010] and a significant Author contributions: J.L.G. designed research; J.L.G. and Y.W. performed research; J.L.G. and Y.W. analyzed data; and J.L.G. wrote the paper. The authors declare no conflict of interest. This article is a PNAS direct submission. Abbreviations: BSTm, medial bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; VP, vasopressin; VT, vasotocin; VT-ir, VT-immunoreactive. *To whom correspondence should be addressed. jgoodson@ucsd.edu by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA cgi doi pnas PNAS November 7, 2006 vol. 103 no

2 Fig. 2. Responses of VT neurons to a same-sex stimulus in the highly gregarious zebra finch. Percentage of VT-ir neurons in the BSTm that express Fos-ir nuclei (means SEM) after exposure to a same-sex conspecific or a control condition (males, solid bars; females, open bars). Two-way ANOVA (sex condition) shows a main effect of condition (P 0.002). Total n 10. Fig. 1. Photomicrographs of VT-ir neurons in the BSTm of a male zebra finch at the level of the anterior commissure (A) and immediately caudal to the anterior commissure (B) (color inverted fluorescence). Asterisks indicate the location of Insets showing neurons double-labeled for VT (green) and Fos (red). (Scale bars: A, 100 m; B, 200 m; Insets, 50 m.) AC, anterior commissure; BSTl, lateral bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; LS, lateral septum; MS, medial septum; PVN, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus; v, ventricle. species condition interaction [F(36,1,3) 3.445; P 0.026], both of which appear to reflect differences between the two territorial species versus the two gregarious species. To directly test this idea, we combined subjects of the two territorial species and subjects of the two gregarious species for an additional two-way ANOVA (sociality condition; Fig. 3B). Consistent with our hypothesis, the territorial subjects exhibited significantly lower levels of VT-Fos colocalization than did the gregarious subjects [main effect of sociality: F(40,1,1) 8.555; P 0.005], and a significant interaction was observed for sociality and condition [F(40,1,1) 6.504; P 0.014]. This interaction reflects the fact that VT-Fos colocalization tended to decrease in the territorial subjects after exposure to a same-sex conspecific, while increasing in the gregarious subjects. Given these species differences in Fos response, a main effect of condition was not observed [F(36,1,3) 0.998; P 0.325]. Notably, the gregarious Angolan blue waxbill exhibits a pattern of colocalization that is similar to the colonial spice finch and strongly dissimilar to its closely related congener, the territorial violet-eared waxbill. A separate analysis of the two waxbill congeners confirms the divergence between them, yielding a significant species condition interaction [F(20,1,1) 6.620; P 0.018]. Overall, the constitutive levels of Fos expression were much higher in this second experiment than in the previous experiment, which may have produced a ceiling effect in the two gregarious species. This difference likely reflects the fact that the subjects used in this second experiment were taken from breeding cages (2 days before testing), whereas zebra finches in the first experiment had been in same-sex housing. Data presented below from breeding-condition zebra finches are consistent with this hypothesis, because these birds likewise show high constitutive coexpression of VT and Fos. Based on the finding that VT-Fos colocalization in the BSTm increases in response to same-sex stimuli in gregarious species but not in relatively asocial species, we hypothesized that these neurons may respond selectively to social stimuli that promote affiliation (positively valenced social stimuli) but not to stimuli that produce aversion or avoidance (negatively valenced social stimuli). To provide additional support for this hypothesis, we Fig. 3. Responses of VT-ir neurons to a conspecific stimulus differ between asocial and gregarious species. (A) Percentage of VT-ir neurons in the BSTm that express Fos-ir nuclei (means SEM) after exposure to a same-sex conspecific (filled bars) or a control condition (open bars) in two relatively asocial, territorial species [melba finch (MF) and violet-eared waxbill (VEW)], a moderately gregarious species [Angolan blue waxbill (ABW)], and a highly gregarious, colonial species [spice finch (SF)]. No sex differences were observed, and sexes are shown pooled. Two-way ANOVA (species condition) yields a main effect of species (P 0.01) and a species condition interaction (P 0.02). Different letters above the bars indicate significant species differences (P 0.05, Fisher s protected least squares difference). Two-way ANOVA (sociality condition) after pooling of the relatively asocial subjects (MF and VEW) and the gregarious subjects (ABW and SF) shows a main effect of sociality (P 0.005) and a sociality condition interaction (P 0.01). Total n 44. (B) Representative double-labeling for Fos (red) and VT (green) in an experimental male spice finch. DAPI nuclear stain is shown in blue. (Scale bar: 50 m.) cgi doi pnas Goodson and Wang

3 Fig. 5. Highly colonial species express more VT-ir neurons than do less gregarious species. Numbers of VT-ir neurons per 40- m section (means SEM) in the BSTm of two relatively asocial, territorial species [melba finch (MF) and violet-eared waxbill (VEW)], a moderately gregarious species [Angolan blue waxbill (ABW)], and two highly gregarious, colonial species [spice finch (SF) and zebra finch (ZF)]. No sex differences were observed, and sexes are shown pooled. Different letters above the bars indicate significant species differences (P 0.05, Fisher s protected least squares difference) after significant ANOVA (P ). Total n 96. Fig. 4. Fos expression in VT-ir neurons increases in response to positively valenced social stimuli but not negatively valenced stimuli. (A) Percentage of VT-ir neurons in the BSTm that express Fos-ir nuclei (means SEM) in the asocial violet-eared waxbill after exposure to a control condition (open bars), a same-sex conspecific (black bars), or the subject s pairbond partner (gray bars). Different letters above the bars indicate significant group differences (Mann Whitney tied P 0.05, after significant Kruskal Wallis tied P 0.003). Total n 16. (B) VT-Fos colocalization in control (open bars), subjugated (black bars), and nonsubjugated (gray bars) zebra finches exposed to mate competition (Kruskal Wallis tied P 0.03). No sex differences were observed, and sexes are shown pooled. Total n 15. conducted two experiments in which subjects were exposed to positively valenced and negatively valenced social stimuli. The first of these experiments was conducted in the relatively asocial violet-eared waxbill. Violet-eared waxbills do not affiliate with same-sex individuals but maintain close contact with their pairbond partners. Thus, we exposed male and female waxbills to the control condition (wire barrier), a same-sex conspecific (negative valence), or their pairbond partner (positive valence). The species differences obtained above were anticipated based on pilot experiments; thus, subjects in the third group (pairbond partner) were run alongside the control and same-sex subjects from the previous experiment. As shown in Fig. 4A, the results provide strong support for our hypothesis. Relative to control subjects, subjects exposed to the same-sex stimulus showed a significant decrease in VT-Fos colocalization, whereas subjects exposed to their pairbond partners exhibited a very robust increase (Mann Whitney tied P 0.05 after significant Kruskal Wallis tied P 0.003). No sex differences were observed [sex condition ANOVA, main effect of sex: F(10,2,1) 0.031; P 0.969]. In a concurrent experiment, we exposed male and female zebra finches to a control condition or a mate-competition paradigm in which the subject and a same-sex conspecific competed for courtship access to an opposite sex bird. We constructed this paradigm such that subjects were intensely subjugated (negative social valence) or were dominant and allowed to court (positive social valence). As expected, the nonsubjugated subjects showed a significant increase in VT-Fos colocalization, whereas the subjugated birds did not (Mann Whitney tied P 0.05 after significant Kruskal Wallis tied P 0.03; Fig. 4B). This experiment additionally demonstrates that social arousal alone does not induce Fos activity in the VT-ir neurons, because the subjugated animals failed to exhibit an increase in VT-Fos colocalization, despite being aggressively displaced times during the 10-min test. Two other observations further strengthen the association between these VT-ir neurons and positive affiliation. First, constitutive VT-Fos colocalization is significantly higher in gregarious versus territorial subjects, as quantified in breedingcondition control subjects of all five species (Figs. 3 and 4; % versus %, respectively; t 2.268, P 0.031, unpaired t test). A similar difference is observed when comparing the number of VT-ir neurons in gregarious and territorial subjects (gregarious territorial; t 3.751, P , unpaired t test), but in this case the moderately gregarious Angolan blue waxbill is most similar to the two territorial species. Thus, the two highly colonial species (spice finch and zebra finch) exhibit significantly more VT-ir neurons than do the other species [one-way ANOVA on species: F(91,4) ; P ; Fig. 5). These species differences in VT-ir cell numbers cannot be attributed to variation in immunocytochemical runs because brains from all species and conditions were run in parallel. Discussion Valence Sensitivity of BSTm VT Neurons. The combined data from the present experiments demonstrate that Fos expression in BSTm VT-ir neurons is increased after exposure to positively valenced social stimuli (stimuli that normally elicit affiliative behaviors) but not negatively valenced social stimuli (stimuli that elicit aversion or territorial aggression). Thus, in both gregarious and relatively asocial species, significant increases in VT-Fos colocalization are observed after exposure to an opposite-sex stimulus such as a potential mate or the subject s pairbond partner. Increases in VT-Fos colocalization are also observed after exposure to a same-sex conspecific in the gregarious species, which flock year-round, but decreases are observed in the relatively asocial species, which are territorial and do not associate with same-sex conspecifics. These species differences in VT neuronal response cannot be attributed to species differences in social arousal, because intensely arousing subjugation ( aggressive displacements in a 10-min period; Fig. 4B) failed to influence VT-Fos colocalization. We additionally found that constitutive VT-Fos colocalization is higher in the three gregarious species than in the relatively asocial species, and the two colonial species exhibit more VT-ir neurons in the BSTm. These findings suggest that gregarious birds are tonically primed to respond to social stimuli in a positive, affiliative manner. Finally, sensitivity to positive social valence is not a property Goodson and Wang PNAS November 7, 2006 vol. 103 no

4 of the BSTm as a whole; in fact, BSTm Fos responses to same-sex stimuli are significantly greater in territorial birds than in gregarious birds (12). VT neurons therefore comprise a functionally distinct population within the BSTm. Whether or not these neurons are sensitive to nonsocial stimuli remains to be determined, although available evidence suggests that the avian BSTm responds preferentially to social stimuli (17). Valence Sensitivity in the BSTm and Amygdala: A General Organizational Scheme? The BSTm and medial amygdala are strongly interconnected and share many functional properties (1, 2). It is therefore noteworthy that various regions of the amygdala contain valence-sensitive neurons in mammals. Activity in the human amygdala is associated with stimulus valence (18) and the encoding of positive and negative items (19). Neurophysiological studies in monkeys further demonstrate that positive and negative sensitivity is exhibited by separate, but intercalated, neuronal populations (20, 21), and recent studies in rats and mice have also identified spatially segregated neuronal populations within the central nucleus that are responsive to appetitive or aversive stimuli (22). The present experiments now identify the neurochemical phenotype of valence-sensitive neurons that are activated during social interactions, which should greatly facilitate the analysis of neural circuits that are involved in social approach and avoidance. The importance of the medial amygdala and BSTm in social approach and avoidance is well established (1, 23), and intercalated populations of medial amygdala neurons exhibit Fos responses to reproductive and predator odors (24). Further analyses may show that these intercalated neurons are more broadly sensitive to positive and negative valence. Importantly, although we have thus far identified only BSTm neurons that are sensitive to positive social valence, it seems likely that an intercalated population of BSTm neurons is also sensitive to negative social valence. This hypothesis is suggested by the fact that same-sex stimuli induce greater BSTm Fos responses in the territorial violet-eared waxbill than in three gregarious species (12), a pattern reversed from that of the VT-ir neurons. VT VP Functions and the Evolution of Sociality. The Angolan blue waxbill and violet-eared waxbill are congeners that have evolved divergently from their common ancestor, becoming more social and less social, respectively (13). These species also diverge significantly in their patterns of Fos induction after exposure to a same-sex conspecific. Furthermore, in diverging from each other, their patterns of VT-Fos colocalization converge with other species that have independently evolved either highly social or highly asocial behavior (see ref. 13). Our finding that BSTm neurons are sensitive to positive social valence in finches provides a strong interpretational framework for other recent data on these same five species, which demonstrate that linear 125 I-VP V 1a antagonist binding in the lateral septum is positively related to sociality (13). Septal VT [presumably of BSTm origin (5, 6)] also influences aggression in these species, inhibiting territorial resident intruder aggression while facilitating aggression that is specific to the context of appetitive courtship behavior (2). Both of these effects are consistent with an increase in affiliative motivation [note that distinctly different influences are exerted by anterior hypothalamic VP neurons, which are known to promote offensive aggression in rodents (25)]. Septal V 1a receptors also mediate social recognition (10, 26, 27) and promote a variety of affiliative behaviors in rodents, including active social interaction in rats (26) and pairbonding in monogamous male voles (28). Somewhat counterintuitively, the various influences VT VP on affiliation are coupled with influences on anxiety and stress processes (10, 17, 29), although the functional significance of this connection remains to be elucidated. Influence of Sex and Reproductive Condition. The number of VT VP neurons in the BSTm is sexually dimorphic (males females) and steroid-dependent in most vertebrate species thus far examined (4, 5, 30), including songbirds (31, 32). In contrast, zebra finches have not been consistently found to exhibit sexually dimorphic or testosterone-dependent immunostaining (33, 34), and we here found no evidence for sex differences in any of the estrildid species examined, either in their numbers of VT-ir neurons or Fos responses to social stimuli. We did however observe a substantial difference in constitutive VT-Fos colocalization between zebra finches that had been housed in same-sex cages ( 12% of VT-ir neurons expressed Fos in control subjects) and those that had been housed in mixed-sex breeding cages until 2 days before testing ( 60%; Figs. 1 and 4B, respectively). Although there were methodological differences in those two experiments, socially induced levels of VT-Fos colocalization were high in both, suggesting that the constitutive differences are not methodological artifacts but rather reflect a strong social and or hormonal influence on constitutive Fos activity. Conclusions. The present findings substantially expand on a variety of findings in humans and other animals, which demonstrate that the amygdala contains neuronal populations that are sensitive to stimulus valence (18, 20, 21). To our knowledge, the present findings provide the first evidence that valence sensitivity is exhibited during social interactions and also represent the first case in which the neurochemical phenotype of the relevant neurons has been identified. Neurochemical identification of valence-sensitive neurons is key, because it essential for systemslevel analysis of valence-related behavioral regulation (e.g., approach avoidance gating). Finally, the general relevance of the present findings should be broad, because the VT VP system arising in the BSTm is conserved across the tetrapod classes (2, 4, 5). Even in cases of independent social evolution, the predictive validity of our results should be strong, given that we have elucidated functional properties of VT neurons that show reliable correspondence to behavior during both evolutionary divergence and convergence in species-typical group size. Methods Animals. Melba finches, violet-eared waxbills, and Angolan blue waxbills were wild-caught by mist net in South Africa and maintained in captivity for 3 years or were captive-bred from wild stock. Wild-caught spice finches were provided by a commercial supplier. Zebra finches were of domestic stock [note that extensive ethological examinations have failed to identify any behavioral differences between wild-caught and domestic zebra finches (35)]. Collections and procedures were conducted legally and humanely under applicable federal and state permits and in compliance with institutional and federal guidelines. Behavioral Testing. The protocol for exposure to conspecifics through a wire barrier has been fully described (12) and was used for experiments presented in Figs. 2, 3, and 4A. Two days before testing, subjects were removed from housing cages and placed into testing cages for acclimation. These cages were then placed into a quiet room the day before testing. In the experiment presented in Fig. 4B, we sought to create a negative social stimulus for the zebra finch by experimentally inducing social subjugation. However, intense subjugation is difficult to experimentally manipulate in this species, given their gregarious nature. Aggression is most reliably elicited by using a mate competition paradigm (36): After several days in same-sex housing, two same-sex individuals are exposed to an opposite sex individual (separated from the other two by a wire barrier). This induces aggressive competition, although it is typically mild. To increase the level of aggression, we used well known bullies from our colony as dominant partners. These were one female cgi doi pnas Goodson and Wang

5 and one male that each had a history of excessive aggression in their housing cages. Two daily conditioning trials (10 min) were conducted before final testing and perfusion. To be retained for the experiment, we required that (i) aggressive subjugation be initiated within 1 min of the start of the trial, thereby minimizing the potential for positive interactions with the opposite-sex stimulus; (ii) a minimum of 40 displacements be exhibited in each of the conditioning trials; and (iii) the number of displacements on test day be equal to, or greater than, the average of the conditioning trials. Final tests were 10 min. Immunocytochemistry and Quantification of Labeling. Tissue was cut into three series of 40- m free-floating sections and one series was immunofluorescently double-labeled for Fos protein and VT by using standard methods (12, 17, 37). For the experiment presented in Fig. 2, we used a custom mouse anti-fos antibody (1:100) (12). A rabbit anti-fos antibody (1:1,000) was used for all other experiments (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, CA). Both antibodies are directed against portions of the chicken Fos sequence. Anti-VP raised in guinea pig (1:1,000; Bachem, Torrance, CA) was used for all experiments. The VP antibody was labeled with anti-guinea pig biotin (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) and visualized with streptavidin conjugated to Alexa Fluor 488 (Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR). The Fos antibodies were visualized with secondary antibodies conjugated to Alexa Fluor 594. Double-labeling was quantified bilaterally in sections containing VT-ir neurons in the commissural BSTm (dorsal to the anterior commissure) and postcommissural BSTm, where a band of VT-ir neurons drops ventrally behind the caudal margin of the anterior commissure (Fig. 1). Subjects were excluded from analyses of VT-Fos colocalization if fewer than five VT-ir neurons were observed. Slides were coded for observer-blind analysis. All counts were conducted on a Zeiss Axioscop with a 62 objective and 10 eyepieces. Statistics. Data were analyzed with StatView 5.0 software for the Macintosh, using ANOVA followed by Fisher s protected least squares difference if variances could be normalized (determined by F tests). Data shown in Figs. 2, 3, and 5 were analyzed after log normal transformations to normalize variances. Data shown in Fig. 4 could not be normalized and were analyzed by using Kruskal Wallis tests followed by Mann Whitney U tests. All tests were 1. Newman SW (1999) Ann NY Acad Sci 877: Goodson JL (2005) Horm Behav 48: Yamamoto K, Sun Z, Wang HB, Reiner A (2005) Brain Res Bull 66: Goodson JL, Bass AH (2001) Brain Res Rev 35: De Vries GJ, Panzica GC (2006) Neuroscience 138: De Vries GJ, Buijs RM (1983) Brain Res 273: Keverne EB, Curley JP (2004) Curr Opin Neurobiol 14: Young LJ, Wang Z (2004) Nat Neurosci 7: Lim MM, Wang Z, Olazabal DE, Ren X, Terwilliger EF, Young LJ (2004) Nature 429: Bielsky IF, Hu SB, Ren X, Terwilliger EF, Young LJ (2005) Neuron 47: Wang Z, Smith W, Major DE, De Vries GJ (1994) Brain Res 650: Goodson JL, Evans AK, Lindberg L, Allen CD (2005) Proc R Soc London Ser B 272: Goodson JL, Evans AK, Wang Y (2006) Horm Behav 50: Skead DM (1975) Ostrich Suppl 11: Goodwin D (1982) Estrildid Finches of the World (Cornell Univ Press, Ithaca, NY). 16. Zann RA (1996) The Zebra Finch: A Synthesis of Field and Laboratory Studies (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford). 17. Goodson JL, Evans AK (2004) Horm Behav 46: Anders S, Lotze M, Erb M, Grodd W, Birbaumer N (2004) Hum Brain Mapp 23: Kensinger EA, Schacter DL (2006) J Neurosci 26: two-tailed with an alpha of Sex could not be included as a factor in the analyses presented in Fig. 3 and Fig. 5 because no female melba finches were available for this study. We therefore conducted ANOVAs with melba finches removed and further analyzed each species separately; these analyses showed no trends toward sex differences (see Results). Although the data shown in Fig. 3A were analyzed by Kruskal Wallis, two-way ANOVA (sex condition) shows a clear lack of sex differences [F(10,2,1) 0.031, P 0.969]. Due to a small n in Fig. 3B, sex differences were not tested. The two colonial species exhibited large numbers of VT-ir neurons, and none of the 13 spice finches or 25 zebra finches were excluded from analyses of VT-Fos colocalization. In contrast, the other three species exhibited relatively few VT-ir neurons, requiring that 1 of 8 melba finches, 16 of 32 violet-eared waxbills, and 14 of 28 Angolan blue waxbills be excluded (fewer than five VT-ir neurons observed). Final subject n values were as follows. In Fig. 2, zebra finches: two control males, three control females, three experimental males, and two experimental females. In Fig. 3, melba finches: three control males and four experimental males; violet-eared waxbills: three control males, two control females, three experimental males, and two experimental females; Angolan blue waxbills: five control males, three control females, four experimental males, and two experimental females; spice finches: four control males, two control females, five experimental males, and two experimental females. In Fig. 4A, violet-eared waxbills: three control males, two control females, three males with same-sex stimulus, two females with same-sex stimulus, three males with pairbond partner, and three experimental females with pairbond partner. In Fig. 4B, zebra finches: three control males, three control females, two subjugated males, one subjugated females, three nonsubjugated males, and three nonsubjugated females. In Fig. 5, eight male melba finches, 15 male and 17 female violet-eared waxbills, 14 male and 14 female Angolan blue waxbills, nine male and four female spice finches, and six male and nine female zebra finches. Note that Fig. 5 includes data from all subjects, including those that were excluded from analyses of VT-Fos colocalization. We thank Marcy Kingsbury, Ben Mines, and Renier van Heerden for assistance with bird collections in Africa. This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health. 20. Nishijo H, Ono T, Nishino H (1988) J Neurosci 8: Paton JJ, Belova MA, Morrison SE, Salzman CD (2006) Nature 439: Knapska E, Walasek G, Nikolaev E, Neuhausser-Wespy F, Lipp HP, Kaczmarek L, Werka T (2006) Learn Mem 13: Sheehan TP, Paul M, Amaral E, Numan MJ, Numan M (2001) Neuroscience 106: Choi GB, Dong HW, Murphy AJ, Valenzuela DM, Yancopoulos GD, Swanson LW, Anderson DJ (2005) Neuron 46: Delville Y, De Vries GJ, Ferris CF (2000) Brain Behav Evol 55: Landgraf R, Gerstberger R, Montkowski A, Probst JC, Wotjak CT, Holsboer F, Engelmann M (1995) J Neurosci 15: Landgraf R, Frank E, Aldag JM, Neumann ID, Sharer CA, Ren X, Terwilliger EF, Niwa M, Wigger A, Young LJ (2003) Eur J Neurosci 18: Liu Y, Curtis JT, Wang Z (2001) Behav Neurosci 115: Pitkow LJ, Sharer CA, Ren X, Insel TR, Terwilliger EF, Young LJ (2001) J Neurosci 21: Panzica GC, Aste N, Castagna C, Viglietti-Panzica C, Balthazart J (2001) Brain Res Rev 37: Voorhuis TAM, Kiss JZ, De Kloet ER, De Wied D (1988) Brain Res 442: Plumari L, Plateroti S, Deviche P, Panzica GC (2004) Brain Res 999: Voorhuis TAM, De Kloet ER (1992) Develop Brain Res 69: Kimura T, Okanoya K, Wada M (1999) Life Sci 65: Morris D (1958) Proc Zool Sci London 131: Adkins-Regan E, Robinson TM (1993) J Comp Psychol 107: Goodson JL, Evans AK, Lindberg L (2004) J Comp Neurol 473: Goodson and Wang PNAS November 7, 2006 vol. 103 no

Abstract. J. L. Goodson and E. Adkins-Regan Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA.

Abstract. J. L. Goodson and E. Adkins-Regan Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 1999, Vol. 11, 19 25 Effect of Intraseptal Vasotocin and Vasoactive Intestinal Polypeptide Infusions on Courtship Song and Aggression in the Male Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia

More information

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia. PRESENTED BY KEN Yasukawa at the 2007 ABS Annual Meeting Education Workshop Burlington VT ANIMAL BEHAVIOR Humans have always been interested in animals and how they behave because animals are a source

More information

Motor-driven gene expression

Motor-driven gene expression Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 94, pp. 4097 4102, April 1997 Neurobiology Motor-driven gene expression (birdsong vocalizations perception immediate early genes ZENK) ERICH D. JARVIS AND FERNANDO NOTTEBOHM

More information

Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males*

Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males* Anita. Behav., 1991,42, 83-89 Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males* KLAUS IMMELMANN, RAGNA PROVE, REINHARD LASSEK & HANS-JOACHIM BISCHOFt

More information

Zuoxin Wang University of Massachusetts

Zuoxin Wang University of Massachusetts Behavioral Neuroscience 1995, Vol. 109, No. 2, 305-311 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/S3.00 Species Differences in the Vasopressin-Immunoreactive Pathways in

More information

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc 1. The money in the kingdom of Florin consists of bills with the value written on the front, and pictures of members of the royal family on the back. To test the hypothesis that all of the Florinese $5

More information

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Edinburgh Research Explorer Edinburgh Research Explorer A role for nonapeptides and dopamine in nest-building behaviour Citation for published version: Hall, ZJ, Healy, SD & Meddle, SL 2015, 'A role for nonapeptides and dopamine

More information

Publication list Peer-reviewed papers

Publication list Peer-reviewed papers Publication list Peer-reviewed papers 1.# Scheffrahn,#W.,#Lipp,#H.2P.,#and#Mahler,#M.#(1975).#Serumproteine#und#Erythrozytenenzyme#bei#Callithrix)jacchus# (Platyrrhina).#Archiv#für#Genetik#47,#962104.#

More information

Behavioral Phenotyping of Naked Mole Rat (Heterocephalus glaber)

Behavioral Phenotyping of Naked Mole Rat (Heterocephalus glaber) Behavioral Bioassay IBRO Neuroscience School 2014 Behavioral Phenotyping of Naked Mole Rat (Heterocephalus glaber) Nilesh B. Patel Dept Medical Physiology University of Nairobi, Kenya Richard Alexander

More information

RESEARCH AND PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE

RESEARCH AND PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE Yu Ping Tang Department of Psychology 293 Farm Lane 108 Giltner Hall Michigan State University East Lansing, Michigan 48824 Tel: 517-4325113 Fax: 517-4322744 E-mail: tangyupi@msu.edu RESIDENCE 3931 Trailwood

More information

Sexual imprinting on a novel blue ornament in zebra finches

Sexual imprinting on a novel blue ornament in zebra finches Sexual imprinting on a novel blue ornament in zebra finches Klaudia Witte ) & Barbara Caspers (Lehrstuhl für Verhaltensforschung, Universität Bielefeld, Postfach 100131, 33501 Bielefeld, Germany) (Accepted:

More information

Biol 160: Lab 7. Modeling Evolution

Biol 160: Lab 7. Modeling Evolution Name: Modeling Evolution OBJECTIVES Help you develop an understanding of important factors that affect evolution of a species. Demonstrate important biological and environmental selection factors that

More information

Sexual dimorphism in song-induced ZENK expression in the medial striatum of juvenile zebra finches

Sexual dimorphism in song-induced ZENK expression in the medial striatum of juvenile zebra finches Neuroscience Letters 401 (2006) 86 91 Sexual dimorphism in song-induced ZENK expression in the medial striatum of juvenile zebra finches David J. Bailey a,, Juli Wade a,b,c a Department of Psychology,

More information

Food preference and copying behaviour in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata

Food preference and copying behaviour in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata 1 Food preference and copying behaviour in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata 2 3 4 5 6 7 Lauren M. Guillette*, Kate V. Morgan, Zachary J. Hall, Ida E. Bailey and Susan D. Healy School of Biology, University

More information

Edited by Donald W. Pfaff, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY, and approved March 9, 2007 (received for review November 21, 2006)

Edited by Donald W. Pfaff, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY, and approved March 9, 2007 (received for review November 21, 2006) Social control of brain morphology in a eusocial mammal Melissa M. Holmes*, Greta J. Rosen*, Cynthia L. Jordan, Geert J. de Vries*, Bruce D. Goldman, and Nancy G. Forger* *Center for Neuroendocrine Studies

More information

How do low-quality females know they re low-quality and do they always prefer low-quality mates?

How do low-quality females know they re low-quality and do they always prefer low-quality mates? Introduction: How do low-quality females know they re low-quality and do they always prefer low-quality mates? The relatively young field of condition-dependent variation in female mate preferences has

More information

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia) Luke Campillo and Aaron Claus IBS Animal Behavior Prof. Wisenden 6/25/2009 Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia) Abstract: The Song Sparrow

More information

Lab 7. Evolution Lab. Name: General Introduction:

Lab 7. Evolution Lab. Name: General Introduction: Lab 7 Name: Evolution Lab OBJECTIVES: Help you develop an understanding of important factors that affect evolution of a species. Demonstrate important biological and environmental selection factors that

More information

Stimulus and Hormonal Determinants of Flehmen Behavior in Cats

Stimulus and Hormonal Determinants of Flehmen Behavior in Cats Hart, B.L. & Leedy, M.G. (1987). Stimulus and hormonal determinants of flehmen behavior in cats. Hormones and Behavior, 21(1): 44-52. (Mar 1987) Published by Elsevier (ISSN: 1095-6867). Stimulus and Hormonal

More information

Removal of adult males from the rearing environment increases preference for same-sex partners in the zebra finch

Removal of adult males from the rearing environment increases preference for same-sex partners in the zebra finch ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2000, 60, 47 53 doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1448, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Removal of adult males from the rearing environment increases preference for same-sex partners

More information

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research Terry J. Ord, Emília P. Martins Department of Biology, Indiana University Sidharth Thakur Computer Science Department, Indiana University

More information

Gonads and Singing Play Separate, Additive Roles in New Neuron Recruitment in Adult Canary Brain

Gonads and Singing Play Separate, Additive Roles in New Neuron Recruitment in Adult Canary Brain The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1, 2002, 22(19):8684 8690 Gonads and Singing Play Separate, Additive Roles in New Neuron Recruitment in Adult Canary Brain Benjamín Alvarez-Borda and Fernando Nottebohm

More information

SEXUAL IMPRINTING IN FEMALE ZEBRA FINCHES: CHANGES IN PREFERENCES AS AN EFFECT OF ADULT EXPERIENCE

SEXUAL IMPRINTING IN FEMALE ZEBRA FINCHES: CHANGES IN PREFERENCES AS AN EFFECT OF ADULT EXPERIENCE SEXUAL IMPRINTING IN FEMALE ZEBRA FINCHES: CHANGES IN PREFERENCES AS AN EFFECT OF ADULT EXPERIENCE by SABINE OETTING and HANS-JOACHIM BISCHOF1) (Lehrstuhl für Verhaltensforschung, Fakultät Biologie Universitat

More information

BRIEF REPORT A Brain of Her Own: A Neural Correlate of Song Assessment in a Female Songbird

BRIEF REPORT A Brain of Her Own: A Neural Correlate of Song Assessment in a Female Songbird NEUROBIOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MEMORY 68, 325 332 (1997) ARTICLE NO. NL973781 BRIEF REPORT A Brain of Her Own: A Neural Correlate of Song Assessment in a Female Songbird Kristy S. Hamilton,* Andrew P. King,*

More information

Bi156 Lecture 1/13/12. Dog Genetics

Bi156 Lecture 1/13/12. Dog Genetics Bi156 Lecture 1/13/12 Dog Genetics The radiation of the family Canidae occurred about 100 million years ago. Dogs are most closely related to wolves, from which they diverged through domestication about

More information

Sexual Differentiation of the Zebra Finch Song System Parallels Genetic, Not Gonadal, Sex

Sexual Differentiation of the Zebra Finch Song System Parallels Genetic, Not Gonadal, Sex Hormones and Behavior 36, 141 152 (1999) Article ID hbeh.1999.1537, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Sexual Differentiation of the Zebra Finch Song System Parallels Genetic, Not Gonadal,

More information

This is an Open Access document downloaded from ORCA, Cardiff University's institutional repository:

This is an Open Access document downloaded from ORCA, Cardiff University's institutional repository: This is an Open Access document downloaded from ORCA, Cardiff University's institutional repository: http://orca.cf.ac.uk/112181/ This is the author s version of a work that was submitted to / accepted

More information

Faculty Mentor, Department of Integrative Biology, Oklahoma State University

Faculty Mentor, Department of Integrative Biology, Oklahoma State University Sex Recognition in Anole Lizards Authors: Shelby Stavins and Dr. Matthew Lovern * Abstract: Sexual selection is the process that furthers a species, and either improves the genetic variability or weakens

More information

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 693 Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp. 693-697 Conspecific aggression in a Wood Stork colony in Georgia.-The probability of interactions among conspecifics, including aggression, is

More information

The melanocortin 1 receptor (mc1r) is a gene that has been implicated in the wide

The melanocortin 1 receptor (mc1r) is a gene that has been implicated in the wide Introduction The melanocortin 1 receptor (mc1r) is a gene that has been implicated in the wide variety of colors that exist in nature. It is responsible for hair and skin color in humans and the various

More information

ANNUAL STATISTICAL REPORT FOR ANIMALS USED IN IRELAND UNDER SCIENTIFIC ANIMAL PROTECTION LEGISLATION

ANNUAL STATISTICAL REPORT FOR ANIMALS USED IN IRELAND UNDER SCIENTIFIC ANIMAL PROTECTION LEGISLATION ANNUAL STATISTICAL REPORT FOR ANIMALS USED IN IRELAND UNDER SCIENTIFIC ANIMAL PROTECTION LEGISLATION 2013 CONTENTS 1. Introduction 2. Summary 3. Results 3.1 Species and numbers of naive animals used in

More information

This article is downloaded from.

This article is downloaded from. This article is downloaded from http://researchoutput.csu.edu.au It is the paper published as: Author: A. Wichman, L. Rogers and R. Freire Title: Visual lateralisation and development of spatial and social

More information

Changes to the IACUC s Environmental Enrichment Policy Addition of the IACUC s Social Housing Policy

Changes to the IACUC s Environmental Enrichment Policy Addition of the IACUC s Social Housing Policy Changes to the IACUC s Environmental Enrichment Policy Addition of the IACUC s Social Housing Policy What is enrichment, why it is important? Why the revision? The Guide. Revision to the IACUC enrichment

More information

Aggression and social structure

Aggression and social structure Aggression and social structure What is aggression? Any behavior that cause injuries to other animal Social interaction with the intention to inflict damage Hostile, injurious or destructive behavior caused

More information

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19, AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19, 575-582 SHIFTS OF 'ATTENTION' IN CHICKS DURING FEEDING BY MARIAN DAWKINS Department of Zoology, University of Oxford Abstract. Feeding in 'runs' of and grains suggested the possibility

More information

Behavioral Characteristics of Scent Marking Behavior in. The Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus)

Behavioral Characteristics of Scent Marking Behavior in. The Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus) Exp. Anim. 30 (2), 107-112, 1981 Behavioral Characteristics Scent Marking Behavior in Mongolian Gerbil (Meriones unguiculus). Hiroyuki YOSHIMURA Department Pharmacology School Medicine Ehime University

More information

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus)

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus) The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus) Abstract L.M. van Zomeren april 2009 supervised by Giuseppe Boncoraglio and Ton

More information

Evolution in dogs. Megan Elmore CS374 11/16/2010. (thanks to Dan Newburger for many slides' content)

Evolution in dogs. Megan Elmore CS374 11/16/2010. (thanks to Dan Newburger for many slides' content) Evolution in dogs Megan Elmore CS374 11/16/2010 (thanks to Dan Newburger for many slides' content) Papers for today Vonholdt BM et al (2010). Genome-wide SNP and haplotype analyses reveal a rich history

More information

LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE'

LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE' LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE' HORACE W. FELDMAN Bussey Inslitutim, Harvard Univwsity, Forest Hills, Boston, Massachusetts Received June 4, 1924 Present concepts of some phenomena of

More information

RARC: Animal Social Housing & Enrichment Requirements (ASHER)

RARC: Animal Social Housing & Enrichment Requirements (ASHER) 1) Social Housing a) Definitions: Single is defined as 1 animal in 1 primary enclosure; pair is 2 animals in 1 primary enclosure, and social is 3 or more animals in 1 primary enclosure. i) There are 2

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5870/1679/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Drosophila Egg-Laying Site Selection as a System to Study Simple Decision-Making Processes Chung-hui Yang, Priyanka

More information

Coyote (Canis latrans)

Coyote (Canis latrans) Coyote (Canis latrans) Coyotes are among the most adaptable mammals in North America. They have an enormous geographical distribution and can live in very diverse ecological settings, even successfully

More information

Institutional Animal Care & Use Program - UTEP Title: Animal Enrichment Policy#: 014 Date in Effect: 27 February 2015

Institutional Animal Care & Use Program - UTEP Title: Animal Enrichment Policy#: 014 Date in Effect: 27 February 2015 IACUC Policy #014-A, Page 1 of 5 Institutional Animal Care & Use Program - UTEP Title: Animal Enrichment Policy#: 014 Date in Effect: 27 February 2015 Version #: A Rev Date: In Effect Rescinded Date Rescinded:

More information

DP.1. Control tables

DP.1. Control tables Data inclusion criteria Report year: 2015 Country: Croatia EU Submission: ALL Genetic status: ALL Animal Species: ALL Species grouping Level 1: ALL Species grouping Level 2: ALL Mammals: ALL Non-human

More information

DP.1. Control tables

DP.1. Control tables Data inclusion criteria Report year: 2014 Country: Croatia EU Submission: ALL Genetic status: ALL Animal Species: ALL Species grouping Level 1: ALL Species grouping Level 2: ALL Mammals: ALL Non-human

More information

stable (11). This work also reported that the size of HVC mechanisms behind this growth were not determined. Thus,

stable (11). This work also reported that the size of HVC mechanisms behind this growth were not determined. Thus, Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 91, pp. 7854-7858, August 1994 Neurobiology Testosterone increases the recruitment and/or survival of new high vocal center neurons in adult female canaries (neurogenesis/neuronal

More information

Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution

Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution Scientific American Magazine - March 16, 2009 Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution Analyses of primate visual pigments show that our color vision evolved in an unusual way and that the

More information

American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior: Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals

American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior: Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior: Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals AVSAB is concerned with the recent re-emergence of dominance theory

More information

The Development of Behavior

The Development of Behavior The Development of Behavior 0 people liked this 0 discussions READING ASSIGNMENT Read this assignment. Though you've already read the textbook reading assignment that accompanies this assignment, you may

More information

Deer Inquiry: Evolution Why have red deer and elk diverged?

Deer Inquiry: Evolution Why have red deer and elk diverged? Texas A&M University Department of Wildlife & Fisheries Sciences Ethology Deer Inquiry: Evolution Why have red deer and elk diverged? Dr. Jane M. Packard j-packard@tamu.edu L e a r n i n g, D i s c o v

More information

Activity 1: Changes in beak size populations in low precipitation

Activity 1: Changes in beak size populations in low precipitation Darwin s Finches Lab Work individually or in groups of -3 at a computer Introduction The finches on Darwin and Wallace Islands feed on seeds produced by plants growing on these islands. There are three

More information

What causes lizards brains to change size?

What causes lizards brains to change size? December 2017 What causes lizards brains to change size? GET OFF MY LAND Authors: Susan Crow, Meghan Pawlowski, Manyowa Meki, Lara LaDage, Timothy Roth II, Cynthia Downs, Barry Sinervo and Vladimir Pravosudov

More information

RELATIONSHIPS AMONG WEIGHTS AND CALVING PERFORMANCE OF HEIFERS IN A HERD OF UNSELECTED CATTLE

RELATIONSHIPS AMONG WEIGHTS AND CALVING PERFORMANCE OF HEIFERS IN A HERD OF UNSELECTED CATTLE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG WEIGHTS AND CALVING PERFORMANCE OF HEIFERS IN A HERD OF UNSELECTED CATTLE T. C. NELSEN, R. E. SHORT, J. J. URICK and W. L. REYNOLDS1, USA SUMMARY Two important traits of a productive

More information

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns Demography and Populations Survivorship Demography is the study of fecundity and survival Four critical variables Age of first breeding Number of young fledged each year Juvenile survival Adult survival

More information

T u l a n e U n i v e r s i t y I A C U C Guidelines for Rodent & Rabbit Anesthesia, Analgesia and Tranquilization & Euthanasia Methods

T u l a n e U n i v e r s i t y I A C U C Guidelines for Rodent & Rabbit Anesthesia, Analgesia and Tranquilization & Euthanasia Methods T u l a n e U n i v e r s i t y I A C U C Guidelines for Rodent & Rabbit Anesthesia, Analgesia and Tranquilization & Euthanasia Methods Abbreviations: General Considerations IV = intravenous SC = subcutaneous

More information

Biology. Slide 1 of 33. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Biology. Slide 1 of 33. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall Biology 1 of 33 16-3 The Process of 16-3 The Process of Speciation Speciation 2 of 33 16-3 The Process of Speciation Natural selection and chance events can change the relative frequencies of alleles in

More information

Social Housing and Environmental Enrichment Policy

Social Housing and Environmental Enrichment Policy Social Housing and Environmental Enrichment Policy Purpose: This document sets forth the policy for housing social species and examples of environmental enrichment that must be provided to all species.

More information

1.14 Infanticide by a male lion. Bad fathers in wild life

1.14 Infanticide by a male lion. Bad fathers in wild life 1.14 Infanticide by a male lion Bad fathers in wild life Proximate and Ultimate questions Finding phenomena Causal Question Hypothesis 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 Experiments Proximate and Ultimate questions Good scientists

More information

Key considerations in the breeding of macaques and marmosets for scientific purposes

Key considerations in the breeding of macaques and marmosets for scientific purposes Key considerations in the breeding of macaques and marmosets for scientific purposes Key considerations in the breeding of macaques and marmosets for scientific purposes Laboratory Animal Science Association

More information

NEURAL RESPONSES TO AUDITORY RHYTHMS IN THE ZEBRA FINCH. Jennifer A. Lampen

NEURAL RESPONSES TO AUDITORY RHYTHMS IN THE ZEBRA FINCH. Jennifer A. Lampen NEURAL RESPONSES TO AUDITORY RHYTHMS IN THE ZEBRA FINCH By Jennifer A. Lampen A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Neuroscience

More information

EDUCATIONAL COMMENTARY - Methicillin-Resistant Staphylococcus aureus: An Update

EDUCATIONAL COMMENTARY - Methicillin-Resistant Staphylococcus aureus: An Update EDUCATIONAL COMMENTARY - Methicillin-Resistant Staphylococcus aureus: An Update Educational commentary is provided through our affiliation with the American Society for Clinical Pathology (ASCP). To obtain

More information

Adjustment Factors in NSIP 1

Adjustment Factors in NSIP 1 Adjustment Factors in NSIP 1 David Notter and Daniel Brown Summary Multiplicative adjustment factors for effects of type of birth and rearing on weaning and postweaning lamb weights were systematically

More information

Female zebra finches prefer unfamiliar males but not when watching noninteractive video

Female zebra finches prefer unfamiliar males but not when watching noninteractive video ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2006, 72, 161 167 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.12.005 Female zebra finches prefer unfamiliar males but not when watching noninteractive video JOHN P. SWADDLE, LARA MCBRIDE & SABINA MALHOTRA

More information

The Evolution of Human-Biting Preference in Mosquitoes

The Evolution of Human-Biting Preference in Mosquitoes Got Blood? The Evolution of Human-Biting Preference in Mosquitoes by Gary H. Laverty Department of Biological Sciences University of Delaware, Newark, DE Part I A Matter of Preference So, what do we do

More information

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories Life History Theory Why do some birds lay 1-2 eggs whereas others 12+? Why do some species begin reproducing at < 1 year whereas others not

More information

Human-Animal Interactions in the Turkey Industry

Human-Animal Interactions in the Turkey Industry Human-Animal Interactions in the Turkey Industry Dr. Naomi A. Botheras 1, Ms. Jessica A. Pempek 2, Mr. Drew K. Enigk 2 1 PI, 222E Animal Sciences Building, 2029 Fyffe Court, Columbus, OH 43210 (614) 292-3776;

More information

ANNUAL STATISTICAL REPORT FOR ANIMALS USED IN IRELAND UNDER SCIENTIFIC ANIMAL PROTECTION LEGISLATION

ANNUAL STATISTICAL REPORT FOR ANIMALS USED IN IRELAND UNDER SCIENTIFIC ANIMAL PROTECTION LEGISLATION ANNUAL STATISTICAL REPORT FOR ANIMALS USED IN IRELAND UNDER SCIENTIFIC ANIMAL PROTECTION LEGISLATION 2015 CONTENTS 1. Introduction 2. Summary 3. Results 3.1 Species and numbers of naïve animals used in

More information

Research Thesis. by Nathaniel J. Sackinger. The Ohio State University June 2013

Research Thesis. by Nathaniel J. Sackinger. The Ohio State University June 2013 1 Do Male House Wrens (Troglodytes aedon) Vary Their Singing Among Various Reproductive Stages? Research Thesis Presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation with Research Distinction

More information

COMMISSION OF THE EUROPEAN COMMUNITIES REPORT FROM THE COMMISSION TO THE COUNCIL AND THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT

COMMISSION OF THE EUROPEAN COMMUNITIES REPORT FROM THE COMMISSION TO THE COUNCIL AND THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT COMMISSION OF THE EUROPEAN COMMUNITIES Brussels, 20.1.2005 COM(2005) 7 final. REPORT FROM THE COMMISSION TO THE COUNCIL AND THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT FOURTH REPORT ON THE STATISTICS ON THE NUMBER OF ANIMALS

More information

Cover picture with friendly permission extracted from a photo by: Barry O`Malley, Sydney, Australia (

Cover picture with friendly permission extracted from a photo by: Barry O`Malley, Sydney, Australia ( Cover picture with friendly permission extracted from a photo by: Barry O`Malley, Sydney, Australia (www.thelandy.com) - Zebra finches in the Australian outback - Social Influences during Adolescence

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALS AND METHODS

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALS AND METHODS SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALS AND METHODS In order to estimate the relative intensity of the mrna labeling, we compared the signal in each brain region with that produced by the [ 14 C] microscales included in

More information

Animal Traits and Behaviors that Enhance Survival. Copyright 2010:PEER.tamu.edu

Animal Traits and Behaviors that Enhance Survival. Copyright 2010:PEER.tamu.edu Animal Traits and Behaviors that Enhance Survival Copyright 2010:PEER.tamu.edu What We Are Going To Learn: What are traits? Inherited vs. Learned Response to stimuli Evolutionary Adaptations Natural Selection

More information

Evolution of Mating system: A Game Theory

Evolution of Mating system: A Game Theory Summary of Chapter-1 Evolution of Mating system: A Game Theory Males that want many females (Polygamous) Males with infanticidal mode Females with promiscuity to protect babies Males that keep one female

More information

Nares Occlusion Eliminates Heterosexual Partner Selection without Disrupting Coitus in Ferrets of Both Sexes

Nares Occlusion Eliminates Heterosexual Partner Selection without Disrupting Coitus in Ferrets of Both Sexes The Journal of Neuroscience, August 1, 2001, 21(15):5832 5840 Nares Occlusion Eliminates Heterosexual Partner Selection without Disrupting Coitus in Ferrets of Both Sexes Kevin R. Kelliher and Michael

More information

An Analysis of the Neural Representation of Birdsong Memory

An Analysis of the Neural Representation of Birdsong Memory The Journal of Neuroscience, May 26, 2004 24(21):4971 4977 4971 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive An Analysis of the Neural Representation of Birdsong Memory Nienke J. Terpstra, 1 Johan J. Bolhuis, 2 and Ardie

More information

1 In 1958, scientists made a breakthrough in artificial reproductive cloning by successfully cloning a

1 In 1958, scientists made a breakthrough in artificial reproductive cloning by successfully cloning a 1 In 1958, scientists made a breakthrough in artificial reproductive cloning by successfully cloning a vertebrate species. The species cloned was the African clawed frog, Xenopus laevis. Fig. 1.1, on page

More information

HEREDITARY STUDENT PACKET # 5

HEREDITARY STUDENT PACKET # 5 HEREDITARY STUDENT PACKET # 5 Name: Date: Big Idea 16: Heredity and Reproduction Benchmark: SC.7.L.16.1: Understand and explain that every organism requires a set of instructions that specifies its traits,

More information

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar Active sensing Ehud Ahissar 1 Active sensing Passive vs active sensing (touch) Comparison across senses Basic coding principles -------- Perceptual loops Sensation-targeted motor control Proprioception

More information

University Council on Animal Care

University Council on Animal Care Policy: Research Animal Procurement Category: Compliance Assurance Subject: Procurement of live animals used in animal-based science Approving Authority: University Council on Animal Care Responsible Office:

More information

Silverback Male Presence and Group Stability in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)

Silverback Male Presence and Group Stability in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) Brief Report Folia Primatol 753 Received: August 16, 2002 DOI: 10.1159/0000XXXXX Accepted after revision: October 30, 2002 Silverback Male Presence and Group Stability in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)

More information

UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee

UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee Standard Operating Procedure (SOP): Approving Investigator-Managed Use Sites and Housing Areas EFFECTIVE ISSUE DATE: 5/2004 REVISION

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature12234 Supplementary Figure 1. Embryonic naked mole-rat fibroblasts do not undergo ECI. Embryonic naked mole-rat fibroblasts ( EF) were isolated from eight mid-gestation embryos. All the

More information

Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and Sociality

Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and Sociality International Journal of Poultry Science 9 (12): 1087-1091, 2010 ISSN 1682-8356 Asian Network for Scientific Information, 2010 Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and

More information

SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT

SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT SHORT TERM SCIENTIFIC MISSION (STSM) SCIENTIFIC REPORT The STSM applicant submits this report for approval to the STSM coordinator Action number: CA15134 Synergy for preventing damaging behaviour in group

More information

Was the Spotted Horse an Imaginary Creature? g.org/sciencenow/2011/11/was-the-spotted-horse-an-imagina.html

Was the Spotted Horse an Imaginary Creature?   g.org/sciencenow/2011/11/was-the-spotted-horse-an-imagina.html Was the Spotted Horse an Imaginary Creature? http://news.sciencema g.org/sciencenow/2011/11/was-the-spotted-horse-an-imagina.html 1 Genotypes of predomestic horses match phenotypes painted in Paleolithic

More information

Arrhythmic Song Exposure Increases ZENK Expression in Auditory Cortical Areas and Nucleus Taeniae of the Adult Zebra Finch

Arrhythmic Song Exposure Increases ZENK Expression in Auditory Cortical Areas and Nucleus Taeniae of the Adult Zebra Finch Arrhythmic Song Exposure Increases ZENK Expression in Auditory Cortical Areas and Nucleus Taeniae of the Adult Zebra Finch Jennifer Lampen 1 *, Katherine Jones 2, J. Devin McAuley 1,2, Soo-Eun Chang 3,

More information

NAME: DATE: SECTION:

NAME: DATE: SECTION: NAME: DATE: SECTION: MCAS PREP PACKET EVOLUTION AND BIODIVERSITY 1. Which of the following observations best supports the conclusion that dolphins and sharks do not have a recent common ancestor? A. Dolphins

More information

Original Draft: 11/4/97 Revised Draft: 6/21/12

Original Draft: 11/4/97 Revised Draft: 6/21/12 Original Draft: 11/4/97 Revised Draft: 6/21/12 Dear Interested Person or Party: The following is a scientific opinion letter requested by Brooks Fahy, Executive Director of Predator Defense. This letter

More information

Overview LANCTB1. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs

Overview LANCTB1. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs Overview This standard covers observing, assessing and responding to the behaviour of dogs. The standard involves understanding the normal behaviour of the type of dog that you are working with and observing

More information

The color and patterning of pigmentation in cats, dogs, mice horses and other mammals results from the interaction of several different genes

The color and patterning of pigmentation in cats, dogs, mice horses and other mammals results from the interaction of several different genes The color and patterning of pigmentation in cats, dogs, mice horses and other mammals results from the interaction of several different genes 1 Gene Interactions: Specific alleles of one gene mask or modify

More information

Male song quality affects circulating but not yolk steroid concentrations in female canaries (Serinus canaria)

Male song quality affects circulating but not yolk steroid concentrations in female canaries (Serinus canaria) The Journal of Experimental Biology 208, 4593-4598 Published by The Company of Biologists 2005 doi:10.1242/jeb.01949 4593 Male song quality affects circulating but not yolk steroid concentrations in female

More information

COMPARING BODY CONDITION ESTIMATES OF ZOO BROTHER S ISLAND TUATARA (SPHENODON GUNTHERI) TO THAT OF THE WILD, A CLINICAL CASE

COMPARING BODY CONDITION ESTIMATES OF ZOO BROTHER S ISLAND TUATARA (SPHENODON GUNTHERI) TO THAT OF THE WILD, A CLINICAL CASE COMPARING BODY CONDITION ESTIMATES OF ZOO BROTHER S ISLAND TUATARA (SPHENODON GUNTHERI) TO THAT OF THE WILD, A CLINICAL CASE Kyle S. Thompson, BS,¹, ²* Michael L. Schlegel, PhD, PAS² ¹Oklahoma State University,

More information

Our Philosophy. Playing for Life! A Shelter s Training Program featuring Canine Play Groups presented by

Our Philosophy. Playing for Life! A Shelter s Training Program featuring Canine Play Groups presented by Playing for Life! A Shelter s Training Program featuring Canine Play Groups presented by Aimee Sadler Director of Training & Behavior Our Philosophy Before we can expect a dog to cope and thrive, we need

More information

Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection

Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection H. L. MARKS US Department of Agriculture, Science & Education Administration, Agricultural Research, uthern Regional Poultry Breeding

More information

Gliding Motility Assay for P. berghei Sporozoites

Gliding Motility Assay for P. berghei Sporozoites Gliding Motility Assay for P. berghei Sporozoites Important Notes: 1. For all dilutions (including antibodies and sporozoites), always make slightly more than needed. For instance, if you need 200 µl sporozoites

More information

3. records of distribution for proteins and feeds are being kept to facilitate tracing throughout the animal feed and animal production chain.

3. records of distribution for proteins and feeds are being kept to facilitate tracing throughout the animal feed and animal production chain. CANADA S FEED BAN The purpose of this paper is to explain the history and operation of Canada s feed ban and to put it into a broader North American context. Canada and the United States share the same

More information

Barbara French, Vice Chancellor, Strategic Communications & University Relations, University of California, San Francisco

Barbara French, Vice Chancellor, Strategic Communications & University Relations, University of California, San Francisco November 27, 2012 UCSF Statement on Its Animal Care and Research Program: Barbara French, Vice Chancellor, Strategic Communications & University Relations, University of California, San Francisco The University

More information

BEYOND MENDEL. Incomplete Dominance: Blue (BB) Red (RR) F 1 hybrids have appearance in between 2 parents Purple (BR)

BEYOND MENDEL. Incomplete Dominance: Blue (BB) Red (RR) F 1 hybrids have appearance in between 2 parents Purple (BR) AP BIOLOGY EVOLUTION/HEREDITY UNIT Unit 1 Part 4 Chapter 14 Activity #5 NAME DATE PERIOD BEYOND MENDEL INCOMPLETE DOMINANCE Incomplete Dominance: Blue (BB) Red (RR) F 1 hybrids have appearance in between

More information

Sincerely, Patrick Melese MA, DVM, DACVB (Behavior) and the staff of the Veterinary Behavior Consultants.

Sincerely, Patrick Melese MA, DVM, DACVB (Behavior) and the staff of the Veterinary Behavior Consultants. Dear Colleague: Thank you for your request for referral supplies to help clients obtain specialty veterinary behavioral medicine services. For 30 years now (VBC) has been seeing clients with companion

More information

Systems Neuroscience Nov. 22, 2016

Systems Neuroscience Nov. 22, 2016 Systems Neuroscience Nov. 22, 2016 Taste and Smell Daniel C. Kiper kiper@ini.ethz.ch http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Brain Facts -- Taste/Smell Average number of human taste buds = 5,000

More information