PREY-HANDLING BEHAVIOR OF HATCHLING ELAPHE HELENA (COLUBRIDAE)

Similar documents
Early experience shapes the development of behavioral repertoires of hatchling snakes

Meal Size Effects on Antipredator Behavior of Hatchling Trinket Snakes, Elaphe helena

Dipsas trinitatis (Trinidad Snail-eating Snake)

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

An Assessment of Environmental Enrichment on Morphology and Behavior of Yearling Rat Snakes (Elaphe obsoleta)

Is That a Boa or a Python?

Northern Copperhead Updated: April 8, 2018

All about snakes. What are snakes? Are snakes just lizards without legs? If you want to know more

Squamates of Connecticut

Malayan Pit Viper Venomous Very Dangerous

SOAR Research Proposal Summer How do sand boas capture prey they can t see?

COMPARING BODY CONDITION ESTIMATES OF ZOO BROTHER S ISLAND TUATARA (SPHENODON GUNTHERI) TO THAT OF THE WILD, A CLINICAL CASE

Station 1 1. (3 points) Identification: Station 2 6. (3 points) Identification:

Corn Snake Care Sheet

Doug Scull s Science and Nature

*Using the 2018 List. Use the image below to answer question 6.

THE concept that reptiles have preferred

CAPTIVE HUSBANDRY AND REPRODUCTION OF THE LEOPARD SNAKE ELAPHE SITULA

reproductive life History and the effects of sex and season on morphology in CRoTALus oreganus (northern PaCifiC RATTLESNAKES)

Motuora island reptile monitoring report for common & Pacific gecko 2017

Banded Krait Venomous Deadly

WHAT ARE HERPTILES? WHICH IS WHICH? 1. Vertebrates are animals that have 2. Complete the following chart of vertebrate groups: EGGS LAID WHERE?

Post-Release Success of Captive Bred Louisiana Pine Snakes

Squamates of Connecticut. May 11th 2017

Maturity and Other Reproductive Traits of the Kanahebi Lizard Takydromus tachydromoides (Sauria, Lacertidae) in Mito

The Rat Lungworm Lifecycle

Parthenogenesis in Varanus ornatus, the Ornate Nile Monitor.

Cobras By Guy Belleranti

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

Announcements. Results: due today at 5pm for weekend feedback, otherwise due at Monday at 9am

The big squeeze: scaling of constriction pressure in two of the world s largest snakes, Python reticulatus and P. molurus bivittatus

Indochinese Rat Snake Non Venomous Not Dangerous

Consequences of Extended Egg Retention in the Eastern Fence Lizard (Sceloporus undulatus)

Today there are approximately 250 species of turtles and tortoises.

Owl Pellet Dissection A Study of Food Chains & Food Webs

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF TWO SPECIES OF EGERNIA (SCINCIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

AN2.3 Curriculum: Animal Growth and Change (grade 2)

Curriculum connections: Science: grade 2 Life Science Animal Growth and Change Art: grades 1-4 Patterns, Animal Portraits

Seasonal Shifts in Reproductive Investment of Female Northern Grass Lizards ( Takydromus septentrionalis

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

P VASANTA KUMARI and JAMIL AHMAD KHAN Department of Zoology, Aligarh Muslim University, Aligarh

REPTILES. Scientific Classification of Reptiles To creep. Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Chordata Subphylum: Vertebrata Class: Reptilia

MATERNAL NEST-SITE CHOICE AND OFFSPRING FITNESS IN A TROPICAL SNAKE (TROPIDONOPHIS MAIRII, COLUBRIDAE)

A. Body Temperature Control Form and Function in Mammals

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

JoJoKeKe s Herpetology Exam

Motuora island reptile monitoring report for common & Pacific gecko 2016

Savannah Monitor. Habitat

B-Division Herpetology Test. By: Brooke Diamond

Taxonomy. Chapter 20. Evolutionary Development Diagram. I. Evolution 2/24/11. Kingdom - Animalia Phylum - Chordata Class Reptilia.

Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens

Notes on Varanus salvator marmoratus on Polillo Island, Philippines. Daniel Bennett.

Lizard malaria: cost to vertebrate host's reproductive success

NAME: DATE: SECTION:

Reproduction in Seed Plants (pp )

Title Madagascan Snake, Leioheterodon mad. Author(s) Mori, Akira; Randriamboavonjy, Tahi. Citation Current Herpetology (2010), 29(2):

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

A description of an Indo-Chinese rat snake (Ptyas korros [Schlegel, 1837]) clutch, with notes on an instance of twinning

About Reptiles A Guide for Children. Cathryn Sill Illustrated by John Sill

muscles (enhancing biting strength). Possible states: none, one, or two.

Morphological Variation in Anolis oculatus Between Dominican. Habitats

The puff adder is a large, sluggish, thick-bodied snake that rarely exceeds a meter in length.

Husbandry Guidelines Name Species Prepared by

MICROHABITAT AND PREY ODOR SELECTION IN THE FORAGING PIGMY RATTLESNAKE

Rules and liability insurance applicable for Expo Mitt and Expo Syd

Evolution of Regulatory Responses to Feeding in Snakes

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Prey-handling Behavior in the Gulf Crayfish Snake

Investigations of Giant Garter Snakes in The Natomas Basin: 2002 Field Season

Effects of nest temperature and moisture on phenotypic traits of hatchling snakes (Tropidonophis mairii, Colubridae) from tropical Australia

SECTION 3 IDENTIFYING ONTARIO S EASTERN MASSASAUGA RATTLESNAKE AND ITS LOOK-ALIKES

Iguana Technical Assistance Workshop. Presented by: Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission

RED-COCKADED WOODPECKERS VS RAT SNAKES: THE EFFECTIVENESS OF THE RESIN BARRIER

Test Booklet. Subject: LA, Grade: 07 NECAP 2012 Grade 7 Reading. Student name:

Field Herpetology Final Guide

A tail of two scorpions Featured scientists: Ashlee Rowe and Matt Rowe from University of Oklahoma

Get the other MEGA courses!

POSTNATAL DEVELOPMENT IN PEROMYSCUS MANICULATUS-POLIONOTUS HYBRIDS I. DEVELOPMENTAL LANDMARKS AND LITTER MORTALITY 12

Characteristics of a Reptile. Vertebrate animals Lungs Scaly skin Amniotic egg

Crotalus durissus vegrandis in captivity / 81

Aerobic metabolism during predation by a boid snake

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

DEFENSIVE BEHAVIOUR IN PIT VIPERS OF THE GENUS BOTHROPS (SERPENTES, VIPERIDAE)

Vertebrates. Vertebrate Characteristics. 444 Chapter 14

Captains Tryouts Herpetology Key. John P. Stevens High School. Rishabh Rout & Cindy Xu. Points: 114

Reptiles Notes. Compiled by the Davidson College Herpetology Laboratory

Folder 1. Turtles. Folder 2

The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae)

Fishes, Amphibians, Reptiles

REPORT OF ACTIVITIES 2009 TURTLE ECOLOGY RESEARCH REPORT Crescent Lake National Wildlife Refuge 3 to 26 June 2009

Spatial Learning of Shelter Locations and Associative Learning of a Foraging Task in the Cottonmouth, (Agkistrodon Piscivorus)

Notes on Road-Killed Snakes and Their Implications on Habitat Modification Due to Summer Flooding on the Mississippi River in West Central Illinois

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Objectives: Outline: Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles. Characteristics of Amphibians. Types and Numbers of Amphibians

ANS 490-A: Ewe Lamb stemperament and Effects on Maze Entry, Exit Order and Coping Styles When Exposed to Novel Stimulus

A Population Analysis of the Common Wall Lizard Podarcis muralis in Southwestern France

Title: Phylogenetic Methods and Vertebrate Phylogeny

Fact Sheet: Oustalet s Chameleon Furcifer oustaleti

Prey and predator in the amazon rainforest

Transcription:

Herpetologica, 59(4), 2003, 469 474 Ó 2003 by The Herpetologists League, Inc. PREY-HANDLING BEHAVIOR OF HATCHLING ELAPHE HELENA (COLUBRIDAE) RITA S. MEHTA 1,2 Department of Biology, University of Texas, Tyler, TX 75719, USA ABSTRACT: The effects of prey size on prey-handling behavior for 60 ingestively naive hatchling Elaphe helena were studied in the laboratory. Hatchlings were randomly assigned to one of three diet categories in which prey (Mus musculus) varied by relative mass differences of 20 35%, 40 46%, or 50 59% of an individual snake s own body mass. The effects of prey size on capture position, direction of ingestion, condition of prey at ingestion (dead/alive), feeding duration, and prey-handling tactic were observed and recorded for each feeding episode. Results indicated that prey size significantly affected the prey-handling behavior of hatchling E. helena. In the largest relative mass category, hatchlings captured prey by the anterior end more often than in the smaller two relative mass categories. Prey from the smallest relative mass category were simply seized whereas, in the medium and large categories, pinion and constriction behaviors were observed. Time to subdue and ingest the prey item increased with prey size categories. Key words: Colubridae; Effects of prey size; Elaphe helena; Prey-handling behavior THE SIZE, type, and activity level of various prey are thought to influence the feeding behaviors for many advanced snakes (de Queiroz, 1984; Moon, 2000). Variation in feeding response to variables, such as prey size, are considered adaptive (Diefenbach and Emslie, 1971; Loop and Bailey, 1972; Sazima, 1989). Prey size may affect two aspects of the feeding repertoire: prey-handling and direction of ingestion. Many derived snakes simply seize small prey items and consume them alive, while larger prey items are pinned or constricted and killed prior to ingestion (Loop and Bailey, 1972). Loop and Bailey (1972) asserted that, independent of their phylogenetic position, snakes tend to swallow large endothermic prey head first. One hypothesis for this behavior is that head-first ingestion may reduce overall feeding time and that such a reduction in feeding time may be advantageous by allowing more time for other activities (de Queiroz and de Queiroz, 1987). Independent of prey size, prey activity level can also affect prey-handling behavior. De Queiroz (1984) used gopher snakes (Pituophis melanoleucus) to show that a small, highly active prey item elicits a different feeding response than larger, inactive prey. Gopher snakes were significantly more likely to pinion 1 PRESENT ADDRESS: Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996-0900, USA. 2 CORRESPONDENCE: e-mail, rmehta2@utk.edu (i.e., press prey against the substrate with the anterior portion of the body) and constrict small active mice than nestling rats. Furthermore, adult mice were more likely to be killed prior to ingestion than neonate rats. Gopher snakes were, thus, able to match their preyhandling behaviors to the activity level of their prey. A more recent electromyography study revealed that muscular activity and constriction pressure in gopher snakes changed in response to struggling of prey (Moon, 2000). Most studies on snake feeding behavior are based on observations of adults, and knowledge of neonatal behavior is limited to a few studies (Mori, 1993a,b, 1994, 1995). Few studies address innate prey-handling behaviors for snake taxa that exhibit diversity in habitat, geography, and diet. In my study, I quantify the feeding behavior of hatchling Elaphe helena. This species is easy to maintain in a laboratory setting and data on preyhandling behavior exists for closely related species within the genus, allowing valuable comparisons. Adults of this species are recorded as preying on mammals but have also been known to incorporate lizards into their diet (Daniel, 1983). Whether or not hatchling E. helena undergo an ontogenetic shift in diet is unknown, as both mice and frogs have been found in wild-caught juveniles (Schulz, 1996). I also compare the feeding behaviors of E. quadrivirgata, E. climacophora, E. taeniura, and E. dione from the investigations of Mori 469

470 HERPETOLOGICA [Vol. 59, No. 4 (1991, 1993a,b) and Mori and Moriguchi (1988) to the feeding behavior of E. helena. My study addresses the following four questions. What prey-handling behaviors are employed by hatchling E. helena? Do hatchling E. helena show a preference for head-first ingestion of prey? Do hatchling E. helena change prey-handling behavior according to prey size? How does the feeding behavior of E. helena compare to other Asian Elaphe species? MATERIALS AND METHODS Experimental Procedures I used 60 hatchling E. helena from seven different clutches. These clutches were laid January May 2000 by four laboratory-raised females originally collected in Sri Lanka. Hatchlings averaged 32.61 cm 6 SE 4.83 snout vent length (SVL) and weighed 10 g 6 SE 3.24 after their primary ecdysis. Hatchlings were housed in 3.8-l glass jars (diameter, 12.5 cm; height, 23 cm) and were kept at an ambient temperature of 25 C. Fluorescent lighting as well as sunlight provided illumination during the day. Each jar contained aspen bedding (2 cm deep) and a small water dish. Feeding tests were conducted over a period of 3 mo when snakes were 15 30 d old. All study subjects were ingestively naïve when trials commenced. Hatchlings were randomly assigned to one of three prey-size groups (small, medium, and large). Each group contained 20 snakes. Prey size relative to snake size was expressed as a mass ratio (MR 5 prey mass/ predator mass; Gans, 1961; Greene, 1983). Small prey ranged from 20 35% of the body mass (BM) of the hatchling. Medium and large prey comprised 40 46% and 50 59%, respectively, of the BM of the hatchling. Each hatchling was only tested once. Feeding trials took place at 1800 2100 h. One hour prior to the experiment, an individual was placed in a 20-l terrarium lined with a sheet of white butcher paper and left to habituate to the enclosure. A red light was used to illuminate the experimental arena. Each trial began with the introduction of a single, live, newborn mouse (Mus musculus) of the appropriate size class into the terrarium using a pair of 50-cm forceps. All feeding behaviors of the snakes were observed as well as recorded with an 8-mm Sharp video recorder VL-E43U until the mouse was completely swallowed. If the snake did not attack the mouse within 45 min, the trial was terminated and repeated 3 d later. I followed eight variables during each trial (Table 1). The MR categories served as independent variables, and the behaviors in each category represented dependent variables. The effects of relative prey size on aspects of feeding duration were assessed with ANOVA and Tukey s (HSD) tests. Non-parametric multiple comparisons were used to examine preyhandling behavior in which Kruskall-Wallis tests were used for more than two samples. Binomial tests were used to assess whether head-first or tail-first ingestion was more common. The level of significance of all tests was P 0.05. RESULTS Twelve of 60 hatchlings refused to eat on the first trial, but eventually all hatchlings of E. helena fed during the experimental period. All 60 individuals were used in the following analysis. There were significant differences in capture positions among relative prey weights (F (2,57) 5 5.769, P 5 0.015). Post hoc pairwise comparisons indicated differences between the small and large MR categories (F (1,57) 5 16.44, P, 0.05). Mice tended to be captured head first as prey increased in size. Prey items were not captured by the middle part of the body. Seven prey-handling behaviors were observed during feeding trials. The methods employed were simple seizing (SS), delayed pinion (DP), delayed hairpin loop (DHL), delayed constriction (DC), immediate pinion (IP), immediate hairpin loop (IHL), and immediate constriction (IC). Different sets of these prey-handling behaviors emerged as MR increased. A Kruskal-Wallis test revealed a difference in the overall frequencies of the prominently seen prey-handling behaviors (SS, IHL, IC) with respect to each of the three MR categories (SS: H 5 11.784, df 5 2,57, P 5 0.003; IHL: H 5 8.429, df 5 2,57, P 5 0.015; IC: H 5 15.602, df 5 2,57, P, 0.005). Simple-seizing was most frequent in the small and medium MR categories, while IC was only observed in the large MR category

December 2003] HERPETOLOGICA 471 TABLE 1. Behaviors recorded during snake-prey encounters (after Mori, 1991, 1994). Behavior Capture position Prey-handling method Condition of prey before ingestion Effectiveness of constriction Swallowing position Time to subdue prey Time to ingest prey Feeding duration Definition Part of the prey s body first grasped by the hatchling: anterior (head and shoulder), middle (abdomen and forelegs), or posterior (pelvic region, hind legs, and tail) Simple seizing (SS): grasping the prey in its jaws without subduing it with the body; pinion (P): pressing the prey against the substrate by the snake s body; hairpin loop (HL): squeezing the prey between non-overlapping portions of the snake s body; constriction (C): constricting the prey by a fully encircling loop. Each of these behaviors can be performed immediately (I) after capture or delayed (D) 1 or more seconds after prey capture Dead or alive Evaluated only when the snake exhibited immediate coiling (HL, C). Four criteria were used: (1) Result of immediate coiling: the snake successfully coiled around the prey (success) or the snake attempted to coil the prey by typical wrapping and/or winding movements (Greenwald, 1978) but was unsuccessful (fail) (2) Coil release: after successful immediate coiling the snake released its coil within 10 s after striking because of irregular, unstable coiling and/ or struggling of the prey (coil release) (3) Occurrence of re-constriction: the snake attempted to re-constrict after initial failure (4) Overall number of prey items killed via constriction Anterior or posterior end of prey first Time from the moment the prey was struck or seized to the start of swallowing Time from the start of swallowing to the point at which the snake began pushing the prey toward its midbody and the snake s mouth could completely close Time from prey capture to prey ingestion as timed and recorded (multiple comparisons; Fig. 1). The preyhandling behaviors observed the least across all relative mass categories were DHL and DC. There were significant size differences between mice killed and mice swallowed alive (F (2,57) 5 10.813, P, 0.01). Mice were killed before ingestion only in the largest MR category and then in only 25% of large prey encounters. In the large prey category, IC was the single method used to subdue as well as kill mice. Eight individuals successfully coiled around their prey after the initial attack. Of these eight individuals, five killed their prey before ingestion. Six different individuals released their coils within 10 s of striking prey because of unstable coiling. All six of these individuals were observed re-constricting their prey. Among the three MR categories, there were significant differences in prey size between mice swallowed head first and tail first (Fig. 2; Z 5 6.043, df 5 2,57, P 5 0.017). The number of snakes ingesting prey head first was positively related to relative prey mass. Tailfirst ingestion was the preferred direction for the smallest MR category. The mean time to subdue prey differed significantly among the three MR categories (F (2,57) 5 54.42, P 5 0.034; Fig. 3). Significant differences resulted between the small and medium MR (F (1,58) 5 60.84, P 5 0.013) and medium and large MR (F (1,58) 5 53.22, P 5 0.042). Mean time to ingest prey differed significantly between the prey MRs (F (2,57) 5 43.48, P, 0.01). Post hoc tests revealed significance between the small MR category and the medium MR category (F (1,58) 5 77.14, P, 0.001) and the medium and large MR (F (1,58) 5 79.22, P, 0.001). Total feeding duration significantly varied between the three groups (F (2,57) 5 57.315, P 5 0.017). Differences in total feeding duration were significant between the small and medium MR (F (1,58) 5

472 HERPETOLOGICA [Vol. 59, No. 4 FIG. 2. Observations of direction of ingestion for hatchlings of Elaphe helena in the three mass ratio categories (20 35%, 40 46%, and 50 59%). Each weight class contained 20 hatchlings. White bars represent tailfirst ingestion and black bars indicate head-first ingestion. FIG. 1. Number of hatchlings of Elaphe helena exhibiting the following seven prey-handling behaviors in three relative prey mass ratio categories: (A) 20 35%; (B) 40 46%; and (C) 50 59%. Each category contained 20 hatchlings and exhibited the following behaviors: simpleseizing (SS), delayed pinion (DP), delayed hairpin loop (DHL), delayed constriction (DC), immediate pinion (IP), immediate hairpin loop (IHL), and immediate constriction (IC). 66.23, P, 0.001) and medium and large MR (F (1,58) 5 58.80, P, 0.003). The greatest difference in total feeding duration was evident between the small and large MR categories (F (1,58) 5 76.80, P, 0.001). DISCUSSION Feeding behavior for hatchlings of E. helena varied with prey size. As prey MR increased, the frequency of hatchlings capturing prey by the head also increased. Seven handling methods were observed during the experimental period, and, within each MR category, a particular prey-handling mode dominated (Fig. 1). These results suggest that hatchlings of E. helena are able to employ different handling behaviors when presented with different prey-size-specific stimuli. Hatchlings coiled larger prey items, while, for smaller prey items perhaps perceived as less challenging, they simply seized and swallowed the prey alive. Direction of ingestion for ingestively naive E. helena differed between the different MR categories. The occurrence of head-first ingestion did not significantly increase as food items increased in MR, suggesting that headfirst ingestion of prey for hatchlings of E. helena might not be an innate behavior, at least when consuming endothermic prey. Tail-first ingestion was actually the predominant direction of ingestion for the 20 35% and 40 46% MR categories. In the largest MR category, equal numbers of prey were consumed head first as were consumed tail first. The stimuli mediating discrimination of the head remain unclear and could vary with taxa. Some snakes may learn to ingest prey by trial and error, while other snakes may have an innate response (Greene, 1997). Hatchling rat snakes and king snakes may use cues, such as FIG. 3. Comparison of the means for time to subdue, time to ingest, and total feeding duration for the three mass ratio categories (20 35%, 40 46%, and 50 59%) in hatchlings of Elaphe helena. Each category contained 20 hatchlings.

December 2003] HERPETOLOGICA 473 the direction of hair or scales, the shape of the snout, and the texture of the underlying skull, to guide direction of prey ingestion (Greene, 1997). The interval between prey capture and prey ingestion (time to subdue prey) varied dramatically between the different MRs as well as between the different methods used to subdue the prey item. The most time intensive part of prey handling was the time required to subdue the prey item; this probably reflects the lack of experience by hatchling snakes. A study of ontogenetic mechanisms in prey-handling behavior of E. helena is needed to determine if head-first ingestion and efficiency in handling prey increases with experience, as well as whether tail-first ingestion actually does increase swallowing time. Subduing prey by hatchlings of E. helena entailed pinning the prey item and more or less constraining the prey rather than killing it, much like behaviors observed in adult Thamnophis elegans (de Queiroz and Groen, 2001; Gregory et al., 1980). Only in the largest MR category, where hatchlings ingested 50 59% of their own mass, was prey killed before ingestion. Prey in the largest mass category was killed by constriction, and the duration to subdue the prey was considerably increased. This increase in time to subdue larger prey and the fact that only 25% of large prey was killed by constriction may indicate that hatchlings of E. helena are not effective constrictors, or at least during the first encounter with endothermic prey. Very few individuals were able to use constriction as a means of subduing and killing the prey item. Also, some individuals uncoiled while struggling with prey. Lack of effectiveness may be due to immaturity of the muscles that are used for constricting. Hatchlings that were successful in killing prey via constriction, however, were not significantly bigger than the other hatchlings within the same MR category or between MR categories. The prey-handling behavior of E. helena is unique in the sense that coils were applied by winding with the posterior body without an initial twist. The coil behavior for other species of Elaphe is described as winding with the anterior body without an initial twist (Greene, 1977; Mori, 1991; Steward, 1971). On three different occasions when E. helena were accidentally disturbed while coiling around a prey item, the hatchlings were able to go into a defensive posture with the anterior portion of their body while the posterior portion was still wrapped around the prey. Coiling around the prey with the posterior portion of the body may be a way that hatchlings can maintain defensive vigilance while feeding. However, it is possible that coiling with the posterior portion of the body is due to inexperience with the prey item as well as a lack of muscle maturity. Three hypotheses, not mutually exclusive, have been proposed for the different responses to various sized prey items observed in some colubroids (Mori, 1991): (1) reduction of total prey-handling time, (2) protection of the snake itself from retaliation, such as biting, by the attacked prey, and (3) prevention of escape by the captured prey. My study indicates that prey size affects the predatory behavior of E. helena. Small prey items were simply seized, and larger prey items elicited the behaviors of pinion and constriction. In my study, the possibility of retaliation by mice in any of the three MR categories was unlikely since mice were still in an altricial state. For the same reason, escape by the captured prey was not possible. Hatchling snakes are probably using both tactile cues and the actual weight of the prey as indicators of which preyhandling behaviors to adopt. Ultimately, these varied responses to prey of different size may enable these snakes to minimize prey-handling time. My results are consistent with the findings of Mori (1994) and further demonstrate the diversity of responses to prey size in Old World Elaphe. The proficiency of constriction by hatchlings of E. helena is intermediate to that of good constrictors such as E. climacophora and poor constrictors such as E. quadrivirgata. My study further substantiates that there is a certain degree of variation in hatchling feeding behavior, even within the same genus. Young, inexperienced animals may reveal that specialized motor patterns exhibited in adults have distinct developmental histories in different taxa (Burghardt, 1978, 1993). Differences in the development of certain motor patterns may be linked to other behaviors, such as defensive postures. Species of the genus Elaphe (sensu lato) exhibit a

474 HERPETOLOGICA [Vol. 59, No. 4 variety of prey-handling behaviors that may be associated with their diverse ecologies. Future studies examining geographic variation and maternal effects on prey-handling behavior in colubrids may reveal extensive plasticity in feeding behavior of advanced snakes. Acknowledgments. I thank L. Almli, G. M. Burghardt, P. A. Burrows, L. Ford, N. B. Ford, R. Gutberlet, M. Kearney, J. Manrod, A. Mori, J. S. Placyk, Jr., J. T. Redwine, and R. Reiserer for helpful suggestions on this manuscript. Animals were kept in captivity in accordance with the University of Texas at Tyler Animal Care Protocol. This project was completed in partial fulfillment of the MS degree from the University of Texas at Tyler under the supervision of N. B. Ford. LITERATURE CITED BURGHARDT, G. M. 1978. Behavioral ontogeny in reptiles: whence, whither, and why? Pp. 149 174. In G. M. Burghardt and M. Bekoff (Eds.), The Development of Behavior. Garland, New York, New York, U.S.A.. 1993. Comparative imperative: genetics and ontogeny of chemoreceptive prey responses in natricine snakes. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 41:138 146. DANIEL, J. C. 1983. The Book of Indian Reptiles. Bombay Natural History Society, Leo at St. Francis Industrial and Technical Institute, Borivli, Bombay. DE QUEIROZ, A. 1984. Effects of prey type on the preyhandling behavior of the bull snake, Pituophis melanoleucus. Journal of Herpetology 18:333 336. DE QUEIROZ, A., AND K. DE QUEIROZ. 1987. Prey-handling behavior of Eumeces gilberti with comments on head first ingestion in squamates. Journal of Herpetology 21: 57 63. DE QUEIROZ, A., AND R. R. GROEN. 2001. The inconsistent and inefficient constricting behavior of Colorado western terrestrial garter snakes, Thamnophis elegans. Journal of Herpetology 35:450 460. DIEFENBACH, C. O., AND S. G. EMSLIE. 1971. Cues influencing the direction of prey ingestion of the Japanese rat snake, Elaphe climacophora (Colubridae, Serpentes). Herpetologica 27:461 466. GANS, C. 1961. The feeding mechanisms of snakes and its possible evolution. American Zoologist 1:217 227. GREENE, H. W. 1977. Phylogeny, convergence, and snake behavior. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, U.S.A.. 1983. Dietary origins and radiation of snakes. American Zoologist 23:431 441.. 1997. The Evolution of Mystery in Nature. University of California Press, Berkeley and Los Angeles, California, U.S.A. GREENWALD, O. E. 1978. Kinematics and time relations of prey capture by gopher snakes. Copeia 1978: 263 268. GREGORY, P. T., J. M. MACARTNEY, AND D. H. RIVARD. 1980. Small mammal predation and prey-handling behavior by the garter snake, Thamnophis elegans. Herpetologica 36:87 93. LOOP, M. S., AND L. G. BAILEY. 1972. The effects of relative prey size on the ingestion behavior of rodent-eating snakes. Psychonomic Science 28:167 169. MOON, B. R. 2000. The mechanics and muscular control of constriction in gopher snakes (Pituophis melanoleucus) and a king snake (Lampropeltis getula). Journal of Zoology (London) (2000) 252:83 98. MORI, A. 1991. Effects of prey size and type on preyhandling behavior in Elaphe quadrivirgata. Journal of Herpetology 25:160 166.. 1993a. Does feeding experience with different size of prey influence the subsequent prey-handling behavior in Elaphe climacophora? Ethology 11:153 156.. 1993b. Prey handling behavior of neonatal rat snakes, Elaphe taeniura and E. dione (Colubridae). Japanese Journal of Herpetology 15:59 63.. 1994. Prey-handling behavior of newly hatched snakes in two species of the genus Elaphe with comparison to adult behavior. Ethology 97:198 214.. 1995. Prey handling behavior of the young rat snake Elaphe taeniura (Squamata: Reptilia). Kyoto University Series of Biology 16:43 47. MORI, A., AND H. MORIGUCHI. 1988. Food habits of the snakes in Japan: a critical review. Snake 20:98 113. SAZIMA, I. 1989. Feeding behavior of the snail-eating snake, Dipsas indica. Journal of Herpetology 23:464 468. SCHULZ, K. 1996. A Monograph of the Colubrid Snakes of the Genus Elaphe Fitzinger. Koeltz Scientific Books, Czech Republic. STEWARD, J. W. 1971. The Snakes of Europe. Fairleigh Dickinson University Press, Canterbury, New Jersey, U.S.A. Accepted: 15 May 2003 Associate Editor: Carl Anthony