NESTLING FOODS AND FORAGING PATTERNS IN THE CLAY-COLORED SPARROW

Similar documents
DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

EVALUATION OF A METHOD FOR ESTIMATING THE LAYING RATE OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

State birds. A comparison of the Northern Mockingbird and the Western Meadowlark. By Shaden Jensen

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Ames, IA Ames, IA (515)

FOOD SAMPLING BIASES ASSOCIATED WITH THE LIGATURE METHOD

ACTIVITY PATTERNS AND HOME-RANGE USE OF NESTING LONG-EARED OWLS

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS OF THE NORTHERN CARDINAL, A LARGE HOST OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS

F RIEDMANN (1963) considers the Lark Sparrow (Chondestes grammacus)

COWBIRD PARASITISM IN THE KANSAS

PREDATION ON RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD EGGS AND NESTLINGS

ASPECTS OF THE BREEDING BIOLOGY AND PRODUCTIVITY OF BACHMAN S SPARROW IN CENTRAL ARKANSAS

NORTHERN GOSHAWK NEST SITE REQUIREMENTS IN THE COLORADO ROCKIES

RESPONSES OF BELL S VIREOS TO BROOD PARASITISM BY THE BROWN-HEADED COWBIRD IN KANSAS

Lynx Update May 25, 2009 INTRODUCTION

Wilson Bull., 94(2), 1982, pp

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Removal of Alaskan Bald Eagles for Translocation to Other States Michael J. Jacobson U.S Fish and Wildlife Service, Juneau, AK

THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

MARY F. WILLSON RESULTS

SITE-RELATED NESTING SUCCESS OF MOURNING DOVES AND AMERICAN ROBINS IN SHELTERBELTS

T HE recent and interesting paper by Alexander F. Skutch (1962) stimulated

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Lay Delay in Four Temperate Passerines. Caitlin Brickman

(MICRORHOPIAS QUIXENSIS), A TROPICAL FOREST PASSERINE

High Mortality of a Population of Cowbirds Wintering at Columbus, Ohio

Scaled Quail (Callipepla squamata)

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

OBSERVATIONS OF HAWAIIAN

FOOD HABITS OF NESTING COOPER S HAWKS AND GOSHAWKS IN NEW YORK AND PENNSYLVANIA

VARIATION IN INCUBATION PERIOD WITHIN A POPULATION OF THE EUROPEAN STARLING ROBERT E. RICKLEFS AND CYNTHIA

OF THE HOUSE FINCH AS A HOST OF THE BROWN-HEADED COWBIRD

EUROPEAN STARLING HOUSE FINCH

Rock Wren Nesting in an Artificial Rock Wall in Folsom, Sacramento County, California

THE WILSON BULLETIN. A QUARTERLY MAGAZINE OF ORNITHOLOGY Published by the Wilson Ornithological Club THE EASTERN CHIPPING SPARROW IN MICHIGAN

Observations on the Nesting of the Northern Hawk Owl, Surnia ulula, near Timmins and Iroquois Falls, Northeastern Ontario, in 2001

Piping Plover. Below: Note the color of the sand and the plover s back.

By ROGER J. SIGLIN and MILTON W. WELLER

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Chapman (1999) provides a comprehensive account.

THE NESTING OF THE BELTED FLYCATCHER. By MIGUEL ALVAREZ DEL TORO

TEMPERATURE REGULATION IN NESTLING CACTUS WRENS: THE NEST ENVIRONMENT

SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 757

FOOTEDNESS IN DOMESTIC PIGEONS

COWBIRD PARASITISM AND EVOLUTION OF ANTI-PARASITE STRATEGIES IN THE YELLOW WARBLER

COOPERATIVE BREEDING IN THE TROPICAL MOCKINGBIRD (MIMUS GILVUS) IN THE PANAMA CANAL ZONE

NOTES ON THE SPRING TERRITORY OF THE BLACKBIRD

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

HOST-PARASITE INTERACTIONS OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS AND DARK-EYED JUNCOS IN VIRGINIA

MORPHOLOGY OF INSECT MOUTHPARTS Erik Smith

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Raptor Ecology in the Thunder Basin of Northeast Wyoming

Evaluation of large-scale baiting programs more surprises from Central West Queensland

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF CTENOPHORUS CAUDICINCTUS (AGAMIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

Subject: Preliminary Draft Technical Memorandum Number Silver Lake Waterfowl Survey

PEREGRINE FALCON HABITAT MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES ONTARIO MINISTRY OF NATURAL RESOURCES

A TEST OF WHETHER ECONOMY OR NUTRITION DETERMINES FECAL SAC INGESTION IN NESTING CORVIDS

EIDER JOURNEY It s Summer Time for Eiders On the Breeding Ground

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Introduction. Description. This bird

RECOGNITION OF NEST, EGGS, NEST SITE, AND YOUNG IN FEMALE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS

Red Crowned Parakeet (Cyanoramphus novaezelandiae) health, disease and nesting study on Tiritiri Matangi 2014/2015. Emma Wells on behalf of

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1995) provides a comprehensive account.

What is the date at which most chicks would have been expected to fledge?

Effect of nestling sex ratio on the provisioning behavior of adult Eastern Bluebirds (Sialia sialis)

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

52 THE CONDOR Vol. 66

NATURAL AND SEXUAL VARIATION

FIELD SPARROW REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS AND

Ornithological Observations

Garden Birds. Blackbird Latin Name: Turdus merula

Management, Univ. California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California Accepted 15 Oct

Field Guide to Swan Lake

Key 1 Key to Insects Orders

468 TYRRELL, Nesting of Turkey Vulture

Western Snowy Plover Recovery and Habitat Restoration at Eden Landing Ecological Reserve

A Comparison of morphological differences between Gymnophthalmus spp. in Dominica, West Indies

So Many Insects! Part 1 Worksheet

Inland and saltmarsh feeding of wintering Brent Geese in Essex

The Effect of Phase Shifts in the Day-Night Cycle on Pigeon Homing at Distances of Less than One Mile

THE IMPACT OF COTTAGE DEVELOPMENT ON COMMON LOON REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS IN CENTRAL ONTARIO

48 RAPTOR RESEARCH Vol. 15 No. 2. top. < 0.Sm > side HABITUATION TO HUMAN DISTURBANCE IN NESTING ACCIPITERS

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1996) provides a comprehensive account.

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

THE MOLT OF THE AMERICAN GOLDFINCH

Parus major L. III. Home range of the basic flocks

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

Mice alone and their biodiversity impacts: a 5-year experiment at Maungatautari

AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA. R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L.

Summary of 2017 Field Season

698 THE WILSON BULLETIN l Vol. 103, No. 4, December 1991

FINAL PERFORMANCE REPORT

706 THE WILSON BULLETIN l Vol. 102, No. 4, December 1990

Transcription:

lf ilson Bull., 92(4), 1980, pp. 458-465 NESTLING FOODS AND FORAGING PATTERNS IN THE CLAY-COLORED SPARROW RICHARD W. KNAPTON The Clay-colored Sparrow (Spizella pallida) is a common breeding bird of central North America (Bent 1968, Erskine 1977), but little is known of its feeding ecology. In this paper, I report on nestling foods, on foraging behavior and on locations where prey was gathered, and I compare prey items brought to young among different pairs and between males and females within each pair. The Clay-colored Sparrow is an appropriate study animal as it is relatively easy to catch, its nests are not difficult to find, it readily accepts blinds close to the nest and it is easy to observe in the low shrub communities and surrounding meadows, marshes and pastures which it inhabits. METHODS The study was carried out in extreme southwestern Manitoba near the town of Lyleton. Two study areas were chosen, each incorporating extensive stands of low shrubs, primarily snowberry (Symphoricarpos occident&s) and silverberry (Elaeugnus commutata), with small areas of grass and of trembling aspen (Populus tremuloides). Study area 1 was a 5.86-ha plot about 2 km west of Lyleton, and study area 2 comprised 5.67 ha 14 km northwest of Lyleton (for further details see Knapton 1979). Daily observations were made during the breeding seasons of 1974-1976. Adult and immature Clay-colored Sparrows were captured in mist nets and nestlings were banded at about 5 days of age. Each bird received an aluminum band plus 2 colored plastic bands in different combinations for individual recognition. I observed individual sparrows in areas around the nesting habitat (the low shrub communities) during May and early June. I noted the identity of each bird, where it was foraging, and whether it was foraging alone or close (within 5 m) to another bird. From 15 June to 17 July 1976, I spent 65 h observing 7 pairs of Clay-colored Sparrows that were feeding nestlings. Observation periods were usually about 2-h long, and were carried out between 06:OO and 12:00 from blinds located 5-10 m from the nest. Pairs appeared to accept the presence of blinds, and no nest under observation was deserted or lost to predators. Information was recorded on portable tape recorders and later transcribed. The parent s line of flight on leaving the nest was recorded as 1 of 8 possible compass directions. Locations to which the parent birds flew to forage were recorded, and the distances determined by plotting the foraging locations on aerial photographs and measuring the distance to the nest. It was relatively easy to observe how far individuals flew before alighting, as the low height of the vegetation permitted an extensive panoramic view of surrounding areas. I obtained information on prey items brought to the young by 2 methods. I used a portable Sony Videocorder equipped with a 230 mm lens, and filmed parent birds carrying food items in their beaks. Characteristically, each parent bird returning with prey perched conspicuously for several seconds before dropping down into the shrubbery to the nest; this allowed 458

Knapton - CLAY-COLORED SPARROW FORAGING 459 several seconds of film footage to be taken per prey item. The video tapes were later analyzed on closed-circuit television, a still switch on the videocorder allowing close analysis of individual frames. It was possible to identify most prey items at least to order. I also obtained actual samples of food delivered to nestlings in 4 other nests using the neck-band method. I attached ligatures around the necks of young Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater), 1 cowbird in each nest, each cowbird from 5-7 days old. Each young cowbird was in a Clay-colored Sparrow nest in which the sparrow s own young were no longer present. The ligatures were left on the young cowbirds for about 1 h at a time. Ligatures were not placed on young Clay-colored Sparrows because concurrent information was being collected on survival rates of the young sparrows, and repeated visits to their nests within 1 day might have influenced predation rates. Also, nestling Clay-colored Sparrows 6 days or older cannot be easily returned to the nest-bowl, and handling often causes nest departure prematurely. The ligature method is not without potential error, as small prey items can slip through the ligature or else food can be coughed up and eaten by the parents (Orians 1966). The similarity in results (see below) between the videocorder and the ligature methods suggest that errors were not large. RESULTS Foods presented to nestlings.-adult Clay-colored Sparrows fed their young a wide variety of invertebrates (Table 1). Predominant foods were lepidopteran larvae, orthopteran nymphs and various types of Odonata. These 3 groups comprised 76% of the total number of prey items recorded. A similar breadth of diet is evident in the sample of prey items collected from the nestling cowbirds (Table l), mainly lepidopteran larvae and orthopteran nymphs, indicating that adult sparrows brought the same sorts of food to young cowbirds as they did to their own young. There were large differences between pairs in foods presented to nestlings (Table 2). Pair 5, whose territory was close to a pond overgrown with smartweed (Polygonurn spp.), fed their young almost exclusively on narrow-winged damselflies (Coenagrionidae), apparently taking advantage of a readily available and abundant food source localized around the pond. Pair 6 fed their young mostly short-horned grasshoppers (Acrididae), whereas lepidopteran larvae predominated in the food of the remaining 5 pairs. Males and females brought the same type of prey items to their young in roughly the same proportions (Table 2). Horn s (1966) measure of overlap, C, is given by c= 2 2 XiYi 1=1 where Xi and Yi are the proportions of prey species i for males and fe-

460 THE WILSON BULLETIN * Vol. 92, No. 4, December 1980 TABLE 1 FOOD ITEMS BROUGHTTO ~~CLAY-COLORED SPARROWS'NESTSI Prey group Videocorder information Number PerCeIlt Ligature information Number PeWXlt Orthoptera 108 18.1 11 29.7 Neuroptera 11 1.8 Lepidoptera (adult) 23 3.8 (larvae) 256 42.8 15 40.5 Odonata 93 15.6 3 8.1 Coleoptera 12 2.0 4 10.8 Hemiptera 1 0.1 Diptera 58 9.7 4 10.8 Araneida 4 0.7 Unidentified 32 5.4 Total 598 100 37 100 Food was recorded at 7 nests by videocorder and at 4 nests by ligatures an young cowbirds (28 samples). males, respectively. A value of 0% means no overlap, a value of 100% means total overlap. For Clay-colored Sparrows, C is 96%, indicating there is no difference between males and females within each pair in prey items presented to nestlings. There was a noticeable change in the relative importance of major food items during the breeding season. Proportionately more lepidopteran larvae were taken in June, Odonata increased in July, and there tended to be a general increase in the relative numbers of orthopteran nymphs over the breeding season. These trends are similar between males and females (Knapton 1978a). Foraging behavior.-observations throughout the breeding season on several pairs of banded Clay-colored Sparrows indicated that the birds did not obtain food for themselves or for their young in their territories. Up to mid-may, male Clay-colored Sparrows sometimes fed in grassy areas adjacent to their territories, possibly because they were reluctant to fly far from their territories before territorial boundaries were firmly established. After mid-may, foraging and food-gathering for the young occurred away from the breeding areas, in arable and pasture land, weedy fields and around the edges of sloughs and marshes. Between territory establishment and the nestling stage of the reproductive cycle, males of adjacent territories foraged at about the same time of day (late morning and late afternoon) off the breeding areas. No overt aggressive interactions were observed between males foraging off territory during 15 h of observation from 6 May to 2 June 1976, although on 9

Knapton - CLAY-COLORED SPARROW FORAGING 461 TABLE 2 FOODS PRESENTED TO NESTLINGS BY EACH OF 7 PAIRS OF CLAY-COLORED SPARROWS Pair 1 Pair 2 Pair 3 Pair 4 Pair 5 Pair 6 Pair 7 Total Prey group Orthoptera Neuroptera Lepidoptera (adult) (larvae) Odonata Coleoptera Hemiptera Diptera Araneida Unidentified -TM FFF%?-=M F 9 6 5 2 13 10 10 8 1 0 27 17 0 0 65 43 3 0 0 1 0 1 0 4 010100 3 8 0 0 4 0 4 3 6 2 0 0 1 3 0 0 15 8 21 24 26 18 38 30 23 26 0 1 2 6 23 18 133 123 4 8 5 2 0 1 0 2 30 36 1 2 2 0 42 51 1 4 0 0 0 0 3 3001000 5 7 00000000000010 10 16 6 5 7 0 5 2 7 0 0 1 0 4 3 30 28 0 0 0 0 1 3 0 0 000000 13 4 264211111 3 2 2 1 19 13 occasions males from adjacent territories were observed within 5 m of each other in the foraging areas. Furthermore, although individual males seemed to forage in specific locations, there was no indication that males defended feeding territories. Flight lines.-parents were not observed to gather food for their young within the confines of the territory nor over the breeding habitat as a whole. I therefore recorded the lines of flight which the birds followed between the nest and the foraging locations. There were no major differences between members of a pair in the direction flown from a nest towards the foraging locations (Knapton 1978a). In general, a male and a female of a pair, whether leaving the nest singly or together, flew predominantly foraging locations, in one and the same direction towards the Pairs 1 and 5 held peripheral territories, and they used feeding areas adjacent to their territories. Pairs holding central territories flew to prob- ably the closest available foraging locations. Pairs 3 and 4, which held adjacent territories and were feeding nestlings at the same time, consis- tently flew to the southwest on leaving their nests, and both pairs foraged in and around a large pond overgrown with vegetation. This suggests that neighboring birds might be following each other to preferred feeding lo- cations. However, the pond was probably the closest foraging location to both nests, and hence evidence for following remains inconclusive. Distances foraged from the nest.-pair 7 is omitted from this analysis as these birds frequently flew around an intervening clump of trees on leaving the nest, and hence went out of sight. Foraging distances (Table 3) varied

462 THE WILSON BULLETIN - Vol. 92, No. 4, December 1980 TABLE 3 DISTANCES (M) FROM THE NEST THAT MALE AND FEMALE CLAY-COLORED SPARROWS FLEW TO FORAGE Pair no. No. forqjng trips i + SD 1 26 46.2 k 18.6 2 41 89.9 ~fr 15.6 3 38 56.2? 6.8 4 29 75.3? 12.4 5 30 32.5 k 6.0 6 18 94.7 + 8.1 21 29.9 + 20.1 32 78.9 2 13.6 34 51.6 k 5.8 32 67.5 8.8 31 31.1 k 5.4 22 77.3 h 20.2 between pairs, and this was partially due to the location of the territory. Pairs 1 and 5, holding territories on the periphery of the nesting habitat, flew shorter distances, presumably because foraging locations were closer than the other 4 pairs, which held territories in the middle of the nesting habitat. The mean distances from the nest to the foraging areas were compared between mates for each direction flown (Fig. l), and were found to be significantly different for all pairs combined (G = 13.28, P < 0.005 [goodness-of-fit test, Sokal and Rohlf 19691). Within most pairs, the male flew farther, on average, from the nest to the foraging locations than the female. This was particularly evident when the pair left the nest together; the female alighted first and closer to the nesting area than the male, who flew farther on and alighted. This suggests that there is spatial partitioning of the foraging areas between members of a pair. DISCUSSION Clay-colored Sparrows presented a wide variety of prey items to their young, and they collected the prey in a variety of foraging locations off territory. Different pairs brought different prey items in different proportions to their young. A change in proportion of prey items in the nestling diet as the young grow older has been indicated in some passerines (Betts 1955, Royama 1970, Robins 1971, Best 1977). In the Clay-colored Sparrow, similar-aged nestlings did not receive the same foods. For example, the oldest nestlings in nests 1, 4 and 6 were about the same age (6 days old), yet orthopteran nymphs were more prominent in pair 6 s prey items, whereas lepidopteran larvae predominated in the other two. Moreover, the oldest nestlings in nests 2, 5 and 7 were about 5 days old; Odonata were the chief food at

Knapton. CLAY-COLORED SPARROW FORAGING 463 100 w 80 d z W LL I E 6. z Z 3 g 40 Z 4 < 20 C 20 40 60 80 100 MEAN DISTANCE (m)- MALES FIG. 1. Comparison of foraging distances between males and females in 6 pairs of Claycolored Sparrows in 1976. Each point plots the mean distance flown in 1 direction by a male against that flown by his mate. Number of directions flown by each pair ranged from 2-6. Points lying below the line (slope of 1) indicate that males flew farther; points above the line that females flew farther. nest 5 and lepidopteran larvae at the other two. This suggests that the birds were not selectively choosing prey according to age of the nestlings. However, nestlings of pair 3, the youngest nestlings under observation at about 3 days old, were the only ones to receive spiders, which is consistent with Royama s (1970) suggestion that spiders have special nutritional value important for nestling growth at an early age. Males and females within a pair did not differ either in type or proportion of prey items presented to their young (Table 2). Hence, the sexes did not

464 THE WILSON BULLETIN - Vol. 92, No. 4, December 1980 appear to select different food items in order to reduce intersexual competition. This is perhaps not surprising as the sparrows are essentially monomorphic in culmen depth and length, and in length of tarsometatarsus (Knapton 197833). Although males and females used essentially the same types of food, they appeared to spatially partition the food gathering areas. Within a pair, the male, on average, foraged at a slightly greater distance from the nest than the female, a tendency reported in other passerines (Morse 1968, Wiens 1969, Robins 1971, Busby 1978). In the Claycolored Sparrow, it is not known if this is a mechanism to reduce intersexual competition for food, or is a result of a greater tendency in females to be more nest oriented than males. SUMMARY Feeding ecology of the Clay-colored Sparrow was studied in 2 banded populations in southwestern Manitoba. Adults brought to their young a wide variety of prey items which they collected off territory and away from the breeding habitat. There were large differences between pairs in proportions of prey items collected, and each pair was probably exploiting whatever suitable foods were seasonally available. There were no major differences between members of a pair in food items brought to the young, or in the direction of foraging flights. However, within each pair, the male foraged at greater distances from the nest than the female, suggesting spatial partitioning of foraging areas. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I thank Roger M. Evans for his advice and constructive criticisms during the study. Jon C. Barlow, Peter Lowther, Spenser G. Sealy and Robert C. Whitmore offered helpful comments, and Kenneth DeSmet, David Duncan and David Ross gave valuable assistance in the field. My study was supported by National Research Council grants to Roger M. Evans and by University of Manitoba fellowships to myself. LITERATURE CITED BENT, A. C. 1968. Life histories of North American cardinals, grosbeaks, buntings, towhees, finches, sparrows, and their allies. U.S. Natl. Mus. Bull. 237. BEST, L. B. 1977. Nestling biology of the Field Sparrow. Auk 94:308319. BETTS, M. M. 1955. The food of titmice in oak woodland. J. Anim. Ecol. 24:282-323. BUSBY, D. G. 1978. Feeding ecology of a dense population of nesting Yellow Warblers. M.Sc. thesis, Univ. Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba. ERSKINE, A. J. 1977. The first ten years of the co-operative breeding bird survey in Canada. Can. Wildl. Serv. Rep. 42. HORN, H. S. 1966. Measurement of overlap in comparative ecological studies. Am. Nat. 100:419424. KNAPTON, R. W. 1978a. Behavioural ecology of the Clay-colored Sparrow, Spizella pallida. Ph.D. thesis, Univ. Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba. -. 1978b. Sex and age determination in the Clay-colored Sparrow. Bird-Banding 49:152-156. -. 1979. Birds of the Gainsborough-Lyleton Region (Saskatchewan and Manitoba). Sask. Nat. Hist. Sot. Spec. Publ. 10.

Knapton * CLAY-COLORED SPARROW FORAGING 465 MORSE, D. H. 1968. A quantitative study of foraging male and female spruce-woods warblers. Ecology 49:779-784. ORIANS, G. H. 1966. Food of nestling Yellow-headed Blackbirds, Cariboo Parklands, British Columbia. Condor 68:321-337. ROBINS, J. D. 1971. Differential niche utilization in a grassland sparrow. Ecology 52:1865-1070. ROYAMA, T. 1970. Factors governing the hunting behaviour and selection of food by the Great Tit (Parus major). J. Anim. Ecol. 39:619-668. SOKAL, R. R. AND F. J. ROHLF. 1969. Biometry. Freeman, New York, New York. WIENS, J. A. 1969. An approach to the study of the ecological relationships among grassland birds. Omithol. Monogr. 8. DEPT. ZOOLOGY, UNIV. MANITOBA, WINNIPEG, MANITOBA, CANADA. (PRES- ENT ADDRESS: DEPT. ZOOLOGY, UNIV. TORONTO, TORONTO, ONTARIO M5S 1A1, CANADA.) ACCEPTED 13 NOV. 1979. WOS ANNUAL MEETING The annual meeting of The Wilson Ornithological Sackville, New Brunswick, from 4 to 7 June 1981. Society will be held in NOTICE OF A VOTE TO CHANGE THE BYLAWS The membership is herewith notified of proposed changes in the Bylaws which will be voted on at the annual business meeting as follow: Article 2, Section 2, second sentence Nominations and applications for membership shall be made through the Secretary and last sentence Nominations presented in the interim between the annual meetings shall be re- ceived and confirmed by the Secretary,... the word Secretary to be changed to Treasurer. These changes will bring our Bylaws into conformance with practices and procedural necessity.