A QUANTITATIVE ETHOGRAM OF BEHAVIOR OF YEARLING EWES DURING TWO HOURS POST-PARTURITION

Similar documents
Lambing Time at Lower Winskill

A COMPARISON OF LAMB SURVIVAL IN FOX PROOF AND UNPROTECTED ENCLOSURES T. L. J. MANN*

(a) Energy reserves (b) Energy expenditure

Increasing Productivity of Triplet Lambs

{Received 21st August 1964)

PREDICTION OF LAMBING DATE BASED ON CLINICAL EXAMINATION PRIOR TO PARTURITION IN EWES

The effect of maternal nutrition during mid- to latepregnancy on ewe and lamb behaviour and the association with lamb survival

AUTUMN AND SPRING-LAMBING OF MERINO EWES IN SOUTH-WESTERN VICTORIA

Lifetime Production Performance by Suffolk x Rambouillet Ewes in Northwestern Kansas

Selection of Time of Mating of Merino Ewes

ANS 490-A: Ewe Lamb stemperament and Effects on Maze Entry, Exit Order and Coping Styles When Exposed to Novel Stimulus

OPP ERADICATION UPDATE. Cindy Wolf DVM Co-chair ASI Animal Health Committee

Pred-X Field Test Results

REDUCING LOSSES AND DISEASE LEVELS IN SHEEP. by Richard Bristol1. Veterinary Medicine and Sheep

LIFETIME PRODUCTION OF 1/4 AND 1/2 FINNSHEEP EWES FROM RAMBOUILLET, TARGHEE AND COLUMBIA DAMS AS AFFECTED BY NATURAL ATTRITION ABSTRACT

Ten Types of Animal Behavior Group 2 - Maternal Behavior Taken from Scientific Farm Animal Production, Robert Taylor

FLOCK CALENDAR OUTLINE. a. Be sure they are vigorous, healthy and in good breeding condition.

Improving lamb survival with calmer sheep

Guide To Having Kittens

Effects of ewe age and season of lambing on proli cacy in US Targhee, Suffolk, and Polypay sheep

Selection of Sheep. Table Ewe (Maternal) Breeds. Characteristics. White face, ears, and legs Acceptable carcass qualities

Flocks and Foliage Can Tree planning improve productivity, profit, health and welfare on livestock farms? Lovatt and Gascoigne 2016

Sheep Breeding. Genetic improvement in a flock depends. Heritability, EBVs, EPDs and the NSIP Debra K. Aaron, Animal and Food Sciences

Physical and social environment for sheep

RELATIONSHIPS AMONG WEIGHTS AND CALVING PERFORMANCE OF HEIFERS IN A HERD OF UNSELECTED CATTLE

Best Practices for Managing Awassi Sheep. Sheep Selection 6

WHEN you first look at a dairy cow, what do

Overview of some of the latest development and new achievement of rabbit science research in the E.U.

Selecting Foundation and Replacement Goats

Breeding Performance of Purebred vs. Crossbred Hampshire and Suffolk Ramsl. David L. Thomas, Debi J. Stritzke and John E. Fields.

1 of 9 7/1/10 2:08 PM

GENETICS OF MATERNAL ABILITY IN CATTLE AND SHEEP

7. IMPROVING LAMB SURVIVAL

FEEDING EWES BETTER FOR INCREASED PRODUCTION AND PROFIT. Dr. Dan Morrical Department of Animal Science Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa

The Effect of Ram Exposure on Uterine Involution and Luteal Function During the Postpartum Period of Hair Sheep Ewes in the Tropics 1

Sexual activity and body and testis growth in prepubertal ram lambs of Friesland, Chios, Karagouniki and Serres dairy sheep in Greece

EAAP 2010 Annual Meeting Session 43, Paper #2 Breeding and Recording Strategies in Small Ruminants in the U.S.A.

Pig Handling & Behaviour Lecture 2 ANS101/Vet

University of Wyoming, Laramie

THE EFFECT OF IBR/PI3 AND PASTEURELLA VACCINATION ON THE MORTALITY RATE OF HIGH PERCENTAGE EAST FRIESIAN LAMBS

Having Puppies. Pregnancy Pregnancy normally lasts 9 weeks (63 days) but puppies may be delivered between 58 and 68 days.

Human-Animal Interactions in the Turkey Industry

CARLISLE HOUSE, TOWNHEAD ROAD, DALSTON, CARLISLE, CUMBRIA, CA5 7JF TEL: (01228) FAX: (01228)

Understanding Postpartum Anestrus and Puberty

Course: Principles of AFNR. Unit Title: Sheep Selection TEKS: (C)(12)(D) Instructor: Ms. Hutchinson. Objectives:

Adjustment Factors in NSIP 1

MOTHER-OFFSPRING RELATIONSHIPS IN SCOTTISH BLACKFACE SHEEP

AGE OF ONSET OF PUBERTY IN MERINO EWES IN SEMI-ARID TROPICAL QUEENSLAND

7. Flock book and computer registration and selection

PRACTICAL APPLICATION OF ARTIFICIAL INSEMINATION IN CONJUNCTION WITH SYNCHRONIZATION OF HEAT CYCLE IN THE EWE

Evaluation of Columbia, USMARC- Composite, Suffolk, and Texel Rams as Terminal Sires in an Extensive Rangeland Production System

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN GROWTH OF SUFFOLK RAMS ON CENTRAL PERFORMANCE TEST AND GROWTH OF THEIR PROGENY

Keeping and Using Flock Performance Records Debra K. Aaron, Animal and Food Sciences

FREQUENCY OF ENGAGEMENT WITH DIFFERENT MATERIALS BY GROWING RABBITS

Crossbreeding to Improve Productivity ASI Young Entrepreneur Meeting. David R. Notter Department of Animal and Poultry Sciences Virginia Tech

List important areas to think about when selecting sheep; Describe what to look for in structural correctness; Explain why we need a structurally

Planning Spring/Summer 2018

PREGNANT FEMALES GATHER IN A MATERNITY GROUP, ATTRACTED BY MOTHERS AND PUPS ALREADY BORN

INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR

The Effect of Ewe Body Condition at Lambing on Colostral Immunoglobulin G Concentration and Lamb Performance 1,2

Surveillance of animal brucellosis

Objectives. ERTs for the New Beef Industry. Ancient History. The EPD we produce entirely depends on the tools we have to use them.

Age of Weaning Lambs

HEAT STRESS EFFECTS ON FETAL DEVELOPMENT DURING LATE GESTATION IN THE EWE 1,2. University of Illinois, Urbana, 61801

Does repeated regrouping alter the social behaviour of heifers?

Genetic approaches to improving lamb survival under extensive field conditions

SHLEEP RESEARCH. --- ~I ~~~_ rrrrr~s~ 1954 to /969 ..~ Agricultural Experiment Station AUBURN UNIVERSITY X10,4 ~ BULLETIN 394 OCTOBER 1969

Provision of additional walls in the resting area the effects on. resting behaviour and social interactions in goats

LOCAL AND GENERAL ANESTHETIC EFFECTS ON BEHAVIOR AND PERFORMANCE OF TWO- AND SEVEN-WEEK-OLD CASTRATED AND UNCASTRATED PIGLETS

TREATMENT OF ANOESTRUS IN DAIRY CATTLE R. W. HEWETSON*

Drexel University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee Mouse Breeding Policy

Experiences with NSIP in the Virginia Tech Flocks Scott P. Greiner, Ph.D. Extension Animal Scientist, Virginia Tech

Managing Perinatal Mortality in Lambs

GROWTH OF LAMBS IN A SEMI-ARID REGION AS INFLUENCED BY DISTANCE WALKED TO WATER

Meadowood Farms 2017 Ram Lambs for sale

BORDER LEICESTER AND FINNSHEEP CROSSES. I. SURVIVAL, GROWTH AND CARCASS TRAITS OF FI LAMBS 1

4-H Swine Bowl Learning Information

Keeping and Using Flock Records Scott P. Greiner, Ph.D. Extension Animal Scientist, Virginia Tech

Genetic approaches to improving lamb survival

Why choose sheep? American Merino

EFFECT OF PADDOCK SIZE, STOCKING RATE, ANTHELMINTICS, AND TRACE ELEMENTS ON THE WEIGHT GAIN OF YOUNG CATTLE

LAMB GROWTH AND EWE PRODUCTION FOLLOWING ANTHELMINTIC DRENCHING BEFORE AND AFTER LAMBING

Proceedings of AVA Annual Conference, Adelaide, Collins, T - The effect of shearing on sheep feeding and behaviour

Comparison of Weigh-Suckle-Weigh and Machine Measuring Ewe Milk Production 1,2

EverGraze: pastures to improve lamb weaning weights

SHOW LAMB SELECTION. Darrell Rothlisberger Rich County Agent Utah State University Extension

The South African National Small Stock Improvement Scheme

Key Information. Mountain Hill Vs Lowland Production. Breeding Strategy

3.9 Fencing. Figure 9 - Concrete waterer with float valve. The length is variable.

Effect of environmental enrichment and breed line on the incidence of belly nosing in piglets weaned at 7 and 14 days-of-age

Mastitis in ewes: towards development of a prevention and treatment plan

Redacted for privacy

GENETIC AND NON GENETIC FACTORS AFFECTING THE LITTER TRAITS OF BROILER RABBITS*

P VASANTA KUMARI and JAMIL AHMAD KHAN Department of Zoology, Aligarh Muslim University, Aligarh

Anestrus and Estrous Detection Aids

EBA Series FOOTHILL ABORTION UPDATE: PART I: THE TICK

Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens

After Fall Breeding some proven rams and bred ewes may be available for sale. Contact if interested.

11 Genetic and Environmental Impacts on Prenatal Loss H.H. Meyer

RAT GRIMACE SCALE (RGS): THE MANUAL

Transcription:

Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 16 (1986) 157--164 157 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., Amsterdam -- Printed in The Netherlands A QUANTITATIVE ETHOGRAM OF BEHAVIOR OF YEARLING EWES DURING TWO HOURS POST-PARTURITION JOHN J. MCGLONE 1 AND ROBERT H. STOBART Department of Animal Science, University of Wyoming, Laramie, WY 82071 (U.S.A.) (Accepted for publication 28 November 1985) ABSTRACT McGlone, J.J. and Stobart, R.H., 1986. A quantitative ethogram of behavior of yearling ewes during two hours post-parturition. Appl. Anita. Behav. Sci., 16: 157--164. A total of 2130 behaviors representing 24 behavioral categories were observed during the period from parturition to limb nursing for seven ewe--limb pairs. Both individual behaviors and behavioral sequences were counted. Ewes showed a high frequency of licking, especially directed at the head (41% of licks) and anterior body (30% of licks) of their lambs. Licking followed a specific, non-random pattern. Ewes licked from front to back of their lambs. Ewes licked their lamb's anterior body and head immediately before their lambs stood. When the Inrnbs sought teats, ewes either presented their udder to their lamb which facilitated nursing, or moved away from the lamb which delayed nursing. This research identified behaviors and behavioral sequences that may be used in future studies seeking to select for improved maternal ability or to design improved lambing environments. Finally, this work may lead to the generation of hypotheses which may be tested in future work examining behaviors involved as causal factors in lamb survival. INTRODUCTION Failure of lambs to survive to weaning is a major problem under range conditions. In the United States, pre-weaning death losses constitute 20--25% of the!~rnbs born (Safford and Hoversland, 1960) and 70% of this loss occurs during the first 5 days post-partum. In Australia, the highest mortality rates also occur within the first few days after birth (Alexander et al., 1959; Alexander and Peterson, 1961; Alexander, 1964; Dennis, 1965). Failure to establish maternal--neonate bonds is a major factor involved. For example, in a study in south-western Wyoming conducted over a 3-year period (Faulkner and Tigner, 1977), 54% of the lambs were lost due to failure of the ewe to accept the lamb or a failure to produce enough milk to allow for lamb survival. ~Author to whom correspondence should be addressed: Department Animal Science, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409, U.S.A. 0168-1591/86/$03.50 1986 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.

158 A better understanding of maternal--young interactions is necessary for more efficient selection of behaviors to improve maternal bonding. Also, a better understanding of these processes may lead to improved management practices during this critical bonding period. The objective of this study was to identify behaviors and sequences of behavior shown by yearling ewes and their lambs during the period from birth to the first successful suckling. METHODS Behavioral data were collected on seven primiparous yearling ewes at the United States Department of Agriculture's Sheep Experiment Station at Dubois, Idaho. Ewes were crossbred, predominantly from Finnish Landrace and Rambouillet crossbred parents. The air temperature during the observations ranged from 9 to 20 C, and days were sunny with light winds. Ewes were observed on 7 May 1984, between 10.15 and 19.15 h. Two lambing pens contained 150 expectant ewes each. Behavior was recorded on videotape. Each video-camera was equipped with a zoom lens so that the cameramen could remain outside of the ewe's flight distance of less than 10 m. The ewes were acclimated to humans, since shepherds remained near them during most of each 24-h day. Video-cameras and recorders filmed ewe and lamb behavior from when each ewe stood after parturition until the lambs suckled for the first time. Computer programs used to identify and count sequences of behavior were previously described (McGlone et al., 1985). The analysis of behavioral sequences followed standard procedure as described by Slater (1973). This analysis identified if behavioral sequences were found at a frequency that was greater or less than that expected by chance. Behavioral categories used in the final analysis and their definitions are given in Table I. Behavioral categories were mutually exclusive and a behavior could not follow itself (e.g. ELKHD, ELKHD could not occur in sequence). Although the final analysis utilized 24 behavioral categories, initial data identified the possibility of 33 behavioral categories. Behavioral categories not found at greater than 0.1 of 1% were eliminated; these categories included: ewe pushes back half of lamb's body; ewe stands up; ewe lies down; ewe visually orients toward her lamb; lamb touches ewe while both are lying; other ewe butts lamb; other lamb touches ewe; other ewe steps away from lamb to stop lamb investigation; lamb nurses other ewe. All of these eliminated behaviors could occur, but they were not observed except at very low frequencies during the time from parturition to the first nursing bout. Ewe investigation of placenta and uterine fluids could not be identified on the video records because membranes and fluid color blended with ground colors.

159 TABLE I Description of behavioral categories and mean frequencies observed from seven ewes Behavior Mean Minimum Maximum Description code ELKHD 50.9 12 93 Ewe licks head (licks include nose-touches) ELKFR 37.9 2 78 Ewe licks front legs or shoulder ELKBK 18.7 3 43 Ewe licks hind legs or posterior back (not rump) ELKRP 5.1 0 13 Ewe licks rump ELKUN 13.0 0 40 Ewe licks underside EPUFR 3.4 0 10 Ewe pushes or moves toward front half of lamb's body and lamb moves LSTANDS 26.9 16 53 Lamb stands up LDOWN 25.6 15 50 Lamb lies or falls down LTOEST 33.6 13 101 Lamb touches ewe while both stand (possible teat-searching) EAWAY 17.9 0 63 Ewe stands away as lamb touched (probably teatsearching) ESTEP 21.3 4 78 Ewe turns away or steps away from lamb ELAPART 2 0 6 Ewe and lamb move apart more than two body lengths LMOAWAY 1.9 0 9 Lambs moves away from ewe EPAWS 4.3 0 14 Ewe paws at bedding or ground with a front foot EINGR 9.7 0 36 Ewe investigates ground or bedding LFOLLOWS 2.7 0 11 Lambs follow ewe EEATS 3.1 0 22 Ewe eats food EPRUD 2.3 0 7 Ewe presents udder to lamb OETOL 8.0 0 21 Other ewe touches lamb (interference) LINOE 3.4 0 11 Lamb investigates other ewe EBLIN 2.1 0 6 Ewe blocks interference (by other ewe) by stepping between lamb and other ewe or by butting ETOOL 1.1 0 7 Ewe touches other lamb LNURSES 1.0 1 1 Lamb nurses (lamb in correct area, down in front, tail wagging) HUINORBL 8.4 1 18 Human interference or animal blocked view Grand mean 304.3

160 RESULTS A total of 2130 behaviors belonging to 24 behavioral categories were present in the repertoire of the seven ewes observed interacting with their lambs (Table I). On average, the ewes and lambs showed 304 behaviors prior to suckling. All lambs reported here suckled within 2 h of parturition. The most common behaviors shown by the ewes were licking their lambs. Most licking by the ewe was directed toward the lamb's head (frequency = 356, 41% of licks) followed by licking the anterior body (frequency = 265, 30%). Licks to the lamb's back (frequency = 131, 15% of licks), underside (frequency = 91, 10% of licks) and rump (frequency = 36, 4% of licks) were less common. TABLE II Behavioral sequences and observed frequencies from seven ewes First Behavior Second behavior N N N N N ELKHD -- 7* 87* 4* 1" 6 17 6 1 4 32 2 ELKUN 4* -- 8 6 1 4* 0 4* 0 0 39* 3* ELKFR 77* 7 -- 42* 2 2 12 2 0 5 22 3 ELKBK 11" 4 29* -- 8* 1 9* 1 2 2 15 2 ELKRP 1" 8* 0* 6* -- 0 2 0 0 0 8* 3* EPUFR 6 0 6 1 0 -- 0 2* 0 0 2 0 EINGR 24* 0 15" 4 0 0 -- 0 1 7* 0 0 EPAWS 7 3 4 3 0 3* 1 -- 0 0 5 0 EPRUD 0 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 -- 0 6* 0 EEATS 3 0 4 5* 0 0 0 0 0 -- 0 0 LTOEST 14" 27* 12" 19 5 1 4 2 8* 0 -- 0 LMOAWAY 3 0 0 1 4* 0 0 0 0 0 3 -- OETOL 13 4 7 2 2 0 0 0 0 0 3 0 ELAPART 3 0 2 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 LSTANDS 20* 3 6* 2* 3 1 1" 1 1 0 26 0 LDOWN 62* 0* 35* 8 2 1 5 0 2 3 2* 0 LNURSES 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 ETOOL 3 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 HUINORBL 14 4 6 6 0 0 0 1 0 0 12" 0 EAWAY 15 15" 11 6 4 3 1 4 0 0 51" 0 LINOE 1 3 + 3 3 4* 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 EBLIN 6* 3* 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 ESTEP 62* 2 + 24 10 0 1 15" 7* 1 1 3* 0 LFOLLOW 6 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 4 0 START 1 0 2 0 0 1" 0 0 0 0 0 0 *Large x 2 value (P < 0.05;1 df). +Marginal x 2 value (P < 0.10, 1 df).

161 After licking, ewes showed a high frequency of moving away as the lamb approached (EAWAY and ESTEP, Table I). Ewes showed low incidence of pawing and investigating the ground (EPAWS and EINGR), eating (EEATS), blocking interference by other ewes (EBLIN) and touching other lambs (ETOOL). The most frequent behaviors observed for lambs were attempts to stand up (LSTAND) and touching the ewe (LTOEST) in search of a teat. Also, lambs frequently fell down (LDOWN). Behaviors shown by lambs in low frequency were lamb moving away from the ewe (LMOAWAY), lamb follows the ewe (LFOLLOW) and lamb investigates another ewe (LINOE). Of course, the seven lambs only nursed a total of seven times since this was the criterion used to end the observations.!- 9 1 68* 16" 0 1 5 2 2 3 76* 6 0 6* 0 0 4 0 1 4 2 5* 0 0* 0 0 9 1 33* 5* 1 1 8 4* 3 1 25 0 0 5 0 7 3* 0 3* 7 4 2 1 14 1 0 2 0 2 0 0 0 2 0 2* 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 4 0 0 1 0 0 1" 0 0 0 0 0 5 1" 0 0 0 1 0 0 9 + 1 0 0 0 3 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 -- 0 0 0 1 1 2* 0 0 2 0 0 0 1" 0 0 0 5* 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 3 0 0 2 0 0 19 4* 0 3 109" 0 0 4* 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 2* 0 0 0 0 -- 0 5 1 0 1 7* 0 3* 6* 2 0 0 0 -- 4* 1 0 1" 0 0 0 0 0 2* 0 3 1 -- 111" 0 0 2 0 1 0 4* 2 0 7 3 35* -- 0 0 6 0 0 0 7 1 0 0 0 0 0 -- 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 7* 2* 0 1 0 0 -- 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 4* 0 5 3 0 0 -- 0 2 0 2 0 0 1 0 0* 6 0 0 7 -- 1 0 0 0 0 4* 0 0 1 0 0 2 0 -- 2* 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1" -- 0 0. 0 1 5* 9 1 0 0 3 0 0* 1 -- 3 0 0 2* 3 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 -- 0 0 1" 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 2* 0 0

162 Two-behavior sequences are presented in Table II. Ewes displayed a sequence of licking lamb body regions that was specific and apparently nonrandom. Ewes licked the lamb's head and front part of its body (ELKHD -~ ELKFR, ELKFR -* ELKHD). Ewes also showed a high frequency of licking the lamb's front and back body regions (ELKFR -~ ELKBK, ELKBK -~ ELKFR). Also, ewes licked the lamb's back body and its rump in sequence (ELKBK -* ELKRP, ELKRP -~ ELKBK). Other licking patterns were found at a low frequency (ELKHD -* ELKUN, ELKUN -* ELKHD, ELKHD -* ELKRP, ELKRP -* ELKHD, ELKHD -~ ELKBK}. In all, ewe licking patterns followed a consistent order. Ewes licked from the head to the rear of the lambs' bodies and then started at the head or shoulders again. Lambs were born with fluids and membranes covering their bodies. The ewes began licking, and the first activity seen by the lambs was an attempt to stand up. Data in Table II are structured to identify which ewe behaviors immediately precede lamb standing. The sequences ELKHD -~ LSTANDS and ELKFR -~ LSTANDS were found at a high frequency. Thus, ewes licking the front part of their lamb may be a stimulus to stand up. Lambs were observed to stand then fall and fall then stand 146 times during the observation period (LSTANDS -~ LDOWN, LDOWN -* LSTANDS). After the lambs stood up, they began teat-seeking activities. The particular body parts of the ewes that the lamb investigated were not identified. The behavior LTOEST is largely (but not exclusively) teat seeking. After the lamb began LTOEST, the ewe licked the lamb's head, front body parts and underside. Also, several ewes (but not all) presented their udder (i.e. they turned their body to place their udder near the lamb's head) after the lambs began teat-seeking. After the lambs began LTOEST, there was a high frequency of the ewe stepping away (LTOEST -* EAWAY). These behaviors resulted in lambs not being able to locate a teat. However, lambs continued pursuit of a teat, as evidenced by a high frequency of the sequence EAWAY -~ LTOEST. After the ewes stepped away from lambs (EAWAY), if the lamb did not continue LTOEST, ewes licked the lamb's underside (ELKUN). Several ewes showed rudiments of apparent aggressive behavior toward their lambs when they pawed the ground (EPAW). EPAW was followed and preceded by the ewe pushing on the front part of the lamb's body. Since EPAW also was preceded by licking (ex ELKUN), this pawing behavior may not represent aggression. After other ewes interfered with the focal ewe's lamb (OETOL), either the focal ewe blocked this interference (EBLIN) or the lamb investigated the other ewe (LINOE). Ewes appeared to feed at the feed bunk (EEAT) infrequently. As logic would predict, EEAT was preceded by the ewe investigating the ground (EINGR). EINGR was present in sequence with licking the lamb's head and front region. Video-cameras were not positioned to show whether the placenta or placental fluids were the point of investigation on the ground.

163 DISCUSSION We must know which behaviors or sequences of behaviors are exhibited by those ewes that have high mothering ability. Behavioral data from this study indicate that licking the lamb was the primary ewe behavior observed. Bareham (1976) found a similar high frequency of ewe licking, primarily directed towards the lamb's front end prior to the lamb standing. Licking of the lambs immediately after birth may play several important roles. In addition to thermal benefits, our data suggest tactile stimulation of the anterior body encourages the lamb to stand. Future studies may investigate the relationship between vigorous head--shoulder licking by the ewe and lamb viability. Perhaps ewes that lick vigorously cause quicker lamb-standing and nursing. Yearling ewes moved away from their lambs at a high frequency. This may indicate these yearling ewes were unfamiliar and somewhat uneasy with behaviors their lambs exhibited. Lambs moved away from the ewe (LMOAWAY) after and before the ewe licked the lamb's rump (ELKRP), However, although these sequences occurred, the frequencies were low and probably not biologically important. After standing, the lambs sought a teat. Teat-seeking was facilitated by those ewes that presented their udder to their lamb. Nursing was prevented or delayed by ewes stepping or moving away from their lamb. This prevention of nursing may have been beneficial, since it gave the lambs time to stand firmly without wobbling. However, ewe-stepping-away certainly delayed nursing. Any delay of nursing would be detrimental in cold weather. ACKNOWLEDGEMENT The authors wish to thank Duane Keisler and the staff of the Sheep Experiment Station at Dubois, Idaho, for help in data collection. Charles Parker suggested and encouraged this project. University of Wyoming Paper No. JA 1334. REFERENCES Alexander, G., 1964. Lamb survival: physiological considerations. Proc. Austr. Soc. Anita. Prod., 5: 113--122. Alexander, G. and Peterson, J.E., 1961. Neonatal mortality in lambs. Austr. Vet. J., 37: 371--381. Alexander, G., Peterson, J.E. and Watson, R.H., 1959. Neonatal mortality in lambs: intensive observations during lambing in a Corriedale flock with a history of high lamb mortality. Aust. Vet. J., 35: 433--441. Bareham, J.R., 1976. The behaviour of lambs on the first day after birth. Br. Vet. J., 132: 152--161. Dennis, S.M., 1965. More light on lamb losses. J.Agric. W. Aust., 6: 686--689. Faulkner, E.K. and Tigner, J.R., 1977. Birth rates of sheep from range operations in Carbon County, Wyoming. Univ. Wyoming Agric. Exp. Stn. Bull., 643.

164 McGlone, J.J., Miller, E.A. and Hayden, S.L., 1985. A description of micro- and mainframe-computer programs to summarize frequency, duration and sequences of behavior. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 13: 219--226. Safford, J.W. and Hoversland, A.S., 1960. A study of lamb mortality in a western range flock. J. Anim. Sci., 19: 265--273. Slater, P.J.B., 1973. Describing sequences of behavior. In: P.G. Batesson and P.H. Klopfer (Editors), Perspective in Ethology. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 131--153.