Department of Zoology, University of California Berkeley, California 94720, U.S.A. INTRODUC'IO N

Similar documents
Doug Scull s SCIENCE & NATURE

FIRST REARING OF THE DICTYNID SPIDER SPECIES, Nigma conducens (O. PICKARD-CAMBRIDGE, 1876) IN EGYPT

M.P.Chikhale,* G.B.Santape* and A.K.Bodkhe** *Rajarshree Shahu Science College, Chandur Rly, Dist.Amravati

What do we do when the butterfly larvae arrive? How can we tell how much the larvae have grown?

Change in the signal-response sequence responsible for

Anatomy of a Swarm. What I Learned from Honeybee Democracy. by Dr. Thomas Seeley. Marja E van den Hende 1

Honey Bees. Anatomy and Function 9/26/17. Similar but Different. Honey Bee External Anatomy. Thorax (Human Chest): 4 Wings & 6 Legs

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

Supporting Online Material for

~!.. RIASSUNTO. Parole chiave: Araneidae, Alimentazione, Predazione, Nidi di riposo, Riparo, Homing, Laboratorio. SUMMARY

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

4. List 3 characteristics of all arthropods. a. b. c. 5. Name 3 main arthropod groups.

Discover the Path to Life with Your Dog. Beginner Obedience Manual 512-THE-DOGS

Under One Roof. Beehive Management During the Swarming Season in a single hive. By: - Nick Withers

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

TARANTULA. Araneae Family: Theraphosidae Genus: 113 genera

Silk feeding as an alternative foraging tactic in a kleptoparasitic spider under seasonally changing environments

HATCHING BEHAVIOR OF THE BOBWHITE

STANDARD OPERATING PROCEDURE

A NEW Plexippus SPIDER FROM THE WESTERN GHATS, KUMBAKARAI FALLS, THENI DISTRICT, TAMIL NADU, SOUTH INDIA (ARACHNIDA: ARANEAE: SALTICIDAE)

K9 Search and Rescue Team Inc. 35 Railroad Avenue, Dolores, CO, (970)

Animal Adaptations. Structure and Function

Laboratory 7 The Effect of Juvenile Hormone on Metamorphosis of the Fruit Fly (Drosophila melanogaster)

SEASONAL CHANGES IN A POPULATION OF DESERT HARVESTMEN, TRACHYRHINUS MARMORATUS (ARACHNIDA: OPILIONES), FROM WESTERN TEXAS

The Effect of Aerial Exposure Temperature on Balanus balanoides Feeding Behavior

NESTS AND NEST-SITE SELECTION OF THE CRAB SPIDE R. Douglass H. Morse

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL

INTERACTIONS BETWEEN THE SOCIAL SPIDER ANELOSIMUS STUDIOSUS (ARANEAE, THERIDIIDAE) AND FOREIGN SPIDERS THAT FREQUENT ITS NESTS

The Development of Behavior

P VASANTA KUMARI and JAMIL AHMAD KHAN Department of Zoology, Aligarh Muslim University, Aligarh

Amazing arthropods. Kindergarten-Second. Life Science TEKS. Life Science Vocabulary

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Rock Wren Nesting in an Artificial Rock Wall in Folsom, Sacramento County, California

FEAR-FREE HOSPITAL DESIGN GUIDELINE Heather E. Lewis, AIA, NCARB. Draft / January 2015 PREPARED BY. architecture animals people

Using Animals in Research at PSU

Egg laying site preferences in Pterostichus melanarius Illiger (Coleoptera: Carabidae)

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and

Sweet Corn Insect Management Update. Rick Foster Department of Entomology Purdue University

The effects of diet upon pupal development and cocoon formation by the cat flea (Siphonaptera: Pulicidae)

A.13 BLAINVILLE S HORNED LIZARD (PHRYNOSOMA BLAINVILLII)

Handler Interaction. Motivation Dos and Don ts

A NEW SALTICID SPIDER FROM VICTORIA By R. A. Dunn

Animals on University Property

SHEEP PREDATION BY COYOTES: A BEHAVIORAL ANALYSIS

Byall, C., H. M. Smith, and D. Chiszar Response of Brown Tree Snakes (Boiga

Insect vectors. Dr. Carmen E. Rexach Micro 1 Mt SAC Biology Department Internet version

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS

All You Ever Wanted to Know About Hornets and Yellowjackets

EVALUATION OF A METHOD FOR ESTIMATING THE LAYING RATE OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS

EnrichmEnt at. Objects for Enriching Bears. An animal welfare project by FOUR PAWS. Encouraging bears to perform species typical.

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar

Appendix from T. J. Ord and J. A. Stamps, Species Identity Cues in Animal Communication

Splits. Aiken Beekeepers Association July 2016 S. Siler

Echinoderms are marine animals with spiny endoskeletons, water-vascular systems, and tube feet; they have radial symmetry as adults.

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

AKC TRAINING. AKC Canine Good Citizen Training

How Turtle Cracked His Shell from the novel Things Fall Apart by Chinua Achebe

SWGDOG SC 9 - HUMAN SCENT DOGS Avalanche Search

Purple Martin. Adult male Purple Martin

I Thought Your Ankle Was a Mouse! Human-Directed Aggression in the Cat Sharon L. Crowell-Davis DVM, PhD, DACVB Professor of Behavioral Medicine

Common Core Lesson Plan. Title: The Tortoise, the Spider, and a Woman Spinning Gold

Animal Behavior. Problem Area: Animal Health and Administering Veterinary Care. Corresponding E-unit(s). Danville, IL: CAERT, Inc.

Egg: Shape, color, & texture vary by species

NZ Federation Clubs Newsletter

(135) OBSERVATIONS IN A ROOKERY DURING THE INCUBATION PERIOD C. M. OGILVIE.

An Owner s Manual for: 10 ESSENTIAL SKILLS: CGC TEST ITEMS. by the AMERICAN KENNEL CLUB

Name Class Date. After you read this section, you should be able to answer these questions:

CHICKEN LICENSE a Small-scale Chicken Flock

List of the Major Changes to CKC Agility for 2014

THE JAPANESE CRANE. endangered species L ARCHE PHOTOGRAPHIQUE CHARACTERISTICS

Top Ten Grape Insect Pests in Nebraska Chelsey M. Wasem and Frederick P. Baxendale Department of Entomology, University of Nebraska-Lincoln

Great Blue Heron Chick Development. Through the Stages

Distribution Unlimited

CHICKEN LICENSE a Small-scale Chicken Flock

DISPERSAL OF THE SPIDERLINGS O F XYSTICUS EMERTONI (ARANEAE, THOMISIDAE), A LITTER-DWELLING CRAB SPIDE R

PROJECT: EGGS OF ANT

Puppy Behavior and Training Handling and Food Bowl Exercises

Species must be adapted to their habitat.

Housing on the Fountainbridge site

ReproMatic & FluxxBreeder

Use of Agent Based Modeling in an Ecological Conservation Context

Genetic improvement For Alternative Hen-Housing

Behaviour of cats and dogs

Topic The traits of offspring are determined by genetic instructions received from the mother and the father.

Chick n Bees. Coops for Chickens

Dog Behavior Problems Veterinary Visits/Examinations

Ethology of Crayfish. Name. Lab partners: Page 1

ANIMAL COMMUNICATION

Crate Training. The great question of dog training is: To Crate or Not To Crate.

NHFG / VICTOR YOUNG PHOTO USFWS PHOTO

Record of Predation by Sugar Glider on Breeding Eastern Rosellas 33Km NE of Melbourne in November 2016

Accessories for Pigeon

Kennel Management: Cats Provide separate cat wards Feliway TM plug-in Through a Dog s Ear music Keep fearful cats on top cages Provide hiding options

PERCEPTION OF OCEAN WAVE DIRECTION BY SEA TURTLES

AN INVESTIGATION OF THE MECHANISMS UNDERLYING NEST CONSTRUCTION IN THE MUD WASP PARALASTOR SP. (HYMENOPTERA : EUMENIDAE)

Rick Klaus (360)

Separation Anxiety. Techniques to calm your dog

Steps Towards a Blanding s Turtle Recovery Plan in Illinois: status assessment and management

Transcription:

Jackson, R. R. 1981. Nest-mediated sexual discrimination by a jumping spider (Phidippus johnsoni). J. Arachnol., 9 :87-92. NEST-MEDIATED SEXUAL DISCRIMINATION BY A JUMPING SPIDER (PHIDIPPUS JOHNSONI ) Robert R. Jackson ) Department of Zoology, University of California Berkeley, California 94720, U.S.A. ABSTRACT Empty nests of conspecific females elicit courtship behavior from males of Phidippus johnsoni. Males discriminate between nests of adult males and adult females and also between nests of subadul t males and subadult females. INTRODUC'IO N Spiders are the major example in the animal kingdom of adaptive radiation in the us e of silk, and it is not surprising that communi4ation is found among its functions. The salticid spiders, however, are unlike most spidesls in having highly developed vision (Lan d 1972) on which they rely in most aspects of their behavior, and the communicator y behavior most commonly associated with spid rs of this family consists of dancing an d specialized movements and postures of the leg, pedipalpi, and abdomen (Crane 1949). Communication involving the use of silk in this family has received little attention. However, Bristowe (1958) noted that the males of various salticid species show excited "signs of awareness" when they touch unoccupied nests built by conspecific females. Males of Phidippus johnsoni Peckham and Peckham, a common species in wester n North America, employ visually mediated cour ship when they encounter adult female s in the presence of adequate light (Jackson 19'77 a,b, 1978 a) ; however, many salticids, including P. johnsoni, build silken nests under rocks, in hollow reeds, and in other dimly lit locations and use these for molting, ovipositing, and passing periods of inactivit y (Jackson 1979). If a male P. johnsoni encounters an adult female inside her nest, h e employs vibratory courtship which does not depend on vision. If the female inside the nest is a subadult, the male first courts, then spins a second chamber on the female's nest and cohabits with her until she matures (Jackson 1977). Males of various non-salticid spiders have been shown to begin courtship in response t o contact with draglines, webs, or nests and to make species- and sex-discriminations on th e basis of the silk after contact (e.g. Tietjen 1977, Jackson 1978b). This paper is concerned 'Present address : Department of Zoology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch 1, New Zealand.

88 THE JOURNAL OF ARACHNOLOG Y with similar adaptations related to communication and the use of silk in P. johnsoni. In an earlier study (Jackson 1976a) it was shown that male P. johnsoni respond to empty nests of adult females with elements of behavior that normally occur during courtshi p and that they discriminate between the nests of adult conspecific females and th e similarly sized nests of Herpyllus hesperolus (Chamberlin), a sympatric gnaphosid spider that is a potential predator of P. johnsoni. Two questions will be considered in this paper. Do adult males discriminate between the nests of adult females and adult males? Do adul t males discriminate between nests of subadult females and subadult males? METHOD S Spiders. Spiders were collected as immatures from Tilden Regional Park, Oakland, California. Each adult male and female was 5 to 30 days post-maturity at the time of th e tests. All females were virgins. None of the spiders had been involved in interactions wit h other spiders in the laboratory previous to the tests. Maintenance and Apparatus. Details of maintenance are provided elsewhere (Jackson 1974, 1976b). Each spider was kept individually in a transparent plastic cage (10 x 10 x 6.5 cm) with a ventilation hole covered by a metal screen and two 4.5-cm-diameter hole s plugged with corks (see Jackson 1978a, Fig. 1). Each spider spun at least one nes t fastened to one of the two corks, and the opposite cork-hole provided access to the interior of the cage without damaging the nest. Testing Procedure. All tests took place within the first 4 hr. after the lights came on in the laboratory (12L : 12D, lights on at 0900 hr ; temp., 24 C). Each spider was forced from its nest with a camel hair brush and removed from the cage 2 to 8 min before introduction of the test male. Any additional nests and any living flies in the cages wer e removed. All nests were relatively dense (see Jackson 1979) and were 7 to 30 days old. Time elapsing between introduction of the male and his first touching the nest wa s recorded. A description of his behavior for 30 min following contact was recorded on a tape recorder with a metronome providing a time base (one beat per sec). The nest used in each test came from a different spider. Each male was tested with on e nest on one day and on the following day with a different one. No male was tested with more than one pair of nests. One nest was from a female and the other from a male, each either adult or subadult. One half of the males were tested on the first day with nests of females; the other half, first with nests of males. Twelve males were tested with nests o f adults and 18 with nests of subadults. A random numbers table (Rohlf and Sokal 1969) was used to assign males to group s and particular nests to particular males. No spider provided nests for more than one test. Some of the males were used both for testing and for nest production, but they wer e never tested with their own nests. Occurrences of behavior in the pairs of tests were compared using McNemar tests wit h Yates correction ; latencies and durations were compared using two-tailed Wilcoxo n signed-ranks tests (Sokal and Rohlf 1969). Latencies were compared only for those male s that performed the behavior in question during both tests. Data in text are means ± S.D's when normally distributed; otherwise, medians followed by maxima are provided. Elements of Behavior. The behavior of males in the presence of occupied nests ha s been described elsewhere (Jackson 1977a), and similar behavior occurred at empty nests. Brief descriptions of the important behaviors are provided here. Twitch abdomen. Rapid, low amplitude, up and down movements of the abdomen.

JACKSON NEST DISCRIMINATION BY SALTICID 89 Table l. Number of males of Phidippus johnsoni that performed different elements of behavior after contact with empty nests. A: nests of adults. S : nests of subadults. Nests at which Behavior was Performe d Behavior Female only Male only Both Neither Abdomen Twitch A 7 0 3 2 S 14 0 1 3 Vibrate A 5 0 2 5 S 5 1 0 1 2 Tug A 2 5 3 2 S 9 3 2 4 Probe A 0 0 12 0 S 3 0 15 0 Enter Nest A 3 0 9 0 S 1 1 16 0 Probe. Pushing and pulling on the nest with the first pair of legs. Tug. Up and down movements of the cephalothorax while the Qhelicerae grip the silk. Vibrate. Very rapid, low amplitude, up and down movements with the forelegs on th e silk. In other studies (Jackson 1977a) twitching of abdomen, probing and vibrating were characteristic of interactions between conspecific spiders. Tugging occurred sometime s while spiders were alone and spinning in their own nests, but it was more prevalent i n interactions with other spiders. RESULT S All males contacted nests 1-28 min after introduction into the cages, and there was no difference in the latency to contact between nests of females and those of males (latenc y for each male to contact nest of female minus that for the same male to contact nest of male : 1.6 ± 9.16 min for nests of adults ; 0.9 ± 10.32 min for nests of subadults), providing no evidence of attraction to nests by airborne pheromones. Each element of behavior occurred sometimes at nests of each sex-age class (Table 1). However, males more often performed abdomen twitching at nests of females than a t those of males (nests of adults : x2 = 5.143, P < 0.025 ; subadults : x2 = 12.071, P < 0.005). Abdomen twitching, vibrating, tugging and probing tended to occur either soon afte r the male touched the nest or not at all (latencies in sec from nest-contact until behavio r was performed : abdomen twitch, 18, 54 ; vibrate, 33, 217 ; tug, 28, 584 ; probe, 2, 83). The durations (in sec) of these behaviors tended to be brief (abdomen twitch, 14, 144;vibrate, 4, 37;tug, 10, 110;probe, 9, 67 ; cases in which the behavior failed to occu r deleted). Times not engaged in these activities were occupied with grooming, walking, spinning, pivoting, or simply standing inactive.

90 THE JOURNAL OF ARACHNOLOGY Spiders that entered nests did so soon (35 sec, 395 sec) after contacting the nest. Sixteen males each entered the nests of both (a) a subadult female and (b) a subadul t male, and latencies were greater when the nests were ones of (a) rather than (b) (latenc y in sec of (a) - (b) : 43, 388 ; Wilcoxon test, Ts = 6, P < 0.01). Males also probed longer at nests of females than at nests of males (durations in sec o f probing at nests of females minus those at nests of males : 10, 41 for nests of adults, T s = 9; P < 0.02; 10, 64 for nests of subadults, T s = 11.5, P < 0.01). Twelve males each first touched one of the two nests with which they were tested i n the vicinity of the door (an elastic slit-like opening to the nest) and the other nes t elsewhere; and latencies to probe for these males were less when first contact was near th e door (latency to probe in sec after contact away from door minus latency after contact near door : 3, 28 ; Ts = 0, P < 0.01). Also, whenever probing occurred, it was concentrate d in the vicinity of the door (duration of probing near door/total duration of probing x 100% : 64% ± 41.3%). DISCUSSION The behavior of male P. johnsoni is adapted to mating with females that they locate i n nests. One line of evidence for this is the different motor patterns and sensory modalitie s involved depending on whether the female is inside or outside her nest (Jackson 1977a, b). The present study provides further evidence. The nest alone elicits courtship from the male and provides the male with information concerning the sex of the previou s occupant. Although empty nests elicit brief courtship behavior, probably the presence of th e nest occupant is necessary in order to sustain the male 's behavior for more than a few minutes, since courtship tended to last many minutes (mean, 16 min ; max., 3 hr 44 min ; Jackson 1978a) when females were inside nests. Males seem to be adapted to briefl y "announcing" themselves (courting) when they contact nests of conspecific spiders and to desisting when they do not receive an "answer" (response of the female). It seems likely that chemical stimuli were involved in the discriminatory behavior o f male P. johnsoni since in other groups of spiders there is evidence of pheromone s associated with silk (Kaston 1936, Millot 1946, Tietjen 1978). There were no obvious differences in the shape, size, or other gross structural characteristics of the nests used i n this study, suggesting that tactile discriminations of gross structural differences were no t involved. Visual discrimination of either gross or fine differences also seems unlikely as a n important factor since nests in nature were located in places with little ambient light ; but it is possible that males discriminated fine tactile differences upon contact with nests. In this study and in ones in which the nests were occupied (Jackson 1977a), probin g was concentrated in the vicinity of the door ; and probing occurred more quickly whe n males contacted nests near the doors. Perhaps tactile stimuli associated with the openin g in the nest facilitate probing, although concentration of pheromones around the door i s another possibility. The communicatory behavior of P. johnsoni is more complex than that normally associated with spiders. One aspect of this complexity is the use of alternative tactics for females inside vs outside nests and for adults vs subadults. Another factor is that the rules describing when different behaviors occur do not seem simple. For example, som e behaviors performed by males are also performed by females and immatures, and some of

JACKSON NEXT DISCRIMINATION BY SALTICID 9 1 the male's behaviors are the same when interacting with males or females encountered i n nests. Since the manner in which behaviors are segregated according to sex-age class i s quantitative rather than qualitative (Jackson 1977a), the task of determining th e functions and the message-meaning relationships of the displays exchanged betwee n spiders and the signals associated with nests will be difficult. The fact that the nests of the sympatric gnaphosid spider H. hesperolus failed to elicit vibratory courtship behavior from males of P. johnsoni (Jackson 1976a) indicates that reliable species-identifying information is provided through the nests, at least with respec t to these two species. The occurrence of many of the same elements of behavior at nest s of both males and females of conspecifics, however, might be related to the presence o f less reliable sex-identifying information or to behaviors having adaptive significanc e during both male-male and male-female interactions. Entry into the nest by a male is likely to elicit departure by a subadult female an d decrease the male 's chances of cohabiting (Jackson 1977a). The adaptive significance of the longer latencies before entering nests of subadult females compared to subadult male s may be related to this. Crane (1949) found evidence suggesting that airborne pheromones emanating from females of Corythalia, Phiale, and other neotropical salticids lowered the males' thresholds for performance of displays. This raises the possibility that residual airborn e pheromones in the cage after removal of the females might have affected the manner i n which males of P. johnsoni responded to nests since the maximum time that elapsed between removal of spiders from their nests and when the test males touched the nest s was 22 min in this study. However, in the earlier study (Jackson 1976a), a full da y elapsed between removal of the females and testing. Also, the cages had ventilation openings. Consequently, chemotactic or tactile stimuli would seem more important tha n residual airborne pheromones in eliciting responses from males in this study. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS For valuable discussions during the early phases of this study, I thank Roy Caldwell. I thank Lawrence Field, Mary Catharine Vick, Jerome Rovner and William Eberhard fo r comments on the manuscript. REFERENCE S Bristowe, W. S. 1958. The world of spiders. London: Collins. Crane, J. 1949. Comparative biology of salticid spiders at Rancho Grande, Venezuela. Part IV. A n analysis of display. Zoologica, 34 :159-214. Jackson, R. R. 1974. Rearing methods for spiders. J. Arachnol., 2 :53-56. Jackson, R. R. 1976a. Predation as a selection factor in the mating strategy of the jumping spider Phidippus johnsoni (Salticidae, Araneae). Psyche, 83 :243-255. Jackson, R. R. 1976b. The evolution of courtship and mating tactics in a jumping spider, Phidippus johnsoni (Araneae, Salticidae). Ph.D. dissertation. University of California, Berkeley. Jackson, R. R. 1977a. An analysis of alternative mating tactics of the jumping spider Phidippu s johnsoni (Araneae, Salticidae). J. Arachnol., 5 :185-230. Jackson, R. R. 1977b. Courtship versatility in the jumping spider, Phidippus johnsoni (Araneae: Salticidae). Anim. Behay., 25 :953-957. Jackson, R. R. 1978a. The mating strategy of Phidippus johnsoni (Araneae, Salticidae) : 1. Pursuit time and persistence. Behay.Ecol. Sociobiol., 4 :123-132.

92 THE JOURNAL OF ARACHNOLOGY Jackson, R. R. 1978b. Male mating strategies of dictynid spiders with differing types of social organization. Symp. Zool. Soc. London, 42 :79-88. Jackson, R. R. 1979. Nests of Phidippus johnsoni (Araneae, Salticidae) : characteristics, pattern of occupation, and function. J. Arachnol., 7 :47-58. Kaston, B. J. 1936. The senses involved in the courtship of some vagabond spiders. Entomol. Amer., 16 :97-167. Land, M. F. 1972. Mechanisms of orientation and pattern recognition by jumping spiders (Salticidae), pp. 231-247. In Information processing in the visual systems of arthropods (ed. by R. Wehner). Berlin : Springer-Verlag. Millot, J. 1946. Sens chemique et sens visuel chez les araignees. Ann. Biol. Anim., 22 :1-21. Rohlf, F. J. and R. R. Sokal, 1969. Statistical Tables. San Francisco : Freeman. Sokal, R. R. and F. J. Rohlf, 1969. Biometry. San Francisco : Freeman. Tietjen, W. J. 1977. Dragline-following by male lycosid spiders. Psyche, 84 :165-178. Tietjen, W. J. 1978. Is the pheromone of Lycosa rabida (Araneae, Lycosidae) deposited on a substratum? J. Arachnol., 6 :207-212. Manuscript received November 1979, revised March 1980.