October 1980] Short Communications 875

Similar documents
A "New" Allele of the Mallard Plumage Pattern in Ducks

TIME BUDGET OF BREEDING NORTHERN SHOVELERS

THE AUK A QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF ORNITHOLOGY. VoL. 104 OCTOBER 1987 NO. 4 MULTIPLE PATERNITY IN A WILD SUSAN EVARTS AND CHRISTOPHER J.

Anas clypeata (Northern Shoveler)

Breeding behaviour f eaptive Shovelers

Pair formation among experimentally introduced mallards Anas platyrhynchos reflects habitat quality

GENERAL NOTES 675. Reproductive behavior and pairing chronology in wintering dabbling ducks.-

Puddle Ducks Order Anseriformes Family Anatinae Subfamily Anatini

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

COMPARATIVE DISPLAYS OF YEARLING AND ADULT MALE WOOD DUCKS CARL E. KORSCHGEN AND LEIGH H. FREDRICKSON

By: Rinke Berkenbosch

Unit E: Other Poultry. Lesson 2: Exploring the Duck Industry

UKRAINIAN CLAY (Ukrainskaya glinistaya)

FREQUENCY AND TIMING OF SECOND BROODS IN WOOD DUCKS

A QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR OF MALLARDS AND BLACK DUCKS

Sciences, the Welder Wildlife Foundation, and Iowa State University.

Question 3 (30 points)

BROOD PARASITISM AMONG WATERFOWL NESTING ON ISLANDS AND PENINSULAS IN NORTH DAKOTA

ESTIMATING NEST SUCCESS: WHEN MAYFIELD WINS DOUGLAS H. JOHNSON AND TERRY L. SHAFFER

Level 2 Biology, 2015

Station 1. Using the cards, match the vocabulary word with its definition. If there are any words you do not know, write them down if you have time!

Genetics and Probability

WING AND TAIL MOLT OF THE SPARROW HAWK ERNEST J. WILLOUGHBY

Subfamily Anserinae. Waterfowl Identification WFS 340. Mute Swan. Order Anseriformes. Family Anatidae

Egg-laying by the Cuckoo

The purpose of this lab was to examine inheritance patters in cats through a

9-2 Probability and Punnett. Squares Probability and Punnett Squares. Slide 1 of 21. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

646 General Notes [Auk, Vol. 93. the fauna, the number of different birds at Reddick stands at 66, 64 of which have

Virtual Lab: Sex-Linked Traits Worksheet. 1. Please make sure you have read through all of the information in the

Cow Exercise 1 Answer Key

NEST PROSPECTING BY COMMON GOLDENEYES

Mendelian Genetics SI

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Correlation of. Animal Science Biology & Technology, 3/E, by Dr. Robert Mikesell/ MeeCee Baker, 2011, ISBN 10: ; ISBN 13:

Notes and Discussion

A Study of Bobwhite Quail Nest Initiation Dates, Clutch Sizes, and Hatch Sizes in Southwest Georgia

A-l. Students shall examine the circulatory and respiratory systems of animals.

Mendelian Genetics Problem Set

Fruit Fly Exercise 2 - Level 2

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

Radiation Research Society is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Radiation Research.

Here are some ground rules that you should ALWAYS follow when tackling an Inheritance Problem:

{Received 21st August 1964)

Seed color is either. that Studies Heredity. = Any Characteristic that can be passed from parents to offspring

CANVASBACK AND REDHEAD PRODUCTIVITY AT RUBY LAKE NATIONAL WILDLIFE REFUGE

ALLELES1 OF THE MALLARD PLUMAGE PATTERN IN DUCKS2

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

How to Raise Healthy Geese for the Backyard Farm

Weights of wild Mallard Anas platyrhynchos, Gadwall A. streperà, and Blue-winged Teal A. discors during the breeding season

The female Mallard s call is a loud quack-quack similar to that given by farmyard ducks. The call of the male is a softer, low-pitched rhab-rhab.

HCPS III Benchmarks SC SC SC Duration (2) One-hour periods. Source Material PRISM

Sex-Influenced (Autosomes) P Horned x Hornless HH H'H' H H' F 1 Horned x Hornless HH' HH' 1/2 H 1/2 H' 1/2 H 1/2 H' F 2 Genotypes Phenotypes

+ Karyotypes. Does it look like this in the cell?

The behaviour of a pair of House Sparrows while rearing young

PRODUCTIVITY OF NESTING SPECTACLED EIDERS ON THE LOWER KASHUNUK RIVER, ALASKA1

Patterns of Inheritance. What are the different ways traits can be inherited?

EMBRYO DIAGNOSIS AN IMPORTANT TOOL TO HELP THE HATCHERY MANAGER

Student Exploration: Mouse Genetics (One Trait)

INFO SHEET. Cull Eggs: What To Expect And How To Reduce The Incidence.

Page 1 of 7. Name: A. Preliminary Assessment #3. You may need a calculator for numbers 2&3.

Sexing Day-Old Chicks on Small and Backyard Flocks

DO NOT WRITE ON THIS TEST Unit 6 Assessment Genetics Objective 3.2.2

Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1

13. Cell division is. assortment. telophase. cytokinesis.

Naked Bunny Evolution

Other Patterns of Inheritance:

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics

INTER-FAMILY DOMINANCE IN CANADA GEESE

Studying Mechanisms of Inheritance using Drosophila melanogaster

Genetic improvement For Alternative Hen-Housing

Waterfowl Along the Road

Biology 3201 Sex Linked Review Mr.Gillam Name:

Chapter 11-2 Probability and Punnett Squares Notes

( 162 ) SOME BREEDING-HABITS OF THE LAPWING.

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

Ducks of Florida 1. Dabbling Ducks WEC243. Emma Willcox and William Giuliano 2

The Pigeon Genetics Newsletter

WATTLED CRANE RECOVERY PROGRAMME Ensuring that Wattled Cranes continue to grace the skies and wetlands of South Africa

IRE SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF A MALLARD POPULATION IN NORTHERN IOWA.THS

Mendel s Laws: Their Application to Solving Genetics Problem

Ecology and Management of Ruffed Grouse and American Woodcock

Avian Reproductive System Female

ILLINOI PRODUCTION NOTE. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Library Large-scale Digitization Project, 2007.

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Anseriformes Family: Anatidae Scientific Name: Mergus squamatus Common Name: Scaly-sided (Chinese) Merganser

Reproductive physiology and eggs

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

MDWFP Aerial Waterfowl Survey Report. December 11-13, 2017

The Influence of Diet Quality on Clutch Size and Laying Pattern in Mallards

LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE'

Genetics Since Mendel. At dog and cat shows, an animal s owner may be asked to show its pedigree. What do you think a pedigree shows?

Understandings, Applications and Skills (This is what you maybe assessed on)

EGG SIZE AND LAYING SEQUENCE

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Text: Elly Vogelaar Photos: Aviculture Europa

Second Broods In Bobwhite Quail

A Geneticist s Perspective from Within a Broiler Primary Breeder Company

Name: Block: Date: Packet #12 Unit 6: Heredity

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING.

Transcription:

October 1980] Short Communications 875 BARNARD, C. J. 1980. Flock feeding and time budgets in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus). Anim. Behav. 28: 295-309. BERTRAM, B.C. R. 1980. Vigilance and group sizes in ostriches. Anim. Behav. 28: 278-286. CARACO, T. 1979a. Time budgeting and group size: a test of theory. Ecology 60: 618-627. 1979b. Time budgeting and group size: a theory. Ecology 60: 611-617. 1980. Stochastic dynamics of avian foraging flocks. Amer. Natur. 115: 262-275. --, S. MakRTINDALE, & H. R. PULLIAM. 1980. Avian flocking in the presence of a predator. Nature 285: 400-401. FRETWELL, S. D. 1972. Populations in a seasonal environment. Monogr. Pop. Biol. 5: 1-219. LAZARUS, J. 1979. The early warning function of flocking in birds: an experimental study with captive Quelea. Anim. Behav. 17: 855-865. PULLIAM, H. R. 1973. On the advantages of flocking. J. Theoret. Biol. 38: 419422. 1976. The principle of optimal behavior and the theory of communities. Pp. 311-332 in Perspectives in ethology (P. H. Klopfer and P. G. Bateson, Eds.). New York, Plenum Press. Received 3 March 1980, accepted 12 June 1980. Forced Copulation in Captive Mallards I. Fertilization of Eggs JEFFREY T. BURNS, KIMBERLY M. CHENG, AND FRANK McKINNEY Department of Ecology and Behavioral Biology, Bell Museum of Natural History, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455 USA Forced copulation (FC) has been reported in a number of avian families but it is especially well known in waterfowl (family Anatidae). Often this behavior has been called "rape" in the bird literature, but we feel that it is best to avoid this controversial term. In contrast to pair copulations, which are typically preceded by characteristic displays by both sexes, FCs are preceded by active pursuit, grasping, and overpowering of the female. Ever since Heinroth (1910, 1911) drew attention to this phenomenon, there has been controversy about its significance, especially in the Mallard (Anas platyrhynchos) and other dabbling ducks (tribe Anatini). Geyr yon Schweppenberg (1924) thought that it occurs after females start incubating and are no longer available to satisfy the sexual urges of males. Christoleit (1929a,b) believed that females do not really try to escape but rather encourage males to chase them and to compete for copulations, thereby ensuring fertilization by the strongest male. Weidmann (1956) concluded that FC is associated with territoriality and is a mechanism whereby a male discourages other pairs from settling on his breeding area. Bezzel (1959) and Wrist (1960) doubted that it is common enough in wild birds to be of significance, and they attributed its occurrence in parks and zoos to abnormally high densities of birds. In a key study of wild Pintails (A. acura), Smith (1968) demonstrated that aerial pursuits (including FC attempts) peaked in frequency during the egg-laying period, and he suggested that these copulations could be related to fertilization of eggs. Reports that male urban Mallards make FC attempts on their own mates (Bezzel 1959, Raitasuo 1964) have been confirmed by recent studies, and the occurrence of such imposed pair copulations soon after a female has been subjected to FC strongly suggests that sperm competition is going on [Barrett 1973, Barash 1977 for Mallards; McKinney 1975, McKinney and Stolen in prep., for Green-winged Teal (A. crecca)]. The experiment on captive Mallards by Elder and Weller (1954), in which they removed males from their mates and checked the fertility of eggs subsequently laid, had already shown that females can store sperm for up to 10 days. Thus, at least in certain species of

876 Short Communications [Auk, Vol. 97 12 THIRD CLUTCHES io SECOND CLUTCHES 12 FIRST CLUTCHES Fig. 1. 0-1 W-1 B-I G-I 0-2 B-2 Y-2 G-2 D UNIDENTIFIED PAIR PROGENY I FC PROGENY FC progeny, pair progeny, and unidentified embryos produced by eight captive dusky females. dabbling duck, FC could be an integral part of the reproductive strategy of paired males (reviewed by McKinney, Derrickson and Mineau, in prep.). Until now, however, there has been no proof that FCs involve actual sperm transfer or subsequent fertilization. To test whether or not eggs can be fertilized by FC, we used Mallards exhibiting the "dusky" trait. Dusky males lack the white neck ring and chestnut breast of wild-type Mallards, females have a darker than normal plumage, and ducklings are almost entirely black. The dusky trait is recessive (Jaap 1934, Lancaster 1963), so that ducklings from dusky x wild-type matings have the familiar yellow and brown wild-type down pattern. The dusky Mallards used in our study were genetically seven-eighths wild as a result of a 5-yr breeding program involving repeated backcrossing of dusky Mallards from gamefarm stock with wild Mallards at the Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center, Jamestown, North Dakota (Lee and Nelson 1972). The wild-type Mallards were offspring of birds taken as eggs from nests of wild birds in central North Dakota during the spring of 1976. Because the dusky gene is extremely rare in wild populations, we assume that the wild-type individuals used were homozygous for the dominant wild-type plumage. In August 1977 we established a flock of 10-week-old Mallards comprised of 16 males (8 wild-type, 8 dusky) and 11 females (all dusky), each marked with a numbered nasal saddle (Sugden and Poston 1968). During the winter they were allowed free choice of mates and were examined weekly for signs of pairing. On 16 April 1978 eight firmly bonded pairs were selected. Two wild-type male + dusky female (WM + DF) pairs and two dusky male + dusky female (DM + DF) pairs were introduced into each of two adjacent flight pens. DM + DF pairs are expected to produce only dusky young, and WM + DF pairs should produce only wild-type young. The appearance of both types of young in a single clutch indicates mixed parentage, barring mutation and the possible occurrence of heterozygous wild-type males in our study flock. With this experimental design we could expect to detect, on average, two out of every three

October 1980] Short Communications 877 MALES 0-1 W-! B-! G-! 0-2 B-2 Y-2 G-2 WM WM DM DM WM WM DM DM FEMALES FC FC ATTEMPT Fig. 2. Individual involvement of wild-type males (WM), dusky males (DM), and females (all dusky) in FC and FC attempts. ducklings resulting from FC (i.e. those cases in which the father was of the alternate type to the female's mate). The flight pens (McKinney 1967) each measured 27.5 m x 27.5 m x 3.6 m, giving enough room for all pairs to establish small territories. Nest scrapes were made by the birds in long grass around the edges of the pens, and by watching them go to their nests we determined which females laid which eggs. Eggs were collected every second day, labelled, and replaced with dummy eggs. When clutches were complete (no new eggs for 2 days), the dummy eggs were removed and the nest destroyed to encourage renesting. We stored the collected eggs in a cool room and incubated them in batches set at about weekly intervals. Eggs were candled on the 7th, 14th, and 21st days of incubation. At the time of the last candling, dead embryos were classified according to phenotype, when possible, and on day 23 all viable eggs were transferred to individual hatching baskets in a "hatcher" incubator. Ducklings were typed 1 or 2 days following hatching. From 17 April to 21 June we made daily 3-h observations, beginning at sunrise, from a blind overlooking both flight pens. All major interactions (chases, pair copulations, FC attempts) and the nesting activities of each female were recorded. We distinguished "FC attempts" from "FCs" by watching for clues indicating whether the male was successful in achieving intromission. FC attempts entailed pursuit with grasping in the male's bill of some part of the female (nape, back, wing, or tail), and mounting might or might not follow. Our criteria for FC were (1) a pronounced thrust by the male as he bent his tail around the side of the female's tail and (2) a deliberate departure by the male after dismounting, usually with tail-wagging (apparently to retract the "penis"). After dismounting, the male sometimes performed postcopulatory displays (Bridling, Nod-swimming), and sometimes we could see the everted penis, but these were not invariable after FC as judged by our two criteria. We could identify to type 156 of the 229 eggs laid by the 8 females (3 clutches each). Thirteen (8%) of the identified progeny differed from the expected type (Fig. 1). We observed 58 FCs and 309 FC attempts

878 Short Communications [Auk, Vol. 97 (Fig. 2). Females producing 5 of the 13 eggs yielding FC progeny were subjected to FC from males of the appropriate type 1 or 2 days before the laying of the eggs, and FC attempts by males of the appropriate type were seen within 2 days before the laying of 2 more of these eggs. We could identify 61 ducklings or embryos from first clutches, 45 from second, and 50 from third clutches. Of these, 8%, 11%, and 6%, respectively, were FC progeny, suggesting no seasonal trend. Nor did there appear to be a trend in relation to the sequence of eggs within a clutch. Although the number of progeny from females paired to dusky males (117 eggs, 74 young) and the number from females paired to wild-type males (112 eggs, 82 young) were nearly equal, wild-type males produced most of the FC progeny (11 of 13, X 2 = 6.63, P < 0.05). We cannot account for this asymmetry, and in fact there were more records of FC by dusky males on the mates of wild-type males (26) than of the reverse (19). Dusky males had a slightly higher success rate in FC attempts than did wild-type males (25% and 19% of contacts leading to FC, respectively). More FCs were recorded in pen 2 (45) than pen 1 (26), and 9 of the 13 FC progeny were produced by pen 2 birds. In theory, the production of mixed broods by the females we studied could be explained by (1) mutation, (2) the presence of heterozygous wild-type males in our experimental population, (3) females laying in other females' nests, (4) mate-switches, and (5) fertilizations by FC. We discount the possibility of mutations, as the rate required would be unrealistically high. All of the females paired to wild-type males produced at least one clutch comprised only of wild-type ducklings (of those identifiable), suggesting that all of our wild-type males were homozygous. In our daily watches we saw no females laying in other birds' nests, and all clutches were consistent in the distinctive characteristics of the eggs they contained (size, shape, and color of eggs varying between individual females). No mate-switches occurred during the season, and, although females were repeatedly observed copulating with their own mates, they were never observed to solicit copulations from males other than their mate. We conclude that these experiments have demonstrated that FC involves successful intromission and insemination and that this insem- ination can result in sperm competition and subsequent fertilization of eggs. These findings must be interpreted cautiously. It seems likely that our captive females were subjected to more FC attempts than would be wild free-ranging birds. The frequency of FC attempts and the proportion of attempts that result in FC in wild Mallards remains an open question. We emphasize that additional research is needed to determine the significance of this phenomenon under natural conditions. This research was supported by the National Science Foundation under grant BNS 76-02233. We are indebted to Forrest Lee, Gary Krapu, and the Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center, Jamestown, North Dakota for supplying the birds; Robert Shoffner and the Department of Animal Science, University of Minnesota, St. Paul Campus for providing incubator facilities; Anne Pilli for drawing the figures; and George Barrowclough, David Bruggers, and Harrison B. Tordoff for comments on the manuscript. L TE TURE C TED BAR SH, D. P. 1977. Sociobiology of rape in Mallards (Anas platyrhynchos): responses of the mated male. Science 197: 788-789. BARRETT, J. 1973. Breeding behavior of captive Mallards. Unpublished M.S. thesis, Minneapolis, Minnesota, Univ. Minnesota. BEZZEL, E. 1959. Beitr ige zur Biologie der Geschlecter bei Entenv6geln. Anz. Orn. Ges. Bayern 5: 269-355. CHRISTOLEIT, g. 1929a. Ueber das Reihen der Enten. Beitr. Fortpfl. V6gel 5: 45-53. 1929b. Nochmals das Reihen der Enten. Beitr. Fortpfl. V6gel 5:212-216. ELDER, W. H., & M. W. WELLER. 1954. Duration of fertility in the domestic Mallard hen after isolation from the drake. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 18: 495-502. GEYR YON SCHWEPPENBERG, H. 1924. Zur Sexualethologie der Stockente. J. Ornithol. 72: 102-108. HEINROTH, O. 1910. Beobachtungen bei einem Einbiirgerungsversuch reit der Brautente (Lampronessa sponsa L.). J. Ornithol. 58: 101-156. 1911. Beitr ige zur Biologie, namentlich Ethologie und Psychologie der Anatiden. Verh. 5th Intern. Ornithol. Kong., 589-702. JAA?, R. G. 1934. Alleles of the mallard plumage in ducks. Genetics 19: 310. LANCASTER, F. m. 1963. The inheritance of plumage color in the common duck. Bibliographica Genetica 19:317-404. LEE, F., & H. K. NELSON. 1972. Summary of research projects at the Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center related to hand-reared ducks. Pp. 91-104 in Symposium on the role of hand-reared ducks in waterfowl management. Dundee, Illinois, Max McGraw Wildlife Foundation.

October 1980] Short Communications 879 McKINNE¾, F. 1967. Breeding behaviour of captive Shovelers. Wildfowl Trust, 18th Ann. Rept. 108-121. 1975. Evolution of duck displays. Pp. 331-357 in Function and evolution in behaviour (G. Baerends, C. Beer, and A. Manning, Eds.). Oxford, Clarendon Press. R^ITAStrO, K. 1964. Social behaviour of the mallard, Anas platyrhynchos, in the course of the annual cycle. Pap. Game Res., Helsinki, No. 24: 1-72. SMITH, R. I. 1968. The social aspects of reproductive behavior in the Pintail. Auk 85: 381-396. SIJGDEN, L. G., & H. J. POSTON. 1968. A nasal marker for ducks. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 32: 984-986. WEIDMANN, U. 1956. Verhaltensstudien an der Stockente (Anas platyrhynchos L.). I. Das Aktionssystem. Z. Tierpsychol. 13: 208-271. W/gST, W. 1960. Das Problem des Reihens der Enten besonders von Anas strepera. Proc. 12th Intern. Ornithol. Cong., vol 2: 795-800. Received I October 1979, accepted 26 March 1980. Sexual Development During Winter in Male American Woodcock THOMAS H. ROBERTS Department of Wildlife and Fisheries, Mississippi State University, Drawer LW, Mississippi State, Mississippi 39762 USA Recent studies of American Woodcock (Philohela minor) have documented that substantial breeding occurs throughout much of the southeastern United States, an area formerly considered to be significant only as a wintering ground (Causey et al. 1974, Stamps and Doerr 1977, Roberts and Dimmick 1978). Woodcock are among the earliest breeders of North American avifauna, but the chronological pattern of their reproductive cycle has not been adequately described. This study was undertaken to delineate the sexual development in the male segment of the population during winter and to determine whether or not physiological differences exist between adult and subadult males. Woodcock were collected from 17 December 1978 to 26 February 1979. Collection sites were southeastern Louisiana, east-central Mississippi, and western Tennessee. Birds were classified as adults or subadults according to characteristics of the secondaries (Martin 1964). After measurement and fixation in 10% formalin, cross sections of left testes were prepared for histological examination. Sections (6 were stained with hematoxylin and eosine and examined with a binocular microscope under 100x. Based on the degree of gonadal development, males were classified into three groups: Class I--sexually mature (spermatozoa present in all seminiferous tubules); Class II--spermatogenesis occurring but not yet sexually mature (spermatids present in most tubules; spermatozoa present in some cases but only in a few tubules and not in large numbers when present); and Class III--quiescent (only spermatogonia present; no evidence of spermatogenesis). Forty-seven males were collected during the study. The size of the 43 testes measured ranged from 3.0 mm to 12.0 min. Considerable variation existed among individuals on any given date, but there was a trend toward increasing testis length during the study period (Fig. 1). There was no significant difference in the regression equations (P = 0.95) of adults and subadults when evaluated separately. The earliest date that spermatogenesis was observed was 19 January, while the earliest individual in Class I was collected on 22 January. All birds taken after 1 February (n = 22) were in Class I or Class II; 76% were in Class I. The Mann-Whitney u-test (Conover 1971) showed no difference in testis size between 14 adult and 14 subadult males collected during February (P > 0.1). Of the subadults shot in February and examined histologically, 91% were in Class I. No difference in either testis size or degree of development that might be attributable to collection site was apparent in the data. Of 39 testes that were examined histologically, 97% of those greater than or equal to 6.0 mm were in Class I or Class II, with 76% being in Class I. Only 14% of those less than 6.0 mm (n = 7) were in Class II; none was in Class I. All less than 5.0 mm (n = 5) were in Class III. Although there was individual variation, the majority of the male woodcock in my sample had attained full breeding condition by early February. Recrudescence of gonads began at least as early as mid- January, although the small number of samples before that period precluded a more precise determi-