Experiential Canalization of Behavioral Development: Results

Similar documents
Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens

Biased Embryos: Prenatal Experience Alters the Postnatal Malleability of Auditory Preferences in Bobwhite Quail

Darwin s Finches: A Thirty Year Study.

Variation of Chicken Embryo Development by Temperature Influence. Anna Morgan Miller. Rockdale Magnet School for Science and Technology

206 Adopted: 4 April 1984

Introduction to phylogenetic trees and tree-thinking Copyright 2005, D. A. Baum (Free use for non-commercial educational pruposes)

Postnatal effects of incubation length in mallard and pheasant chicks

Mr. Bouchard Summer Assignment AP Biology. Name: Block: Score: / 20. Topic: Chemistry Review and Evolution Intro Packet Due: 9/4/18

Do the traits of organisms provide evidence for evolution?

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

Dogs and More Dogs PROGRAM OVERVIEW

Dogs and More Dogs PROGRAM OVERVIEW

Reproductive physiology and eggs

This article is downloaded from.

Correlation of. Animal Science Biology & Technology, 3/E, by Dr. Robert Mikesell/ MeeCee Baker, 2011, ISBN 10: ; ISBN 13:

EVOLUTIONARY GENETICS (Genome 453) Midterm Exam Name KEY

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Effects of Natural Selection

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified):

Patterns of heredity can be predicted.

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation?

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics

How do low-quality females know they re low-quality and do they always prefer low-quality mates?

A-l. Students shall examine the circulatory and respiratory systems of animals.

Biology 2108 Laboratory Exercises: Variation in Natural Systems. LABORATORY 2 Evolution: Genetic Variation within Species

Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males*

ESTIMATING NEST SUCCESS: WHEN MAYFIELD WINS DOUGLAS H. JOHNSON AND TERRY L. SHAFFER

Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1

ON FORCE-FEEDING GEESE AND DUCKS (GAVAGE)

Maternal Effects in the Green Turtle (Chelonia mydas)

Biology. Slide 1 of 33. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

On Deriving Aspectual Sense

Song in the city: the effects of urban noise on communication patterns and population genetics of an Australian passerine

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

Arkansas State FFA Poultry Exam 2016

Egg-laying by the Cuckoo

INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR

Selective Breeding. Selective Breeding

CHAPTER XI. NEST-BUILDING, INCUBATION, AND MIGRATION.

Was the Spotted Horse an Imaginary Creature? g.org/sciencenow/2011/11/was-the-spotted-horse-an-imagina.html

University of Canberra. This thesis is available in print format from the University of Canberra Library.

A was analyzed recently in two papers by the author (GERSTEL 1943: 1945a).

1 This question is about the evolution, genetics, behaviour and physiology of cats.

Allocating Feed to Female Broiler Breeders: Technical Bulletin #2

Interpreting Evolutionary Trees Honors Integrated Science 4 Name Per.

SEXUAL IMPRINTING IN FEMALE ZEBRA FINCHES: CHANGES IN PREFERENCES AS AN EFFECT OF ADULT EXPERIENCE

Canine Behavior and Acoustics

The color and patterning of pigmentation in cats, dogs, mice horses and other mammals results from the interaction of several different genes

INCUBATION AND VITAL MORPHOLOGICAL TRAITS IN EGGS FROM AGE-RELATED TURKEYS

Lesson 4.7: Life Science Genetics & Selective Breeding

Darwin and the Family Tree of Animals

Individual sibling recognition in experimental broods of common tern chicks

SUMMARY OF THESIS. Chapter VIII "The place of research, its purpose, the biological material and method"

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

POSTNATAL DEVELOPMENT IN PEROMYSCUS MANICULATUS-POLIONOTUS HYBRIDS I. DEVELOPMENTAL LANDMARKS AND LITTER MORTALITY 12

NAME: DATE: SECTION:

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below.

CLADISTICS Student Packet SUMMARY Phylogeny Phylogenetic trees/cladograms

Evolution on Exhibit Hints for Teachers

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

Course # Course Name Credits

Bio homework #5. Biology Homework #5

1.5 C: Role of the Environment in Evolution Quiz

Title: Sources of Genetic Variation SOLs Bio 7.b.d. Lesson Objectives

2013 Holiday Lectures on Science Medicine in the Genomic Era

Trapped in a Sea Turtle Nest

Punnett Squares. and Pedigrees. How are patterns of inheritance studied? Lesson ESSENTIAL QUESTION. J S7L3.b Reproduction and genetic variation

HATCHING BEHAVIOR OF THE BOBWHITE

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite

Ch 1.2 Determining How Species Are Related.notebook February 06, 2018

SELECTION FOR AN INVARIANT CHARACTER, VIBRISSA NUMBER, IN THE HOUSE MOUSE. IV. PROBIT ANALYSIS

Title: Phylogenetic Methods and Vertebrate Phylogeny

Life History and Social Learning: Megapode Chicks Fail to Acquire Feeding Preferences From Conspecifics

Section 6. Embryonic Development and Hatchery Management Notes

Representation, Visualization and Querying of Sea Turtle Migrations Using the MLPQ Constraint Database System

By: Rinke Berkenbosch

EXOTIC PETS The landscape has changed

Red Eared Slider Secrets. Although Most Red-Eared Sliders Can Live Up to Years, Most WILL NOT Survive Two Years!

The Development of Behavior

What Canadian vets need to know and explain about antimicrobial resistance

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING.

King Fahd University of Petroleum & Minerals College of Industrial Management

Modern Evolutionary Classification. Lesson Overview. Lesson Overview Modern Evolutionary Classification

Poultry in behaviour research.

A Geneticist s Perspective from Within a Broiler Primary Breeder Company

THE ROLE OF WATER IN THE EVOLUTION OF THE TERRESTRIAL VERTEBRATES

Taxonomy and Pylogenetics

FOOTEDNESS IN DOMESTIC PIGEONS

OBSERVATIONS ON SWALLOWS AND HOUSE- MARTINS AT THE NEST. BY

Is it better to be bigger? Featured scientists: Aaron Reedy and Robert Cox from the University of Virginia Co-written by Matt Kustra

DAM (1929) as reported by Cheney

Short-term Water Potential Fluctuations and Eggs of the Red-eared Slider Turtle (Trachemys scripta elegans)

Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee Guideline. Defining Humane Endpoints and End-stage Illness BACKGROUND. Definitions.

REPORT FROM THE FIRST GLOBAL MILK QUALITY EXPERT FORUM

Brine Shrimp Investigation AP Biology Name: Per:

Transcription:

Developmental Psychology Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1991, Vol. 27, No. 1, 35-39 0012-1649/91/$3.00 Experiential Canalization of Behavioral Development: Results Gilbert Gottlieb University of North Carolina at Greensboro In exploring the possible experiential canalization of development, it was found that the mallard duck embryo's contact call plays a canalizing role in species-specific perceptual development. When mallard ducklings were prevented from hearing the contact call, they were susceptible to developing a preference for an extraspecific maternal call over the mallard maternal call. Exposure to the contact call prevented the ducklings from developing a preference for the extraspecific maternal call. Thus, normally occurring experience, in concert with genetic and other activities, can canalize behavioral development. Canalizing influences account for developmental stability, so that what we think of as normal or typical for a species repeats itself generation after generation. In order for evolution to occur, through genetic mutation or otherwise, the canalizing influences associated with normal development must be overcome. Canalization is thus a conservative feature of development that prevents evolution from occurring in a ready fashion. My preceding article (Gottlieb, 1991 a) established the theoretical framework for examining the possibility that normally occurring experience can canalize behavioral development. The present article presents empirical evidence for the experiential canalization of species-specific maternal attachment. The present hypothesis is that normally occurring experience helps to achieve species-specific development by making the developing organism unresponsive to extraspecific influences. For example, if the mallard duck (Anas platyrhynchos) embryo is devocalized, the usually highly selective response to the mallard maternal call is weakened: After hatching, the duckling is as responsive to a chicken (Gallus gallus) maternal call as to a mallard maternal call in a simultaneous-choice test (Gottlieb, 1978). Exposing otherwise devocalized embryos to a recording of mallard embryo contact vocalizations causes them to show the usual preference for the mallard call versus the chicken call in the postnatal test (Gottlieb, 1985). Exposure to other types of sibling vocalizations in embryo, or exposure to the contact call after hatching, is ineffective, so the kind of experience and the timing of the experience are highly specific. If the experiential canalization hypothesis is correct, embryonic devocalization should make the mallard embryo and hatchling more susceptible to exposure to an extraspecific maternal call, assuming that the contact vocalization is acting as a canalizing or buffering experience. With the necessary control groups, five experimental conditions are necessary to test the canalization hypothesis, as indicated in Table 1. In Table 1, X indicates the predicted preference and X----X indicates prediction of no preference. The two most critical Research was supported by National Science Foundation Grant BNS-8419011 and National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH- 44813. Margaret Horton, Ramona Rodriguiz, Wayne Kelly III, and April Gray provided excellent assistance with the experiments reported in this article. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gilbert Gottlieb, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina, Greensboro, North Carolina 27412-5001. groups are 4 and 5. It is essential to demonstrate the induction of a preference for the chicken call in devocal-isolates and the buffering effect when vocal-isolates are the embryo contact call and the chicken call. The other groups are necessary control groups. Subjects Method Peking ducklings are a domestic form of mallard duck. Despite their domestication, this breed has been shown to be similar to their wilder counterparts in the specificity of their auditory perception of maternal calls, as well as in various other forms of species-typical behavior (Gottlieb, 1971; Johnston & Gottlieb, 198 l; Miller, 1977, 1980; Miller & Gottlieb,! 981 ). Unincubated eggs were received weekly from a large commercial supplier and incubated in the laboratory under conditions described in previous publications (Gottlieb, 1971, 1975). Conditions 1. Vocal-communal. The vocal-communal mallards were incubated together and brooded together in groups of 15 to 25 in individual opaque boxes after hatching. Thus, they could hear themselves and each other both before and after hatching but could not see each other after hatching. 2. Vocal-isolated(Vl). At the beginning of Day 24 of incubation, 2 to 3 days before hatching, when the embryo is first able to vocalize, the VI mallards were put in individual soundproof incubators where they were able to hear their own vocalizations but not those of siblings. These birds served as controls for the devocal conditions below. 3. Devocal-isolated(Dl). As indicated in Figure 1,2 to 3 days before hatching, during the first 8 hours of Day 24 of incubation, these embryos were muted and placed in individual soundproof incubators. They were muted in a brief operation in which their syringeal ("voice box") membranes were coated with a nontoxic surgical glue (collodion). Collodion forms a rigid sheath over the membranes, and when they are thus rigidified, the embryo (and later the hatchling) cannot vocalize (Gottlieb & Vandenbergh, 1968). Our "hit" rate on devocalization is around 90%; if an embryo or hatchling can utter even the scratchiest vocalization it was eliminated from the experiment. There 35

36 GILBERT GOTTLIEB Table 1 Conditions Favoring Canalization or Malleability of Species- Specific Perceptual Preference Predicted preference Condition Mallard Chicken 1. Vocal communal X 2. Vocal-isolated X 3. Devocal-isolated X... X 4. Devocal-isolated, exposed to chicken call X 5. Vocal-isolated, exposed to chicken call and mallard duck embryo contact vocalization X Note. X = preference; X... X = no preference. is a small toll associated with the operation: About 10% fewer birds hatch than from unopened eggs. 4. Devocal-isolated, exposedtochickencall. The DIs the chicken call received that exposure in their soundproof incubator compartments, where a speaker repetitively broadcast a single burst of a chicken maternal call for 30 min per hour from Day 24 until 48 hr after hatching, at which time the birds were tested. (The call was actually on for 22.5 min of the 30-min period.) The call peaked at 65 db (Scale B, fast) in the incubator and in the test (described below). 5a. Vocal-isolated, chicken call. These birds were treated the same as above with the exception that they could hear themselves vocalize, as well as being the 30-min chicken call each hour. (In Groups 5a and 5b, the embryos' heads were extricated from the shell in the same manner as for Group 3 and 4 birds, but they were not devocalized.) The prediction here, as indicated in Table 1, was the failure of the exposure to the chicken call to induce a preference for it because the birds had been permitted to hear their own contact call. Although I did not monitor the vocalizations of these particular birds, I knew from unpublished observations that isolated birds produce the contact call numerous times both before and after hatching. 5b. Vocal-isolated, chicken and embryo contact calls. These VIs were treated the same as Group 5a, except that they were l 0 min of the variable-rate embryonic contact call (Gottlieb, 1985) after 30 min of exposure to the chicken call each hour. Because I did not monitor the self-produced vocalizations of the birds in Group 5a, this group provided a more precise f.x on a known amount of exposure to the contact call that is required for the buffering effect. The choice of the amount of exposure to the contact call was originally arbitrary. In the 10-min period, the call was actually on for 4.6 min, which we now know from monitoring studies is at the bottom of the range produced by individual birds in isolation (4.8-6.2 min per hour, from unpublished observations). This amount of "sib stimulation" is well below the amount that would be received under natural incubation and brooding conditions, in which the birds would be exposed to as many as 8 to 13 conspecific siblings. Thirty min per hour of exposure (actually 22.5 min) to the extraspecific chicken call was shown to be effective in a previous study of malleability in devocalized ducklings (Gottlieb, 1987a), but those ducklings were tested with the chicken call versus the wood duck maternal call, not versus the mallard maternal call. The presence of the mallard call in the simultaneous auditory choice test with the chicken call provided a much sterner test of malleability than using two extra-specific maternal calls. Test. Each bird was tested individually at 48 hr 0:4 hr) after hatching, as illustrated in Figure 2. A speaker was mounted behind each of the two elliptical areas, each broadcasting the chicken or mallard calls over the 5-min test period. As the bird entered the elliptical area in the vicinity of a speaker, the duration of its stay (in seconds) was measured. The duration scores for each call were analyzed by the Wilcoxon test to get a preference for each group (condition) in the experiment. A further measure of preference for each bird was derived as follows: Ifa bird stayed in one approach area more than twice the time it spent in the opposing area, a preference was registered. This measure of preference gives a useful Figure 1. Pulmonary respiration begins on Day 23 of incubation in the domestic mallard duck embryo and is well established by Day 24. When respiration is established, the embryo survives the devocalization operation, which requires extricating its head from the eggshell and painting its syringeal membranes with surgical glue.

SPECIAL SECTION: EXPERIENTIAL CANALIZATION 37 Figure 2. Overview of simultaneous auditory-choice test. (l) Duckling placed equidistant between two speakers (not visible), in front of which are painted elliptical approach areas. (2) Duckling on its way toward approach area on its left. (3) Duckling in approach area. (4) Duckling snuggling to curtain and orienting to nonvisible speaker broadcasting a maternal call of its own (or another) species. summary tabulation of the behavior of each bird in the experiment and allowed further statistical treatment of preference in terms of the chisquare test (between conditions) and the binomial test (within conditions). The latter almost always agreed with the duration measure described earlier and thus lent strength to the results, whether for a preference or the absence of a preference in the choice test. A preference was indicated by a two-tailed p value of.05 or less. Ps greater than.05 were indicative of no preference. In all cases but one, the results of the binomial test and the Wilcoxon test were in agreement as to the presence or absence of a preference, so to conserve space and avoid repetition, only the former are reported in detail. The single exception concerned Group 5b's performance at 48 hr, for which the binomial was nonsignificant and the Wilcoxon indicated a mallard preference. To assess the "durability" of the 48-hr test results, each bird in Groups 4, 5a, and 5b was retested at 65 hr. Because exposure to the chicken call (and contact call) was terminated at 48 hr, the 65-hr test results give some indication of the enduring effect of the earlier experience. Results As can be seen in Table 2, the v o c a l - c o m m u n a l and VI birds showed, as expected, a u n a n i m o u s preference for the mallard call over the chicken call, whereas the unstimulated Dis did not show a preference. Furthermore, as predicted, the DI birds the chicken call showed a preference for the chicken call over the mallard call at 48 hr and continued to show that preference at 65 hr. Also as predicted, the VI birds the chicken call (Group 5a) did not develop a preference for it, as evidenced by their behavior in the 48- and 65-hr tests. The buffering effect o f the contact call was even more clearly in evidence in G r o u p 5h, in which the birds received explicit exposure to the contact call, as well as the chicken call: These birds preferred the mallard over the chicken call in both tests. The results from G r o u p 5a indicate a blockage o f the chicken preference by self-stimulation, but, to maintain the species-typical preference for the mallard call in the face o f exposure to the chicken call, exposure to contact calls produced by siblings is required (Group 5b). G r o u p 5b heard the broadcast o f only one sibling's calls, which would be considerably less than would occur under n o r m a l incubation and brooding conditions in nature. According to the chi-square test, when the preferences o f the groups are c o m p a r e d across conditions, the unstimulated vocal-intact (Group 2) birds showed a greater preference for the mallard call than did the unstimulated DI (Group 3) birds, As predicted, the DI birds that were the chicken call

38 GILBERT GOTTLIEB Table 2 Preferences of Mallard Ducklings & Simultaneous Auditory-Choice Test Preference N Mallard Chicken Condition N responded call call Both 1. Vocal-communal 30 24 24*** 0 0 2. Vocal-isolated 20 14 14"** 0 0 3. Devocal-isolated 83 43 26 15 2 4. Devocal-isolated, chicken call 26 21 3 17** 1 Retest 23 17 2 15 ** 0 5a. Vocal-isolated, chicken call 36 26 11 15 0 Retest 36 30 16 12 2 5b. Vocal-isolated, chicken and contact calls 41 31 19 12 0 Retest 38 28 20* 6 2 Note. Results of Conditions I and 3 are from Table 2 in "Development of Species Identification in Ducklings: IV. Change in Species-Specific Perception Caused by Auditory Deprivation" by G. Gonlieb, 1978, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 92, p. 380. Copyright 1978 by the American Psychological Association. In the first test of Group 5b, the binomial test is nonsignificant, whereas the Wilcoxon analysis of differences in approach duration scores favoring mallard is significant (p <.02). Otherwise, all Wilcoxon outcomes agree with the presence or absence o fpreference according to the binomial test. *p<.01. **p_<.004. ***p<.00006. (Group 4) showed a greater preference for it than did the unstimulated DI (Group 3) birds. As predicted, the stimulated DI (Group 4) birds also showed a greater preference for the chicken call than did the stimulated VI (Group 5b) birds, which showed a greater relative preference for the mallard call. For all of the above differences, p <.00006. Discussion To my knowledge, this is the first explicit demonstration of the canalizing influence of normally occurring sensory experience. Exposure of mallard ducklings to their variable-rate contact call not only fosters species-specific perceptual development (i.e., ensuring selective responsiveness to the maternal call of the species), it also buffers the duckling from becoming responsive to social signals from other species. In the absence of exposure to the contact call, the duckling is capable of becoming attached to the maternal call of another species even in the presence of its own species call (in simultaneous auditorychoice tests). My previous demonstration of malleability in devocalized mallard ducklings (Gottlieb, 1987a) involved the induction of a preference for either a chicken maternal call or a wood duck (Aix sponsa) maternal call; the birds were tested with the chicken versus wood duck calls, not the mallard maternal call. To be quite frank, I did not believe that it was possible to demonstrate malleability in the presence of the species- specific maternal call. It was only when I began to think of the possible canalizing effect of experience that it became apparent that devocalization might permit such a degree of malleability that exposure to an extraspecific maternal call would override the "innate" attractiveness of the species maternal call. Clearer thinking about what genes do and do not do in individual development gave rise to the idea that canalization must take place not only at the genetic level but at all levels of the developing system (Gottlieb, 1991a, Figure 1), including the developing organism's usually occurring experiences. The fact that canalizing influences are potentially present at all levels of the developing system has not been widely appreciated. Because the developmental ramifications of canalization were described fully in the foregoing article (Gottlieb, 1991a), I limit the present discussion to the evolutionary implications of canalization. Canalization and Evolution Canalizing influences account for developmental stability, so that what we think of as normal or typical for a species repeats itself generation after generation. For evolution to occur, through genetic mutation or otherwise, the canalizing influences associated with normal development must be overcome. Canalization is thus a conservative feature of individual development that prevents evolution from occurring in a ready fashion. Elsewhere (Gottlieb, 1987b), I took Kuo's (1976) idea of behavioral neophenotypes and applied it to the problem of evolution. In essence, the creation of a new behavioral phenotype (behavioral neophenogenesis) requires a change in normal or usual rearing circumstances that ordinarily function to canalize development along species-typical lines. An altered behavioral phenotype causes the affected individuals to change their way of life, exploiting new habitats, diets, and so on, which will eventually lead to morphological change and perhaps eventually to genetic change (through long-continued selective breeding among the descendants of the affected individuals). Behavioral change is widely considered to be the first step in the pathway to evolutionary change (Bonner, 1983; Hardy, 1965; Lamarck, 1809/1984; Larson, Prager, & Wilson, 1984; Leonovicovfi & Novfik, 1987; Piaget, 1978; Reid, 1985; Sewertzoff, 1929; Wyles, Kunkei, & Wilson, 1983), and behavioral change is most readily accomplished by overcoming or changing the experiential canalizing influences that are present in the usual course of individual development. Behavioral neophenogenesis allows evolution to proceed at a much more rapid rate than is the case when a species must await a severe environmental change or catastrophe such as oceans drying up or months of darkness to prune individuals that are capable of adapting to the change from those that are not. Such extreme environmental changes are rare and widely spaced in time compared with the frequency and tempo of evolution that could be instigated by behavioral neophenotypes. Thus, an understanding of the experiential canalization of behavioral development has potentially significant implications for our understanding of evolution (and especially the resistance to evolution). The present proposal is unconventional in relation to the neo-darwinian concept ofevolntion in that it postulates an extragenetic "behavioral mutation" rather

SPECIAL SECTION: EXPERIENTIAL CANALIZATION 39 than a genetic mutation as the instigator of evolutionary change. Along these lines, it has been demonstrated that certain extragenetic behavioral and morphological changes are "heritable" in the sense that they recur in subsequent generations (Clark & Galef, 1988; Denenberg & Rosenberg, 1967; Ho, 1984; Skolnick, Ackerman, Hofer, & Weiner, 1980). It is important to realize that although experimentally produced genetic mutations and selective breeding have produced many new varieties or subspecies, such mutations have never produced a new species, even when the experimental program extended over 30 years and involved tens of thousands of generations (e.g., Goldschmidt, 1933). Ever since the time of W. Bateson (1894), a minority of biologists have despaired of a new species evolving from a new variety that has been produced by genetic mutation and selective breeding. Thus, while I do not want to present the behavioral mutation idea as a mutually exclusive alternative to genetic mutation, I do think that it is worthwhile to consider the experiential dissolution of canalization and the behavioral route to evolutionary change as a potentially valid pathway. The behavioral-developmental pathway to evolutionary change is described in detail in Gottlieb (1991 b). References Bateson, W. (1894). Materials for the study of variation, treated with especial regard to discontinuity in the origin of species. London: Macmillan. Bonner, J. T. (1983). How behavior came to affect the evolution of body shape. Scientia, 118, 175-183. Clark, N. M., & Galef, B. G. (1988). Effects of uterine position on rate of sexual development in female mongolian gerbils. Physiology & Behavior, 42, 15-18. Denenberg, V. H., & Rosenberg, K. M. (1967). Nongenetic transmission of information. Nature, 216, 549-550. Goldschmidt, R. (1933). Some aspects of evolution. Science, 78, 539-547. Gottlieb, G. (1971). Development of species identification in birds. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gottlieb, G. (1975). Development of species identification in ducklings: I. Nature of perceptual deficit caused by embryonic auditory deprivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 89, 387-399. Gottlieb, G. (1978). Development of species identification in ducklings: IV. Change in species-specific perception caused by auditory deprivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 92, 375-387. Gottlieb, G. (1985). Development of species identification in ducklings: XI. Embryonic critical period for species-typical perception in the hatchling. Animal Behaviour, 33, 225-233. Gottlieb, G. (1987a). Development of species identification in ducklings: XIII. A comparison of malleable and critical periods of perceptual development. Developmental Psychobiology, 20, 393-404. Gottlieb, G. (1987b). The developmental basis of evolutionary change. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 101, 262-271. Gottlieb, G. (1991 a). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology 27, 4-13. Gottlieb, G. (1991 b). Individual development and evolution: The genesis of novel behavior. New York: Oxford University Press. Gottlieb, G., & Vandenbergh, J. G. (1968). Ontogeny of vocalization in duck and chick embryos. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 168, 307-325. Hardy, A. C. (1965). The living stream. London: Collins. Ho, M.-W (1984). Environment and heredity in development and evolution. In M.-W Ho & P. T. Saunders (Eds.), Beyondneo-Darwinism: An introduction to the new evolutionary paradigm (pp. 267-289). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Johnston, T. D., & Gottlieb, G. (1981). Development of visual species identification in ducklings: What is the role of imprinting? Animal Behaviour, 29, 1082-1099. Kuo, Z.-Y. (1976). The dynamics of behavior development (rev. ed.). New York: Plenum Press. Lamarck, J. B. (1984). Zoological philosophy Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1809). Larson, A., Prayer, E. M., & Wilson, A. C. (1984). Chromosomal evolution, speciation and morphological change in vertebrates: The role of social behavior. Chromosomes Today 8, 215-228. Leonovicovh, V., & Nov,~k, V. J. A. (Eds.). (1987). Behavioras oneofthe main factors of evolution. Praha, Czechoslovakia: Czechslovak Academy of Science. Miller, D. B. (1977). Social displays of mallard ducks (Anas platyrhynchos): Effects of domestication. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91, 221-232. Miller, D. B. (1980). Maternal vocal control of behavioral inhibition in mallard ducklings (Anas platyrhynchos). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 94, 606-623. Miller, D. B., & Gottlieb, G. (198 I). Effects of domestication on production and perception of mallard maternal alarm calls: Developmental lag in behavioral arousal. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 95, 205-219. Piaget, J. (1978). Behavior and evolution. New York: Pantheon Books. Reid, R. G. B. (1985). Evolutionary theory: The unfinished synthesis. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Sewertzoff, A. N. (1929). Directions of evolution. Acta Zoologica, 10, 59-141. Skolnick, N., Ackerman, S., Hofer, M. A., & Weiner, H. (1980). Vertical transmission of acquired ulcer susceptibility in the rat. Science, 208, 1161-1163. Wyles, J. S., Kunkel, J. G., & Wilson, A. C. (1983). Birds, behavior, and anatomical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 80, 4394-4397. Received October 10, 1989 Revision received March 5, 1990 Accepted July 16, 1990