An Evaluation of Respondent Conditioning Procedures to Decrease Barking in an Animal Shelter

Similar documents
IMPROVING IN-KENNEL PRESENTATION OF SHELTER DOGS THROUGH RESPONSE-DEPENDENT AND RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT TREAT DELIVERY ALEXANDRA PROTOPOPOVA

Canine Behavior and Acoustics

John Reilly. Canisius College. March 17, 2018

Life for Dogs Living in Shelters

Minimising pet stress at veterinary practice visits

Noise in the Animal Shelter Environment: Building Design and the Effects of Daily Noise Exposure

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

It s a (shelter) dog s life: Just how important is human contact?

Conflict-Related Aggression

Reducing Surrenders. Dayna Kennedy Shelter Manager Upper Peninsula Animal Welfare Shelter

An Evaluation of a Shelter Dog Training Class: Outcomes for Volunteer Trainers and for Dogs

The Value of Data Gary Patronek & Stephen Zawistowski Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

How do we assess for welfare of individuals? Can we have a generic welfare assessment? Will it work for all shelters?

TITLE: The effects of video footage versus photographs on perception of dog behavioral

Separation Anxiety Syndrome

A Comparison of Classical Counterconditioning and Differential Reinforcement of. Alternative Behavior on Aggressive Behavior in Dogs

The Kennel Club has long campaigned for a ban on the use and sale of electric shock collars in Scotland.

Higher National Unit Specification. General information for centres. Unit code: F3V4 34

Stockton Animal Shelter Operations. City Council May 23, 2017 Study Session

Sex, age and size as factors affecting the length of stay of dogs in Czech shelters. Jiří Žák, Eva Voslářová, Vladimír Večerek, Iveta Bedáňová

March 16, Guide's space recommendations as a minimum while always recognizing that performance standards also must be met.

The role of environmental and owner-provided consequences in canine stereotypy and

Care For Us Arc$c Wolf (Canis lupus arctos)

Companion Animal Welfare Student Activities

Birth and Death Rate Estimates of Cats and Dogs in U.S. Households and Related Factors

Housing for Health, Wellness and Success: Standards for Facility Design and Environment. What is a healthy environment made of?

SP6B NUISANCE BARKING IN DOGS AND THE WELFARE IMPLICATIONS OF DIFFERENT TREATMENT MODALITIES

Judging a Dog by Its Cover: Morphology but Not Training Influences Visitor Behavior toward Kenneled Dogs at Animal Shelters

Effects of interactive visitor encounters on the behaviour and welfare of animals commonly housed in Australian zoos

Bayne, K.A. (2002) Development of the human-research animal bond and its impact on animal well-being. ILAR Journal. 43(1) 1-9

Tug Dogs Canine History Form

for Assistance Elise R. Shore a, Charles Burdsal a & Deanna K. Douglas b a Psychology Department, Wichita State University

If they don t have a box they do their best to hide

Nathaniel J. Hall. Curriculum Vitae NW 7 th Ave 931 Center Drive Gainesville, Fl Gainesville, Fl 32611

Social Housing and Environmental Enrichment Policy

Proceeding of the LAVC Latin American Veterinary Conference Oct , 2010 Lima, Peru

IT S ALL ABOUT THE ANIMALS

Managing Separation Anxiety: An Evidence-Based Approach

Effect of Multidog Play Groups on Cortisol Levels and Behavior of Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) Housed in a Humane Society

Amelia J. Cook a & Emily McCobb a a Center for Animals and Public Policy, Cummings

Dog Behavior Problems Barking and Training Quiet

Dog Behavior and Training - Teaching Calm Settle and Relaxation Training

Title: The effects of audiobooks on the behaviour of dogs at a rehoming kennels

Reports hom the Research Laboratories... of the Department of Psychiatry University of Minnesota

Guide Dogs Puppy Development and Advice Leaflet. No. 3 Relief routines

Animal Enrichment Best Practice

Puppy Socialization and Fear Prevention

Outline CANINE ENRICHMENT: STRESS REDUCTION FOR HAPPY AND HEALTHY SHELTER DOGS. What is Environmental Enrichment?

TRAINING DOMESTIC DOGS (CANIS LUPUS FAMILIARIS) ON A NOVEL ODOR- DETECTION TASK IN DISCRETE TRIALS

The minimum age to adopt a pet is 21.

SPAY / NEUTER: IT S NOT JUST ABOUT KITTENS AND PUPPIES

Name: Spouse/Partner s Name: Address: Home Phone: City/State/Zip: Work Phone: Address: Cell Phone: TX DL # : Employer:

TOMPKINS COUNTY SOCIETY FOR THE PREVENTION OF CRUELTY TO ANIMALS

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Thursday, November 12, :30 pm 7:30 pm Gainey Ranch Golf Club Scottsdale, AZ

Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by owners of dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

Enrichment...on a Dime

Companion Animal Management in Victoria

The S Files Success with Maria: Sunshine: Biting Reported by S.G. Friedman, PhD and L. McGuire

SECTION P EXEMPTION FROM ANIMAL WELFARE STANDARDS (AMENDMENT) This application is used to request an exemption from animal welfare standards.

Dealing With Territorial and Protective Aggression

FEAR-FREE HOSPITAL DESIGN GUIDELINE Heather E. Lewis, AIA, NCARB. Draft / January 2015 PREPARED BY. architecture animals people

I Thought Your Ankle Was a Mouse! Human-Directed Aggression in the Cat Sharon L. Crowell-Davis DVM, PhD, DACVB Professor of Behavioral Medicine

Thursday, November 12, :30 pm 7:30 pm Gainey Ranch Golf Club Scottsdale, AZ

Managing separation anxiety in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) Discusses the efficacy of behavioural modification, auditory and olfactory

AnimalShelterStatistics

TRAINING & BEHAVIOR QUESTIONNAIRE

Shelter Guidelines Project. Shelter Guidelines - Content

Desensitization and Counter Conditioning

Management of bold wolves

University of Washington Live Mascot Family Application

Published online: 05 Dec 2007.

Hsin-Yi Weng a & Lynette A. Hart b a Department of Pathobiology, College of Veterinary

COUNTY OF ALBEMARLE STAFF REPORT SUMMARY

Behaviour of dogs adopted from an animal shelter. Svatava Vitulová, Eva Voslářová, Vladimír Večerek, Iveta Bedáňová

Protopopova & Wynne 1 ADOPTER-DOG INTERACTIONS AT THE SHELTER: BEHAVIORAL AND CONTEXTUAL PREDICTORS OF ADOPTION. Alexandra Protopopova a,

AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF. Vanessa M. Hajek for the Master of Science

Anthony Richard/Kendra Richard 6885 Mesa Ridge Pkwy. #169 Fountain, CO Phone: March 19, 2018

Veterinary Care for Shelter Pets

Community Report. Community Report. Proudly supporting the following community initiatives

6. SPAY/NEUTER: FINANCIAL ASSISTANCE PROGRAMS FOR PET CARETAKERS LIVING IN POVERTY-- WE CAN T GET TO ZERO WITHOUT THEM

Accepted Manuscript. The effect of different genres of music on the stress levels of kennelled dogs. A. Bowman, F.J. Dowell, N.P. Evans, Scottish SPCA

Surveys of the Street and Private Dog Population: Kalhaar Bungalows, Gujarat India

Kennel Management: Cats Provide separate cat wards Feliway TM plug-in Through a Dog s Ear music Keep fearful cats on top cages Provide hiding options

SHELTER DOGS AND THEIR DESTINY A RETROSPECTIVE ANALYSIS TO IDENTIFY PREDICTIVE FACTORS: A PILOT STUDY

Personal Information Name Age Physical Address

To choke or not to choke How positive reinforcement has affected the use of choke collars in dog training

HOPE. Comfort in times of crisis. Animal-Assisted Crisis Response.

Tips on Report Writing

Owner s Manual DOGS (3647)

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

Cat Adoption Criteria

Cats in Canada A five year review of overpopulation

The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs

Dog Behavior Problems House Soiling

The Effect of Play Group on the Behavior of Shelter Dogs

180 Degree Rescue Canine Adoption Contract

ANS 490-A: Ewe Lamb stemperament and Effects on Maze Entry, Exit Order and Coping Styles When Exposed to Novel Stimulus

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY THE EFFECT OF A WATER FLOW TIMER ON FOODSERVICE FOOD HANDLERS HANDWASHING BEHAVIOR

Transcription:

2017 Vol. 3 19-24 An Evaluation of Respondent Conditioning Procedures to Decrease Barking in an Animal Shelter Payen, S. W*. and Assemi, K.S. Abstract A common problem behavior in animal shelters is excessive noise from barking, which can regularly exceed 100dBs. Noise levels in animal shelters are correlated with increased stress in dogs, which may lead to increased problem behavior and a decrease in adoption. The purpose of the current study was to evaluate the use of respondent conditioning procedures to reduce barking noise level in an animal shelter by pairing a door chime with edible items. Following a baseline and neutral stimulus phase, the door chime was paired with edible items over a period of three weeks. Following this pairing phase, the pairing was stopped to determine if the door chime would act as a conditioned stimulus and reduce barking. These procedures were replicated following an additional baseline phase. Overall, the procedure was effective in reducing the noise level of the kennel area as compared to baseline levels. Implications and future research areas are discussed. HIGHLIGHTS Many animals relinquished to shelters are relinquished due to problem behavior. A common problem behavior in dog shelters is barking. Researchers tested the effectiveness of a simple respondent conditioning procedure in order to reduce the noise level of barking in a dog shelter. Pet Behaviour Science 2017, Vol.3, 19 24 DOI: 10.21071/pbs.v0i3.5758 Payne S. W*. and Assemi, K.S. California State University, Fresno USA * Corresponding author: spayne@csufresno.edu Paper Research Keywords: Animal shelter, barking, dogs, respondent conditioning The overall noise levels in the shelter decreased as a result of intervention. Reduction in noise may increase time adopters spend in dog area and increase adoptions. 1. INTRODUCTION Approximately 7.6 million animals are residing in animal shelters in the United States at any given time (American Society for the Protection and Care of Animals, 2016). In addition, approximately 25% of animals relinquished to animal shelters are relinquished due to engaging in some form of problem behavior (Kwan & Bain, 2013; Salman et al., 1998). Lepper, Kass, and Hart (2002) found that dogs relinquished to animal shelters due to behavior problems were less likely to be adopted than animals relinquished for non-behavioral reasons. Therefore, it may be important to reduce the problem behavior of animals in shelters, which could lead to increased adoption rates. Page 19

2017 Vol.3 19-24 www Pet Behaviour Science org One relatively common problem behavior in animal shelters is excessive barking. Sales et al. (1997) found that the noise level in dog kennels in animal shelters regularly reaches over 100 decibels (dbs), mainly due to excessive barking. Hearing damage in humans can occur at noise levels of 85 dbs or greater, which is well below the levels commonly seen at animal shelters (National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, 2016). In addition, excessive noise can cause both psychological and physical stress on subjects in animal shelter. (Coppola, Enns, & Grandin, 2006). Due to this increased stress, it is possible that animals may engage in higher levels of problem behavior (Dreschel & Granger, 2005), which may decrease their chances of adoption. Despite excessive barking being a relatively common problem at animal shelters, little research has been conducted to attempt to reduce excessive noise in kennel areas. Several studies have shown that playing different genres of music (e.g., classical, reggae, and soft rock) in animal shelters can reduce the amount of barking in the short term (e.g., Bowman Kogan, Schoenfeld-Tacher, & Simon, 2012; Wells, Graham, & Hepper, 2000). However, Bowman et al. (2015) found that habituation to classical music occurred rapidly, and the behavioral effects did not maintain. Others have suggested redesigning shelters and shelter policies to reduce problem barking. Coppola et al. (2006) suggested redesigning shelter environments, including using soundproofing materials, in order to reduce the overall noise. In addition, Hewison et al. (2014) demonstrated that by restricting access to the dog kennel area to one adopter at a time with staff supervision, noise was substantially reduced. However, it should be noted that many animal shelters depend on public funding or donations, and may not have the financial resources to redesign their kennel areas or the staff necessary to restrict access to the kennel area. Therefore, solutions that require fewer monetary and staff resources may be more beneficial to animal shelters. Protopopova and Wynne (2015) conducted a study to increase appropriate behavior that was correlated with adoption in the kennel area of an animal shelter. Across two experiments, the authors assessed the effects of a response-dependent reinforcement procedure (Differential Reinforcement of Other Behavior [DRO]) and a response-independent procedure (Noncontingent Reinforcement [NCR]). In the DRO condition, experimenters would stand at the front of the kennel and wait for the dog to stop engaging in problem behavior (including barking). The experimenter would then deliver an edible item to the dog. In the NCR condition, the experimenter would stand at the front of the kennel and deliver an edible item, regardless of the dog s behavior. The authors found that both procedures were equally effective in reducing problem behavior. The authors suggested that NCR might be a good method for decreasing problem behavior, because it requires no special training in behavioral observation. The authors also suggested that the effects of the NCR procedure might be due to respondent conditioning, and that the pairing of the edible item with the approaching experimenter may have elicited responding that was incompatible with problem behavior. It is possible that pairing the edible item with another stimulus, such as a door chime, could alleviate the need for an additional person, especially given the potential staffing issues in animal shelters. The purpose of the current study was to extend the results of Protopopova and Wynne (2015) by explicitly conditioning a previously neutral stimulus (a door chime) with the delivery of edible items (unconditioned stimulus) to determine if the door chime would elicit reduced levels of barking in an animal shelter following the conditioning procedure. 2. METHODS Subjects Subjects were 50 dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) of various breeds and ages (range: 6 months to 12 years), including mixed breeds. The mean length of stay in the shelter for dogs in the study was 6 months (range: 2 weeks to 5 years). All dogs in the shelter were spayed/neutered. Throughout the course of the study, several dogs were adopted and/or introduced to the animal shelter, but the global population remained stable. No steps were taken to control for a stable population throughout the study, as we did not want to prevent the adoption or intake of any shelter dogs. Page 20

Respondent Conditioning To Reduce Barking Payne and Assemi Additionally, most animal shelters have regular intakes/adoptions, and thus the results of the current study would have more generality to other shelters where this is commonplace. Setting All sessions took place in the kennel area of a local nokill animal shelter in Fresno, California. The kennel area contained 26 1m x 4m kennels with access to an outdoor kennel area of the same size. Dogs could move freely between the outdoor area and indoor area throughout the observation times in the study. The kennels had opaque concrete walls on two sides, and chain-link doors at either end. Dogs were housed between one to four dogs per kennel depending on size and temperament, although a majority of the dogs were housed alone. Kennels were situated in such a way that dogs could see other dogs in the kennel across from them. Kennels contained either raised-platform beds or soft beds. No enrichment items were provided to the dogs per shelter policy. Kennels were cleaned twice daily prior to the shelter opening (8:00AM-10:00AM) and after the shelter closed to the public (6:00PM- 8:00PM). Dogs were fed once per day following the closure of the shelter to the public. The kennel area had three doors, one of which could be entered by the general public and two that were restricted to shelter staff and volunteers. Response Definition and Measurement The dependent variable in the study was the noise level due to barking. Barking was measured in decibels (dbs) continuously on a second-by-second basis using a Wensn Digital Sound Level Meter (item number WS1361C; manufactured in the U.S.A.) with a range of 30dB to 130dB. For all sessions, the sound level meter was placed in an unobtrusive location near the center of the kennel area. The sound level meter saved data to a PC based computer program. Equipment Throughout the study, a Wensn Digital Sound Level Meter (described above) was used to record the sound level in the kennel area. Additionally, three SABRE digital door chimes (Item number HS-DWA2; manufactured in China) were used in the Neutral Stimulus, Pairing Procedure, and Post-Pairing conditions. The door chimes consisted of two parts, a sensor and receiver. The sensor was attached to the door and the receiver was attached to the doorframe on all of the entrances to the kennel area. When the door was opened, the sensor and receiver were separated and the door chime emitted a 120 db tone for 0.5 s. Procedures All sessions began when the sound level meter was activated and lasted for 5 minutes. Following the activation of the sound level meter, the experimenter left the kennel and did not return until the end of the session. Sessions were conducted at varying times per day during the operating hours of the animal shelter when the shelter was open to the public (10:00am- 4:30pm), with one to three sessions per day, three to five days per week. Care was taken to ensure that no single time period was favored in the recording. An ABCAC (A: Basline; B: Neutral Stimulus; C: Post- Pairing) reversal design was used for experimental control. Baseline. During baseline, no programmed stimuli were in place in the kennel area. Sound levels were recorded for the duration of the sessions. Neutral Stimulus (NS). During the neutral stimulus condition, a digital door chime was placed on all three access points into the kennel area such that the door would chime when any door was opened. There was no programmed pairing of this stimulus with any other stimulus under the control of the experimenter. Pairing Procedure. Following the NS condition, the pairing procedure began. During this procedure, the experimenter walked to the front of each kennel. The experimenter activated the digital door chime and provided an edible treat (which varied based on the treats available from donations to the shelter; e.g., Milk- Bone Soft and ChewyTM, Bil-Jac Original Recipe, Pup-Peroni ) to each dog in the kennel. Following the delivery of the edible treat, the door chime was sounded two additional times during consumption to provide additional pairings of the chime with the treats. Each dog throughout the study accepted all Page 21

2017 Vol.3 19-24 www Pet Behaviour Science org treats. This procedure was conducted once per day, three-to-five days per week for three weeks (for a total of 45 pairings). During the replication of this phase, the procedure was conducted once per day, three-to-five days per week for one week. During this condition, the dogs consumed the treats throughout the time of the two additional chimes. Post-Pairing (CS). The procedures of the CS condition were identical to the NS condition, with the exception that the pairing procedure was conducted prior to the CS condition. Data Analysis Data were analyzed throughout the course of the study using visual analysis of the graphical depiction of the data. Data were also summarized using means, Tukey s tri-mean, and ranges for each phase. Statement of Approval The procedures described in this manuscript were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at California State University, Fresno. Figure 1. The mean dbs of noise per session in the kennel area across conditions. The dotted line represents the threshold at which hearing damage in humans can occur following prolonged exposure. Conflict of Interests Statement No conflicts of interest were present in the conducting of this study. 3. RESULTS and DISCUSSION The results of the study suggest that the respondent conditioning procedure was effective in decreasing the noise intensity of the kennel area (see Figures 1 and 2). During baseline and the NS condition, the noise of the kennel area was variable across sessions, but maintained at relatively high levels (X = 85.1 dbs; TM= 82.6 dbs; range= 55.3 dbs-111.4 dbs), often above the threshold for damage to hearing in humans (85 dbs). Following the pairing procedure, the noise level reduced to relatively low levels in the CS condition (X = 68.1 dbs; TM= 67.8 dbs; range= 36.1 dbs-106.1 dbs), with noise levels comparable to normal speech level. We next returned to baseline and sound levels initially remained at relatively low levels, possibly due to carryover from the previous condition (i.e., stimulus control), but gradually increased to levels similar to the initial baseline (X = 71.3 dbs; TM= 70.9 dbs; range = 46.4 dbs- 106.3 dbs). Finally, we conducted the pairing procedure for one week, and in the subsequent CS condition, we saw the noise levels decrease to levels similar to the initial CS condition (X = 70.1 dbs; TM= 70.8 dbs; range= 48.4 dbs-102.5 dbs). Page 22

Respondent Conditioning To Reduce Barking Payne and Assemi Overall, the results were similar to those of Protopopova and Wynne (2015). The non-contingent delivery of edible items paired with the door chime reduced barking behavior in the animal shelter when only the door chime was present. In addition, mean noise levels were reduced below the threshold of hearing damage in humans. The decrease in noise in the kennel area of the animal shelter may reduce physiological stress on the animals and humans in the kennel area. It is also possible that potential adopters may spend more time in the kennel area and be more likely to adopt an animal. Figure 2. The mean dbs of noise in the kennel area between the baseline and post-pairing conditions. There are several possible mechanisms by which barking was reduced. First, following the pairing of the door chime and the edible items, it is possible that the door chime became a conditioned stimulus that elicited responding that was incompatible with barking (such as salivation). Second, the door chime, through its pairing with the edible item, may have become a conditioned reinforcer. It is possible that humans entering the kennel area were previously aversive to the dogs, and barking may have been maintained by negative reinforcement in the form of escape from humans. Following the pairing of the door chime with the edible item, it is possible that the door chime served as an abolishing operation for the escape-maintained behavior and reduced the aversiveness of humans entering the kennel area and the kennel area in general (similar to enrichment). Future researchers should evaluate these specific mechanisms to determine the mechanism by which the pairing reduced barking. There are a few limitations to the current study. First, our data collection mechanism did not discriminate between barking and other sounds in the kennel area. Anecdotally, almost all of the sound in the kennel area was from barking, and thus it is unlikely that other extraneous noise influenced the results in a meaningful way. Second, dogs were adopted and introduced throughout the course of the study. Although not documented, it is possible that dogs were introduced following the pairing procedures, and thus the dogs had never come into contact with the edible item paired with the door chime. Third, the housing of other dogs was not controlled. While this may have increased the ecological validity of the current study, it is also possible that the presence of other dogs in the same kennel may have influenced levels of barking during the study. Fourth, the respondent conditioning procedure used in this study did not specifically target an unconditioned response, so it was unclear by what mechanism the barking decreased. Future researchers should continue to evaluate respondent conditioning procedures to reduce problem behavior and increase appropriate behavior in shelter animals. Protopopova and Wynne (2015) found that the response-independent delivery of edible items was effective at reducing many different problem behaviors. It is possible that other problem behaviors were reduced in the current study, but due to the nature of measurement we did not obtain this information. Therefore, additional measures including observational measures should be used to determine if respondent conditioning procedures have effects on other potentially problematic behaviors in addition to barking in animal shelters. Additionally, future researchers should train animal shelter staff and volunteers to use these procedures. It is possible that pairing procedure could be conducted within the normal feeding routines of the shelter and may help maintain the effects of the respondent conditioning procedure. Future researchers should also conduct social validity measures to determine if the procedures decrease problem behavior and increased the perceived adoptability of the animals in a socially significant way. Finally, future researchers should determine the effects of various procedures to reduce problem behavior on the overall adoption rates of animals in animal shelters. Page 23

2017 Vol.3 19-24 www Pet Behaviour Science org 4. REFERENCES American Society for the Protection and Care of Animals (2016). Pet Statistics. Retrieved from http://www.aspca.org/animal-homelessness/shelterintake-and-surrender/pet-statistics. Bowman, A., Dowell, F. J., Evans, N.P., & Scottish SPCA (2015). Four seasons in a rescue centre: classical music reduces environmental stress in kenneled dogs. Physiology & Behavior, 143, 70-82. Bowman, A., Scottish SPCA, Dowell, F. J., & Evans, N. P. (2017). The effect of different genres of music on the stress level of kenneled dogs. Physiology and Behavior, 171, 207-215. Coppola, C. L., Enns, R. M., & Grandin, T. (2006). Noise in the animal shelter environment: Building design and the effects of daily noise exposure. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 9, 1-7. Dreschel, N. A., & Grander, D. A. (2005). Physiological and behavioral reactivity to stress in thunderstormphobic dogs and their caregivers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 95, 153-168. Hewison, L. F., Wright, H. F., Zulch, H. E., & Ellis, S. L. H. (2014). Short term consequences of preventing visitor access to kennels on noise and the behavior and physiology of dogs housed in a rescue shelter. Physiology & Behavior, 133, 1-7. Kogan, L.R., Schoenfeld-Tacher, R., & Simon, A.A. (2012). Behavioral effects of auditory stimulation on kenneled dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 7, 268-275. Kwan, J. Y., & Bain, M. J., (2013). Owner attachment and problem behaviors related to relinquishment and training techniques of dogs. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 16, 168-183. Lepper, M., Kass, P. H., & Hart, L. A. (2002). Prediction of adoption versus euthanasia among dogs and cats in a California animal shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 5, 29-42. National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (2016). Noise-induced hearing loss. Retrieved from https://www.nidcd.nih.gov/health/noise-inducedhearing-loss. Protopopova, A., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2015). Improving in-kennel presentation of shelter dogs through response-dependent and response-independent treat delivery. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 590-601. Sales, G., Hubrecht, R., Peyvandi, A., Milligan, S., & Shield, B. (1997). Noise in dog kenneling: Is barking a welfare problem for dogs. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 52, 321-329. Salman, M.D., New, J.C., Scarlett, J.M., Kass, P.H., Hetts, S., Ruch-Gallie, R. (1998). Human and animal factors related to the relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected animal shelters in the USA. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 1, 207-226. Wells, D. L., Graham, L, & Hepper, P. G. (2002). The influence of auditory stimulation on the behaviour of dogs housed in a rescue shelter. Animal Welfare, 11, 385-393. Page 24