The navigational feats of green sea turtles migrating from Ascension Island investigated by satellite telemetry

Similar documents
Navigation by green turtles: which strategy do displaced adults use to find Ascension Island?

Bibliografia. Bjorndal K. A. (1985). Nutritional ecology of sea turtles. Coepia, 736

Dive-depth distribution of. coriacea), loggerhead (Carretta carretta), olive ridley (Lepidochelys olivacea), and

BBRG-5. SCTB15 Working Paper. Jeffrey J. Polovina 1, Evan Howell 2, Denise M. Parker 2, and George H. Balazs 2

BIODIVERSITY CONSERVATION AND HABITAT MANAGEMENT Vol. II Initiatives For The Conservation Of Marine Turtles - Paolo Luschi

Migrations of green turtles in the central South Pacific

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

PERCEPTION OF OCEAN WAVE DIRECTION BY SEA TURTLES

DETECTION OF MAGNETIC INCLINATION ANGLE BY SEA TURTLES: A POSSIBLE MECHANISM FOR DETERMINING LATITUDE

Marine Conservation Society of Seychelles, P.O. Box 445, Victoria, Mahe, Seychelles 2

Navigation and seasonal migratory orientation in juvenile sea turtles

Title Temperature among Juvenile Green Se.

PROCEEDINGS OF THE TWENTY-THIRD ANNUAL SYMPOSIUM ON SEA TURTLE BIOLOGY AND CONSERVATION

Proceedings of the 2nd Internationa. SEASTAR2000 Workshop) (2005):

Representation, Visualization and Querying of Sea Turtle Migrations Using the MLPQ Constraint Database System

Post-nesting movements and submergence patterns of loggerhead marine turtles in the Mediterranean assessed by satellite tracking

RWO 166. Final Report to. Florida Cooperative Fish and Wildlife Research Unit University of Florida Research Work Order 166.

Notes on Juvenile Hawksbill and Green Thrtles in American Samoa!

Bycatch records of sea turtles obtained through Japanese Observer Program in the IOTC Convention Area

Swim speed and movement patterns of gravid leatherback sea turtles (Dermochelys coriacea) at St Croix, US Virgin Islands

BRITISH INDIAN OCEAN TERRITORY (BIOT) BIOT NESTING BEACH INFORMATION. BIOT MPA designated in April Approx. 545,000 km 2

Insights into the management of sea turtle internesting area through satellite telemetry

D irections. The Sea Turtle s Built-In Compass. by Sudipta Bardhan

Behavioural plasticity in a large marine herbivore: contrasting patterns of depth utilisation between two green turtle (Chelonia mydas) populations

ORIENTATION TO OCEANIC WAVES BY GREEN TURTLE HATCHLINGS

Appendix F27. Guinea Long Term Monitoring of the Marine Turtles of Scott Reef Satellite Tracking of Green Turtles from Scott Reef #1

Marine Turtles Use Geomagnetic Cues during Open-Sea Homing

HYDRODYNAMIC EFFECT OF A SATELLITE TRANSMITTER ON A JUVENILE GREEN TURTLE (CHELONIA MYDAS)

Yonat Swimmer, Richard Brill, Lianne Mailloux University of Hawaii VIMS-NMFS

Who Really Owns the Beach? The Competition Between Sea Turtles and the Coast Renee C. Cohen

MAGNETIC ORIENTATION AND NAVIGATION BEHAVIOR OF LOGGERHEAD SEA TURTLE HATCHLINGS (Caretta caretta) DURING THEIR TRANSOCEANIC MIGRATION

Jesse Senko, 2,8,9 Melania C. López-Castro, 3,4,8 Volker Koch, 5 and Wallace J. Nichols 6,7

A Sea Turtle's. by Laurence Pringle illustrated by Diane Blasius

Migration of green turtles Chelonia mydas from Tortuguero, Costa Rica

Green Turtle (Chelonia mydas) nesting behaviour in Kigamboni District, United Republic of Tanzania.

METHODS FOR PRODUCING DISTURBANCES IN PIGEON HOMING BEHAVIOUR BY OSCILLATING MAGNETIC FIELDS

BIBLIOGRAPHY OF TERRESTRIAL BASKING AND NON-NESTING EMERGENCE IN MARINE TURTLES

HOMING BEHAVIOUR OF PIGEONS SUBJECTED TO UNILATERAL ZINC SULPHATE TREATMENT OF THEIR OLFACTORY MUCOSA

Sea Turtles and Longline Fisheries: Impacts and Mitigation Experiments

SEA TURTLE MOVEMENT AND HABITAT USE IN THE NORTHERN GULF OF MEXICO

Effect of tagging marine turtles on nesting behaviour and reproductive success

B E L I Z E Country Report. WIDECAST AGM FEB 2, 2013 Linda Searle ><> Country Coordinator

Allowable Harm Assessment for Leatherback Turtle in Atlantic Canadian Waters

Climate change and sea turtles: a 150-year reconstruction of incubation temperatures at a major marine turtle rookery

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

PROJECT DOCUMENT. Project Leader

Honu : our turtle tracked by satellites... Te mana o te moana supported by NOAA in its turtle research programs...(may 17 th 2011)

Title. Grade level. Time. Student Target. PART 3 Lesson: Populations. PART 3 Activity: Turtles, Turtle Everywhere! minutes

Home Range as a Tool for Conservation Efforts of Sea Turtles at the north Pacific coast of Costa Rica

The Seal and the Turtle

SEA TURTLE CHARACTERISTICS

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

Marine Turtle Research Program

Aspects in the Biology of Sea Turtles

Rookery on the east coast of Penins. Author(s) ABDULLAH, SYED; ISMAIL, MAZLAN. Proceedings of the International Sy

D. Burke \ Oceans First, Issue 3, 2016, pgs

Variability in Reception Duration of Dual Satellite Tags on Sea Turtles Tracked in the Pacific Ocean 1

Dr Kathy Slater, Operation Wallacea

Title. Grade level. Time. Student Target. Materials. PART 2 Lesson: Nesting. PART 2 Activity: Are you my Mother? minutes

PROJECT DOCUMENT. This year budget: Project Leader

Habitat utilization by juvenile hawksbill turtles (Eretmochelys imbricata, Linnaeus, 1766) around a shallow water coral reef

REPORT Annual variation in nesting numbers of marine turtles: the effect of sea surface temperature on re-migration intervals

Tristan Darwin Project. Monitoring Guide. A Guide to Monitoring Albatross, Penguin and Seal Plots on Tristan and Nightingale

Migration routes and foraging behaviour of olive ridley turtles Lepidochelys olivacea in northern Australia

Dugong movements Current knowledge and tracking tools

Biology Of Sea Turtles, Vol. 1

EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name

American Samoa Sea Turtles

Marine Debris and its effects on Sea Turtles

Impacts of coastal development on hawksbill hatchling survival and swimming success during the initial offshore migration

Voyage of the Turtle

Proceedings of the International Sy. SEASTAR2000 Workshop) (2004):

GREEN TURTLE MIGRATIONS IN THE HAWAIIAN ARCHIPELAGO

Size, growth, and reproductive output of adult female leatherback turtles Dermochelys coriacea

I. Introduction. Orientation and Navigation 3/8/2012. Most difficult problem Must know. How birds find their way. Two terms often misused

Status of olive ridley sea turtles (Lepidochelys olivacea) in the Western Atlantic Ocean

Steve Russell. George Balazs. Scott Bloom Norie Murasaki

EFFECTS OF THE DEEPWATER HORIZON OIL SPILL ON SEA TURTLES

Gulf and Caribbean Research

Growth analysis of juvenile green sea turtles (Chelonia mydas) by gender.

Nature Watch. Tracking Turtles through Time and Space. Kartik Shanker I ARTICLE

Your web browser (Safari 7) is out of date. For more security, comfort and the best experience on this site: Update your browser Ignore

Since 1963, Department of Fisheries (DOF) has taken up a project to breed and protect sea Turtles on Thameehla island.

Movement patterns of green turtles in Brazilian coastal waters described by satellite tracking and flipper tagging

--- By --- Joshua Frazier Hanover. March 21 st, 2017

OKUYAMA, JUNICHI; SHIMIZU, TOMOHITO OSAMU; YOSEDA, KENZO; ARAI, NOBUAKI. Proceedings of the 2nd Internationa. SEASTAR2000 Workshop) (2005): 63-68

Great Science Adventures Lesson 12

1995 Activities Summary

Sea Turtle, Terrapin or Tortoise?

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

Woodcock: Your Essential Brief

SHORT NOTE THE INCIDENTAL CAPTURE OF FIVE SPECIES OF SEA TURTLES BY COASTAL SETNET FISHERIES IN THE EASTERN WATERS OF TAIWAN

Nature Watch. The Ancient Mariners. Kartik Shanker

9-12 Sea Turtle Survivorship Activity

PROCEEDINGS OF THE TWENTY-THIRD ANNUAL SYMPOSIUM ON SEA TURTLE BIOLOGY AND CONSERVATION

LOGGERHEADLINES FALL 2017

CLEANING SYMBIOSIS AND DIEL BEHAVIOR OF GREEN TURTLES (CHELONIA MYDAS) AT PUAKO, HAWAII

Caretta caretta - Urgent conservation measures of Caretta caretta in the Pelagian Islands LIFE99 NAT/IT/006271

DOWNLOAD OR READ : SEA TURTLES ANIMALS THAT LIVE IN THE OCEAN PDF EBOOK EPUB MOBI

Transcription:

The navigational feats of green sea turtles migrating from Ascension Island investigated by satellite telemetry P. Luschi 1, G. C. Hays 2, C. Del Seppia 3, R. Marsh 4, and F. Papi 1* 1 Dipartimento di Etologia, Ecologia, Evoluzione, University of Pisa,Via A.Volta 6, I-56126 Pisa, Italy 2 School of Biological Sciences, University of WalesöSwansea, Singleton Park, Swansea SA2 8PP, UK 3 Centro di Studio per la Faunistica ed EcologiaTropicali del CNR,Via Romana 17, I-50125 Firenze, Italy 4 James Rennel Division, Southampton Oceanography Centre, Empress Dock, Southampton SO14 3ZH, UK Previous tagging studies of the movements of green turtles (Chelonia mydas) nesting at Ascension Island have shown that they shuttle between this remote target in the Atlantic Ocean and their feeding grounds on the Brazilian coast, a distance of 2300 km or more. Since a knowledge of sea turtle migration routes might allow inferences on the still unknown navigational mechanisms of marine animals, we tracked the postnesting migration of six green turtle females from Ascension Island to Brazil. Five of them reached the proximity of the easternmost stretch of the Brazilian coast, covering 1777^2342 km in 33^47 days. Their courses were impressively similar for the rst 1000 km, with three turtles tracked over di erent dates following indistinguishable paths for the rst 300 km. Only the sixth turtle made some relatively short trips in di erent directions around Ascension. The tracks show that turtles (i) are able to maintain straight courses over long distances in the open sea; (ii) may perform exploratory movements in di erent directions; (iii) appropriately correct their course during the journey according to external information; and (iv) initially keep the same direction as the west-south-westerly owing current, possibly guided by chemical cues. Keywords: green turtles; migration; orientation; Ascension Island; sea currents 1. INTRODUCTION Sea turtles, like other marine animals, face formidable navigational challenges when they undertake long journeys for distant breeding or foraging towards areas that may be tiny oceanic islands or speci c spots of continental shores (Carr 1984). So far, the guidance mechanisms used by adult turtles have only been matter of speculation (see Papi & Luschi 1996). Green turtles (Chelonia mydas) nesting at Ascension Island (7857' S, 14822' W) are often cited as a paradigm since the recovery of 66 females tagged at the island with numbered ipper tags showed that they migrate to feeding grounds located on the Brazilian coast, a distance of 2300 km or more (Koch et al. 1969; Carr 1975; Mortimer & Carr 1987). As emphasized by the pioneer of sea turtle research, Archie Carr (Carr 1972, 1984), a rst step in understanding the underlying navigational mechanisms would be to identify the courses followed by these migrating turtles. This is now made possible by satellite telemetry, which has proved to be a reliable tool for tracking longdistance journeys of marine animals (Taillade 1993; McConnell & Fedak 1996; Morreale et al. 1996; Papi & Luschi 1996). In our case, tracking the journey from * Author for correspondence (papi@discau.unipi.it). Brazil to Ascension would have been the best approach to study the turtles' ability to pinpoint a small target, but this approach is hard to achieve due to the di culty in capturing females when they are just about to leave Brazil. However, tracking the reverse journey could also be helpful to study turtle navigational skills, since green turtles are also known to undertake postnesting migrations directed towards speci c targets (see, for instance, Balazs 1994, Balazs et al. 1994, Luschi et al. 1996). In the case of Ascension turtles, it might be expected that the rst target of migrating turtles would be the coastal waters of the easternmost part of the Brazilian bulge into the Atlantic (i.e. the coast between ca. 58 and 108 S). It is in fact reasonable that turtles leaving Ascension Island should move towards this area to reduce the length of their journey in the open sea, even when their coastal feeding grounds are located to the north or south. Indeed, most of the recovered females tagged at Ascension were from this area (Carr 1975, 1984; Mortimer & Carr 1987). Even if such a target is not as small as Ascension is for turtles migrating from Brazil, the reconstruction of the routes followed by postnesting turtles may still allow an evaluation of their navigational performance. This would then allow a re-examination of the hypotheses so far proposed about the navigational system used by these turtles to pinpoint Ascension, assuming that the same mechanisms guide the postnesting migration as well. 265, 2279^2284 2279 & 1998 The Royal Society Received 17 July 1998 Accepted 14 August 1998

2280 P. Luschi and others Green turtle migration from Ascension Island Figure 1. Migratory routes of the turtles B^F as they left Ascension Island for their postnesting migration towards Brazil. Filled symbols represent xes of the three most accurate classes (typically within 1 km of the true location), while open symbols represent the less accurate xes. Only xes considered valid are plotted in the gure (see text for details). The chart is constructed on equatorial Mercator's projection. 2. MATERIALS AND METHODS During April^July 1997, six female green turtles were located while nesting on Long Beach near Georgetown, Ascension Island (table 1). Immediately after egg laying, Telonics ST-14 satellite transmitters were attached to their carapace using standard methods (Balazs et al. 1996; Papi et al. 1997). The turtles were then localized through the Argos satellite system (Taillade 1993; French 1994), which classi ed their locations into six classes of decreasing accuracy (less than 1km for the rst three classes). The routes followed by the turtles have been reconstructed disregarding those xes of the lower three classes which were considered to be erroneous (33 out of 585) because they inferred either a swimming speed exceeding 5 km h 71 (nˆ25 xes; this threshold value was derived from speed values calculated from high-accuracy localizations only), or a swimming direction which di ered for more than 908 from those recorded in the preceding and following three days (n ˆ8 xes). These disregarded xes were thought to be due to inaccuracies in the locations provided by the Argos system. For each turtle, total distance covered was computed by adding the distances between successive valid xes. The straightness index was calculated as the ratio between the beeline distance from Long Beach to the last x of a turtle's route and the total length of the route (Batschelet 1981). To compare turtle performances in periods which had a di erent availability of orientating cues, we distinguished four types of segment between successive valid xes: those which had been travelled mostly (490%) during the day, or mostly during the night; those covered at night when the moon was above the horizon (moon ); and those when the moon was not above the horizon (moonless ). Turtles' speed and direction recorded in the di erent were compared by means of t- or U 2 -test (Batschelet 1981). Data from covered in less than 120 min were excluded. To assess the prevailing ocean currents in the study area, we used a primitive equation general circulation model called the global isopycnic model (GIM, Marsh et al. 1998). GIM is essentially a 1.258 resolution global version of the Atlantic model used in a previous study of juvenile loggerhead turtles (Hays & Marsh 1997). This mathematical model provided information on the direction and relative strength of the currents (see Marsh et al. (1998) and Hays & Marsh (1997) for fuller technical descriptions of the model). Table 1. Details of the six turtles for which postnesting migrations were tracked by satellite for 19^47 days (CCL, curved carapace length.) turtle release date 3. RESULTS CCL (cm) tracking duration (d) distance covered (km) straightness index A 27 April 115.5 19 969.0 0.13 B 12 May 120.0 35 2320.7 0.96 C 22 June 112.0 39 2346.6 0.95 D 25 June 110.0 33 1792.9 0.95 E 28 June 107.0 47 2095.0 0.97 F 2 July 117.0 36 2284.8 0.95 Five of the tracked turtles (B ^ F) directly headed towards Brazil ( gure 1), whereas turtle A moved around in di erent directions until the transmitter stopped emissions after 19 days ( gure 2). The routes clearly show that the turtles are able to maintain straight courses in the open sea and to move towards a common target. Turtle D, which left Ascension in a north-westerly direction, soon corrected her course in line with the others. A striking feature of tracks B^F is their coincidence, which is particularly marked in the rst 500 km that were covered in a west-south-western direction ( gures 1, 2). Turtles B^E continued in the same direction for a further 500 km, whereas turtle F turned and headed north-west. It is particularly impressive that turtles C, E and F followed indistinguishable paths for the rst 300 km even though they were tracked over di erent dates. Sooner or later, all of the turtles corrected their initial west-southwestern course and headed west-north-west. Turtles B and F later compensated again for the excess of the rst correction with the result that all ve tracks converged on the easternmost part of the Brazilian bulge ( gures 1, 3). The straightness index of the ve courses ranges between 0.95 and 0.97 (table 1), thus indicating similar navigational performances. The ve turtles were tracked for a large part of their journey to the bulge of Brazil (1770^ 2342 km), but the early cessation of the emissions of all

Green turtle migration from Ascension Island P. Luschi and others 2281 Figure 2. The rst part of the migratory journey of turtles B^F and the reconstructed route of turtle A. Other explanations are as in gure 1. Figure 3. Progressive adjustment of course of the turtles as they migrate towards Brazil. Symbols on the charts represent the site where the single turtles were expected to cross the 358 W meridian or to reach the coast if they maintained the mean direction held (a) before 208 W, (b) between 20^258 W, (c) between 25^308 W and (d) after 308 W (excluding turtle D, which did not cross the 308 W meridian). For turtles B and C, which were tracked up to the coast, the symbols in chart (d) show their last x. All the turtles progressively corrected their course while approaching Brazil to orientate towards the easternmost part of the bulge. transmitters prevented us from unveiling where the turtles' feeding grounds were located. The emissions lasted for much less than expected according to the speci cations of the transmitters and to our previous experience with transmitters of the same model (Luschi et al. 1996; Papi et al. 1997; Hughes et al. 1998). With the exception of turtle D, all the turtles maintained a slightly slower swimming speed during the night,

2282 P. Luschi and others Green turtle migration from Ascension Island Table 2. Turtle performances in terms of speed and direction in between successive valid xes (For each turtle, means.e. speed and mean vector direction and length in four di erent types of are reported. Mean heading calculations were not done for turtle A, who repeatedly changed her direction of movement. Other explanations are in the text. Vector length, which varies from zero to one, is inversely proportional to the extent of scatter of the segment directions (Batschelet 1981). (a) Shows data for turtles travelling 490% during the day or the night. (b) Compares migrations on nights where the moon was above the horizon (moon ) or not (moonless ). In (a) and (b), n day, n night, n moon and n moonless represent the number of di erent recorded.) (a) means.e. speed (km h 71 ) mean vector direction and length turtle (n day, n night) daytime night-time t-test daytime night-time U 2 -test A (20, 18) 3.13 0.11 2.85 0.15 p ˆ 0.143 ö ö ö B (28, 22) 3.16 0.08 2.83 0.08 p ˆ 0.006 2678, 0.96 2738, 0.95 p40.05 C (24, 25) 2.86 0.16 2.27 0.15 p ˆ 0.010 2798, 0.94 2788, 0.95 p40.05 D (15, 10) 2.82 0.15 2.91 0.14 p ˆ0.693 2608, 0.91 2528, 0.99 p40.05 E (39, 9) 2.51 0.12 1.65 0.27 p ˆ 0.004 2658, 0.96 2658, 0.90 p40.05 F (30, 28) 2.85 0.13 2.45 0.11 p ˆ 0.027 2808, 0.97 2828, 0.97 p40.05 (b) means.e. speed (km h 71 ) mean vector direction and length turtle (n moon, n moonless) moon moonless t-test moon moonless U 2 -test A (6, 12) 3.22 0.21 2.670.18 pˆ0.087 ö ö ö B (14, 8) 2.74 0.07 3.00 0.19 p ˆ 0.138 2708, 0.96 2788, 0.94 p40.05 C (17, 8) 2.40 0.20 1.99 0.15 p ˆ 0.214 2778, 0.93 2788, 0.98 p40.05 D (4, 6) 3.23 0.15 2.69 0.16 p ˆ 0.049 2498, 0.99 2548, 0.99 p40.05 E (5, 4) 2.03 0.37 1.19 0.24 p ˆ0.117 2708, 0.97 2588, 0.84 p40.05 F (18, 10) 2.21 0.13 2.89 0.12 p ˆ 0.001 2848, 0.97 2788, 0.97 p40.05 the di erence being signi cant in turtles B, C, E, and F (table 2). In none of the cases were signi cant di erences found between nocturnal and diurnal directions (table 2a). When the moon was absent, turtles carried on with apparently unchanged speed and heading (table 2b). 4. DISCUSSION The behaviour of turtle A is di cult to understand ( gure 2). It seems to be improbable that her 280^ 150 km-long trips in di erent directions represent wrong attempts to move towards Brazil. Unlike the other turtles we tracked, turtle A was a icted with a bropapilloma, but these tumours do not seem to produce the major anomaly in migratory behaviour that we observed (Balazs 1994). It may be possible that the turtle was exploring the waters around Ascension in search of new feeding grounds. The only safe conclusion about the navigational skills of this turtle is that she showed the ability of course reversal since she returned to the Ascension area after the trips she made. All the other turtles directly migrated towards the Brazilian bulge ( gure 1). It is impossible to know whether this was their nal target or just a stop-over area in the journey to more distant feeding grounds (see also Mortimer & Carr 1987). However, the reconstructed tracks allow some inferences to be drawn regarding the navigational mechanisms. The trips occurred over deep oceanic waters and thus in the absence of landmarks or detectable bathymetric features. In such conditions, covering long, straight journeys at least requires the usage of compass mechanism(s). It is very probable that marine turtles rely on a time-compensated sun-compass, which is widespread in animals and has also been demonstrated in freshwater turtles (De Rosa & Taylor 1980). As found in previous studies (Luschi et al. 1996; Hughes et al. 1998), the turtles were also able to keep their course at night when the moon was absent. Since they are too myopic in the air to see stars (Ehrenfeld & Koch 1967), it seems likely that they can orientate using non-visual cues. Indeed experimental studies, which have so far been limited to hatchlings, have shown that turtles have the ability to direct their movements in relation to both the earth's magnetic eld and wave direction (Lohmann & Lohmann 1996a). As described above, the turtles appropriately adjusted their course while migrating to orientate towards their target ( gure 3). This shows that Ascension turtles, besides one or more compasses, also rely on additional navigational tools based on external information picked up en route. This had already been suspected, considering that compass mechanisms alone could hardly allow turtles arriving from Brazil to nd their target in the middle of the Ocean (Koch et al. 1969; Carr 1984). In connection with this, two hypotheses have been put forward. According to Lohmann & Lohmann (1996b), adult turtles might retain the capacity they have as hatchlings of detecting di erences in magnetic eld intensity and inclination. If so, Ascension turtles could determine their position from the isoclinics and isodynamics that intersect at a great angle in the equatorial Atlantic Ocean, thus making up a bicoordinate grid. This could

Green turtle migration from Ascension Island P. Luschi and others 2283 theoretically allow turtles to determine their position over the entire journey, and to select the most appropriate route to orientate themselves towards their target. Reliance on such a mechanism of magnetic navigation, however, hardly explains the initial de ection of all our turtles towards the west-south-west, which leads them away from the shortest course towards Brazil. However, this de ection might be due to a drift produced by the South Atlantic equatorial current (SAEC, see later), for which turtles are initially unable to compensate. The second hypothesis was advanced by Carr and coworkers (Koch et al. 1969; Carr 1972, 1984) speci cally to address the question of how Ascension Island was pinpointed by the incoming turtles from Brazil. Chemical substances originating from the target island would be transported westwards by the SAEC at a speed of up to 1.8 km h 71, giving rise to an odour plume that might be detected by migrating turtles. Turtles would leave Brazil and head eastwards by means of a compass mechanism and, once they intersected the plume, they would remain within it until reaching the Ascension area. As shown by theoretical calculations, the features of the SAEC limit the dilution of the dissolved chemicals, thus maintaining their concentration high enough to be detected by a biological chemosensor, even at a large distance from the Island (Koch et al. 1969). The females on their rst reproductive migration would recognize the Ascension-derived chemical stimuli as the same one they had been imprinted with as hatchlings (Owens et al. 1986). Although it has been suggested that the SAEC may temporarily reverse its direction during the austral summer (i.e. while the pre-nesting migration to Ascension occurs; Brown 1990), we think that the existence of a chemical link between Ascension and Brazil needs to be considered at least in relation to the postnesting migration. Since the current ow during the rest of the year is known to be westerly, turtles might use the chemical cues from the plume to orientate even during the return trip to Brazil. At the time of year that we tracked the turtles, two buoy tracking studies (Reverdin & McPhaden 1986; Scha«fer & Krauss 1995) have shown that the surface current from Ascension ows in a direction of 255^2608, i.e. between west and west-south-west. Near-surface (model mixed layer) currents in the equatorial South Atlantic of the GIM are very similar in May, June and July ( gure 4). In common with the buoy studies, in these three months the model also showed a west-south-western current ow from Ascension towards South America, with this current tending to weaken with distance west and south ( gure 4). These empirical and modelling studies therefore suggest that during May and July the west-south-western current is a constant feature of the area close to Ascension. The directions followed by the turtles in the rst part of their trip show an impressive coincidence with the current, both being ca. 2608. To remain within the SAEC, turtles might rely on several cues which certainly characterize the current (e.g. temperature, salinity, etc.), but the fact that the courses, in the rst part of the journey, were con ned within a narrow corridor starting from Ascension, indicates that the turtles referred to cues derived directly from the island itself, such as the chemical substances borne by the current. The coincidence of the rst part of the routes can Figure 4. Tracks of turtles B^F superimposed to mixed layer currents in the equatorial South Atlantic of GIM after 30 years spin-up, during the middle of (a) May, (b) June, (c) July. Charts are equatorial Mercator's projections. thus be interpreted as evolving from the turtles' tendency to swim in the axis of the plume of Ascension-derived chemicals, by correcting their course when perceiving a decrease in the concentration of the current-borne substances. On average, turtles spent 490% of the time submerged with dive durations generally between about 4 and 16 min (G. C. Hays, P. Luschi, C. Del Seppia and F. Papi, unpublished data). These dives may potentially be important in exploring the depth pro le of the odour plume from Ascension. The occurrence of migratory corridors in sea turtles has also been observed in other areas (Balazs 1994; Balazs et al. 1994; Morreale et al. 1996; Balazs & Ellis 1998), but their relationship with currents has not been investigated in detail. As well as possibly providing navigational cues to aid migration, swimming with the prevailing current will also increase the turtles' speed of travel and so may help to minimize the energetic cost of the postnesting migration. As the distance from the source increases the canalization e ect of the plume will become less e ective since the steepness of the north ^ south gradient will decrease. This may explain why the turtle tracks began to diverge hundreds of kilometres downstream from Ascension. Moreover, in the second part of the journey, the turtles tended to abandon the stream of the current, which, if followed, would have led them too far south. It is uncertain whether these course changes might be due to a modi cation in the pattern of current-borne chemical cues or to a switching to orientational cues of a di erent nature. For

2284 P. Luschi and others Green turtle migration from Ascension Island instance, an approximate evaluation of the latitude, possibly based on magnetic cues (Lohmann & Lohmann 1996a), might account for these course corrections. In conclusion, the present results show that Ascension turtles use information picked up en route to adjust their migratory course, and suggest that current-borne cues, probably of a chemical nature, may be involved in this process for at least part of the journey. Swimming within the plume provided the turtles with a reliable and e cient guidance mechanism towards Brazil, even though the current direction diverged slightly from the shortest straight-line course to Brazil. Clearly, reliance on such a mechanism of navigation would only be possible in the particular case in which currents set up a sensory link between the rookery and the feeding grounds. In the other cases, turtles may rely on di erent navigational strategies, of unknown nature, to nd their way through the oceans. We are especially grateful to H. H. H. C. Huxley, Administrator of Ascension Island and to W. Wilkie, Vice Administrator, for permission to work at the island and for their encouragement and welcome during our stay. We also wish to thank E. Zambianchi (IUN Naples) and S. Stutzer (Institute fur Meereskunde Kiel) for their precious help in obtaining information about oceanic currents, S. Ghione (CNR Pisa) for his help in the eld. C. Vieitas and the people of the Pro-Tamar Foundation (Brazil), R. Mencacci, L. Morelli and E. Crosio (University of Pisa) helped us with technical assistance. The Global Isopycnic Model (GIM) used here was set up and run at the Southampton Oceanography Centre by Alex Megann and Adrian New. Two anonymous referees provided useful comments to an earlier version of the manuscript. This research was supported by the Italian CNR, NERC (UK), the Darwin Initiative (DETR, UK) and the Accademia Nazionale dei Lincei. REFERENCES Balazs, G. H. 1994 Homeward bound: satellite tracking of Hawaiian green turtles from nesting beaches to foraging pastures. In Proc. 13th Ann. Symp. Sea Turtle Biology and Conservation, NOAA Technical Memorandum NMFS-SEFSC- 341 (ed. B. A. Schroeder & B. E. Witherington), pp. 205^208. Washington, DC: National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. Balazs, G. H. & Ellis, D. M. 1998 Satellite telemetry of migrant male and female green turtles breeding in the Hawaiian Islands. In Proc. 16th Ann. Symp. SeaTurtle Biology and Conservation, NOAA Technical Memorandum. Washington, DC: National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. (In the press.) Balazs, G. H., Craig, P., Winton, B. R. & Miya, R. K. 1994 Satellite telemetry of green turtles nesting at French Frigate Shoals, Hawaii, and Rose Atoll, American Samoa. In Proc. 14th Ann. Symp. Sea Turtle Biology and Conservation, NOAA Technical Memorandum, NMFS-SEFSC-351 (ed. K. A. Bjorndal, A. B. Bolten, D. A. Johnson & P. J. Eliazar), pp. 184^187. Washington, DC: National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. Balazs, G. H., Miya, R. K. & Beavers, S. C. 1996 Procedures to attach a satellite transmitter to the carapace of an adult green turtle, Chelonia mydas. In Proc. 15th Ann. Symp. Sea Turtle Biology and Conservation, NOAA Technical Memorandum NMFS- SEFSC-387 (ed. J. A. Keinath, D. E. Barnard, J. A. Musick & B. A. Bell), pp. 21^26. Washington, DC: National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. Batschelet, E. 1981 Circular statistics in biology. New York: Academic Press. Brown, C. W. 1990 The signi cance of the South Atlantic equatorial countercurrent to the ecology of the green turtle breeding population of the Ascension Island. J. Herpetol. 24, 81^84. Carr, A. 1972 The case for long-range, chemoreceptive piloting in Chelonia. In Animal orientation and navigation, NASA SP-262 (ed. S. R. Galler, K. Schmidt-Koenig, G. J. Jacobs & R. E. Belleville), pp. 469^483. Washington, DC: NASA. Carr, A. 1975 The Ascension Island green turtle colony. Copeia 1975, 547^555. Carr, A. 1984 The sea turtle. So excellent a she. 2nd edn. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. De Rosa, C. T. & Taylor, D. H. 1980 Homeward orientation mechanisms in three species of turtles (Trionix spinifer, Chrysemys picta and Terrapene carolina). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7, 15^23. Ehrenfeld, D. W. & Koch, A. L. 1967 Visual accommodation in the green turtle. Science 155, 827^828. French, J. 1994 Wildlife telemetry by satellite. Endeavour 18, 32^37. Hays, G. C. & Marsh, R. 1997. Estimating the age of juvenile loggerhead sea turtles in the North Atlantic. Can. J. Zool. 75, 40^46. Hughes, G. R., Luschi, P., Mencacci, R. & Papi, F. 1998 The 7000 km oceanic journey of a leatherback turtle tracked by satellite. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 229, 209^217. Koch, A. L., Carr, A. & Ehrenfeld, D. W. 1969 The problem of open-sea navigation: the migration of the green turtle Chelonia mydas to Ascension Island. J.Theor. Biol. 22, 163^179. Lohmann, K. J. & Lohmann, C. M. F. 1996a Orientation and open-sea navigation in sea turtles. J. Exp. Biol. 199, 73^81. Lohmann, K. J. & Lohmann, C. M. F. 1996b Detection of magnetic eld intensity by sea turtles. Nature 380, 59^61. Luschi, P., Papi, F., Liew, H. C., Chan, E. H. & Bonadonna, F. 1996 Long-distance migration and homing after displacement in the green turtle (Chelonia mydas): a satellite tracking study. J. Comp. Physiol. A178, 447^452. McConnell, B. J. & Fedak, M. A. 1996 Movements of southern elephant seals. Can. J. Zool. 74, 1485^1496. Marsh, R., Nurser, A. J. G., Megann, A. P., & New, A. L. 1998 Water mass transformation in the Southern Ocean of a global isopycnal coordinate GCM. J. Phys. Oceanogr. (In the press.) Morreale, S. J., Standora, E. A., Spotila, J. R. & Paladino, F. V. 1996 Migration corridor for sea turtles. Nature 384, 319^320. Mortimer, J. A. & Carr, A. 1987 Reproduction and migrations of the Ascension Island green turtle (Chelonia mydas). Copeia 1987, 103^113. Owens, D. W., Comuzzie, D. C. & Grassman, M. A. 1986 Chemoreception in the homing and orientation behavior of amphibians and reptiles, with special reference to sea turtles. In Chemical signals in vertebrates (ed. D. Duvall, D. Muller- Schwarze & R. M. Silverstein), vol. 4, pp. 341^355. New York: Plenum Press. Papi, F. & Luschi, P. 1996 Pinpointing `Isla Meta': the case of sea turtles and albatrosses. J. Exp. Biol. 199, 65^71. Papi, F., Luschi, P., Crosio, E. & Hughes, G. R. 1997 Satellite tracking experiments on the navigational ability and migratory behaviour of the loggerhead turtle Caretta caretta. Mar. Biol. 129, 215^220. Reverdin, G. & McPhaden, M. J. 1986 Near-surface current and temperature variability observed in the equatorial Atlantic from drifting buoys. J. Geophys. Res. C5 91, 6569^6581. Scha«fer, H. & Krauss, W. 1995 Eddy statistics in the South Atlantic as derived from drifters drogued at 100 m. J. Mar. Res. 53, 403^431. Taillade, M. 1993 Trends in satellite-based animal tracking. In Biotelemetry XII (ed. P. Mancini, S. Fioretti, C. Cristalli & R. Bedini), pp. 291^297. Pisa, Italy: Litogra a Felici.