Carotenoid-based breast plumage colour, body condition and clutch size in red fodies (Foudia madagascariensis)

Similar documents
Blue structural coloration of male eastern bluebirds Sialia sialis predicts incubation provisioning to females

Cryptic sexual dichromatism occurs across multiple types of plumage in the Green-backed Tit Parus monticolus

An experimental test of female choice relative to male structural coloration in eastern bluebirds

Achromatic plumage reflectance predicts reproductive success in male black-capped chickadees

Structural and melanin coloration indicate parental effort and reproductive success in male eastern bluebirds

Carotenoid-based plumage coloration and aggression during molt in male house finches

Pierre-Paul Bitton and Russell D. Dawson

VARIATION IN PLUMAGE COLORATION OF EASTERN BLUEBIRDS (SIALIS SIALIS) IN RELATION TO WEATHER AND GEOGRAPHY. Mary-Catherine Warnock.

SEXUAL SELECTION ON PLUMAGE COLOR IN A NORTH CAROLINA POPULATION OF EASTERN BLUEBIRDS. Callie Lynn Younginer. Honors Thesis

Effects of nestling condition on UV plumage traits in blue tits: an experimental approach

Blue, not UV, plumage color is important in satin bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus violaceus display

doi: /

The Evolution of Signal Design in Manakin Plumage Ornaments

Supplementary Materials for

Within-Male Melanin-Based Plumage and Bill Elaboration in Male House Sparrows

EFFECTS OF COMMON ORIGIN AND COMMON ENVIRONMENT ON NESTLING PLUMAGE COLORATION IN THE GREAT TIT (PARUS MAJOR)

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

Effect of feather abrasion on structural coloration in male eastern bluebirds Sialia sialis

Evolution of sexual dichromatism: contribution of carotenoid- versus melanin-based coloration

Preening, plumage reflectance and female choice in budgerigars

MA41 Colour variability and the ecological use of colour in the chameleons and geckos of Mahamavo

How to reduce the costs of ornaments without reducing their effectiveness? An example of a mechanism from carotenoid-based plumage

FEATURE ARTICLES SEXUAL DICHROMATISM, DIMORPHISM, AND CONDITION- DEPENDENT COLORATION IN BLUE-TAILED BEE-EATERS

The evolutionary significance of plumage variability in

Sexual Dichromatism in the Blue-fronted Amazon Parrot (Amazona aestiva) Revealed by Multiple-angle Spectrometry

Sexual Dichromatism in the Northern Map Turtle, Graptemys geographica CONSTANCE M. O CONNOR 1,4, AND GABRIEL BLOUIN-DEMERS 3

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

Plumage and its Function in birds

Assortative mating by multiple ornaments in northern cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis)

PLUMAGE BRIGHTNESS AND BREEDING-SEASON DOMINANCE IN THE HOUSE FINCH: A NEGATIVELY CORRELATED HANDICAP? 1

NATURAL AND SEXUAL VARIATION

Red plumage and its association with reproductive success in red-capped robins

STATUS SIGNALING IN DARK-EYED JUNCOS

Plumage Color as a Composite Trait: Developmental and Functional Integration of Sexual Ornamentation

Cosmetic enhancement of signal coloration: experimental evidence in the house finch

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories

CARRY-OVER EFFECTS IN AMERICAN REDSTARTS: IMPLICATIONS FOR SEXUAL SELECTION AND BEHAVIOUR MATTHEW WILLIAM REUDINK

A Natural Experiment on the Condition-Dependence of Achromatic Plumage Reflectance in Black-Capped Chickadees

The effect of mycoplasmosis on carotenoid plumage coloration in male house finches

Diet quality affects an attractive white plumage pattern in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis)

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

Ultraviolet reflectance influences female preference for colourful males in the European serin

Bacteria as an Agent for Change in Structural Plumage Color: Correlational and Experimental Evidence

Colour in a new light: a spectral perspective on the quantitative genetics of carotenoid colouration

CAUSES AND CONSEQUENCES OF BLUE-GREEN EGGSHELL COLOUR VARIATION IN MOUNTAIN BLUEBIRDS (SIALIA CURRUCOIDES) Jeannine A. Randall

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Elk (Cervus canadensis) Extra credit assignment. Sad Underwing (Catocala maestosa) 10/11/2017

Carotenoid pigments in male American goldfinches: what is the optimal biochemical strategy for becoming colourful?

Plumage coloration can be perceived as a multiple condition-dependent signal by Great Tits Parus major

Variable visual habitats may influence the spread of colourful plumage across an avian hybrid zone

Spectral mouth colour of nestlings changes with

Differential access to social mates, to extrapair fertilizations,

Evolution of Avian Plumage Color in a Tetrahedral Color Space: A Phylogenetic Analysis of New World Buntings

TESTIS MASS AND SUBADULT PLUMAGE IN BLACK-HEADED GROSBEAKS

Proximate mechanisms of variation in the carotenoid-based plumage coloration of nestling great tits (Parus major L.)

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

CAROTENOID PIGMENTS IN MALE HOUSE FINCH PLUMAGE IN RELATION TO AGE, SUBSPECIES, AND ORNAMENTAL COLORATION

CAROTENOID-BASED DEWLAP COLOR AS A VISUAL SIGNAL IN SOCIAL. COMMUNICATION OF BROWN ANOLES (Norops sagrei) John Edward Steffen

Signaling Individual Identity versus Quality: A Model and Case Studies with Ruffs, Queleas, and House Finches

Hole-nesting birds. In natural conditions great and blue tits breed in holes that are made by e.g. woodpeckers

Consistency of Structural Color across Molts: The Effects of Environmental Conditions and Stress on Feather Ultraviolet Reflectance

ECOL /8/2019. Why do birds have colorful plumage? Today s Outline. Evolution of Animal Form & Function. 1. Functions of Colorful Plumage

ACQUISITION OF PLUMAGE POLYMORPHISM IN WHITE-THROATED SPARROWS

Does Egg Coloration Signal Female Quality to House Wren Males (Troglodytes aedon)? Research Thesis

저작권법에따른이용자의권리는위의내용에의하여영향을받지않습니다.

FEATHERS AT A FINE SCALE. Except where reference is made to the work of others, the work described in this

Brightness variability in the white badge of the eagle owl Bubo bubo

Melanin coloration in New World orioles II: ancestral state reconstruction reveals lability in the use of carotenoids and phaeomelanins

Dacnis cayana (Blue Dacnis or Turquoise Honeycreeper)

Different colors reveal different information: how nutritional stress affects the expression of melanin- and structurally based ornamental plumage

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

[ Post a Response Precious Fids Chat ] Novel Chemistry at Work To Provide Parrot's Vibrant Red Colors.

Revisiting the condition-dependence of melanin-based plumage

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

Plumage color as a dynamic trait: carotenoid pigmentation of male house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) fades during the breeding season

Color variation in museum specimens of birds: effects of stress, pigmentation, and duration of storage

Low Cross-Sex Genetic Correlation in Carotenoid-Based Plumage Traits in the Blue Tit Nestlings (Cyanistes caeruleus)

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Differences in Visual Signal Design and Detectability between Allopatric Populations of Anolis Lizards

Sexual imprinting on a novel blue ornament in zebra finches

Research Thesis. by Nathaniel J. Sackinger. The Ohio State University June 2013

Sexy smells Featured scientist: Danielle Whittaker from Michigan State University

Section 1: fill in the blanks (2 pts each) Note: Some questions have more than correct answer.

Red-winged blackbirds Agelaius phoeniceus use carotenoid and melanin pigments to color their epaulets

The effect of dietary carotenoid access on sexual dichromatism and plumage pigment composition in the American goldfinch

A record of a first year dark plumage Augur Buzzard moulting into normal plumage.

Afring News. An electronic journal published by SAFRING, Animal Demography Unit at the University of Cape Town

Winter male plumage coloration correlates with breeding status in a cooperative breeding species

THE NUMBER OF PROVISIONING VISITS BY HOUSE FINCHES PREDICTS THE MASS OF FOOD DELIVERED

Species introductions can reveal the operation of natural

Offspring sex ratio in red-winged blackbirds is dependent on

PERSISTENCE OF EGG RECOGNITION IN THE ABSENCE OF CUCKOO BROOD PARASITISM: PATTERN AND MECHANISM

SEXING COMMON SNIPE (Gallinago gallinago) IN THE FIELD IS THERE ANY SIMPLE METHOD?

Two pieces of information in a single trait? The yellow breast of the great tit (Parus major) reflects both pigment acquisition and body condition

Chapter 3. Bird colors as intrasexual signals of aggression and. dominance. Juan Carlos Senar Unidad Asociada CSIC, Museu Ciències Naturals Barcelona

Fiery red heads: female dominance among head color morphs in the Gouldian finch

Not all Red, Orange, and Yellow Colors are Carotenoid-based: The Need to Couple Biochemical and Behavioral Studies of Color Signals in Birds

Is the structural and psittacofulvin-based coloration of wild burrowing parrots Cyanoliseus patagonus condition dependent?

Transcription:

Ostrich 2006, 77(3&4): 164 169 Printed in South Africa All rights reserved Copyright NISC Pty Ltd OSTRICH EISSN 1727 947X Carotenoid-based breast plumage colour, body condition and clutch size in red fodies (Foudia madagascariensis) Laura K Estep 1 *, Matthew D Shawkey 2 and Geoffrey E Hill Department of Biological Sciences, 331 Funchess Hall, Auburn University, Alabama 36849, USA 1 Current address: Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, Yale School of Medicine, 60 College Street, PO Box 208034, New Haven, CT 06520 8034, Connecticut, USA 2 Current address: Department of Environmental Science, Policy and Management, 137 Mulford Hall #3114, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720-3114, USA * Corresponding author, e-mail: laura.estep@yale.edu Carotenoid-based feather colouration is a classic example of a condition-dependent ornamental trait. In some species of birds, red, orange and yellow feather colouration reflects male quality and advertises the carotenoid concentration of feathers. Such colouration is an important aspect of mate selection by females. The red fody (Foudia madagascariensis) is a highly sexually dimorphic species of the family Ploceidae, in which males exhibit extensive carotenoid-based plumage. We investigated associations between body condition, clutch size, first egg date and carotenoid-based red plumage colour of male red fodies. Breast plumage brightness was positively associated with male body condition, suggesting that this component of colour is condition-dependent. Brighter-red males did not breed earlier but were mated to females that laid larger clutches compared to females mated to duller-coloured males. These data suggest that red colour in red fodies has the potential to function as an honest signal of male quality. Introduction One explanation for the existence of ornamental traits in animals is that such traits evolved as reliable signals of individual quality (Zahavi 1975, Kodric-Brown and Brown 1984, Grafen 1990). Carotenoid-based colour displays in the skins of fish and the feathers of birds have played a central role in testing honest-signalling theory (Andersson 1994, Hill 2002). This theory relies upon the precept that ornaments used in honest signalling must be costly to produce or maintain (Andersson 1994). Carotenoid-based ornaments are likely to be honest signals of quality, because of the cost associated with the acquisition and utilisation of the pigments needed for maximum colour display (reviewed in Hill 2002, 2006a). Numerous studies on fish and birds have shown that carotenoid-based colouration can serve as an honest signal of quality (reviewed in Houde 1997, Hill 2002). In general, these studies have shown that males with access to more pigment resources, who are in better nutritional condition, or who have lower parasite loads during moult, grow feathers or scales with more saturated or longer-wavelength colouration. Furthermore, many studies have shown that females prefer to mate with males who have more saturated or redder ornaments (Houde 1997, Hill 2006b) and a few studies have shown that females mated to redder males experience material benefits in the form of greater paternal provisioning and higher reproductive success (Hill 1991, McGraw et al. 2001). Within birds, the vast majority of studies of carotenoid colouration have been conducted on Cardueline finches (family Fringillidae, sub-family Carduelinae) and tits (family Paridae). It is primarily from these studies that the above generalities are drawn (e.g. Hill and Montgomerie 1994, Thompson et al. 1997, Lindström and Lundström 2000, Hõrak et al. 2001, Tschirren et al. 2003). Studies of carotenoid-based plumage colour in weaver finches (family Ploceidae) show that carotenoid colouration can also serve functions other than mate attraction. The carotenoid-based plumage ornaments of both Red-collared (Euplectes ardens) and Red-shouldered Widowbirds (Euplectes axillaries) are important for signalling status and resource-holding potential. In Cardueline finches, the quality of colour display is condition-dependent and the object of sexual selection, but in weaver finches, it is generally the size of the ornament rather than colour quality that is condition-dependent and functions in sexual signalling (Pryke et al. 2001, Pryke and Andersson 2003a, 2003b). In the Red-billed Quelea (Quelea quelea), another weaver finch, carotenoid-based plumage ornamentation is not condition-dependent and does not function in sexual signalling (Dale 2000), but rather appears to be important in individual recognition (Dale et al. 2001). In a model, Dale et al. (2001) proposed a number of features that distinguish plumage colour that functions in individual recognition from plumage colour that functions in sexual signalling (Dale 2006). For example, a bimodal distribution of colour suggests that it functions in individual recognition, while a unimodal distribution suggests that it functions in sexual signalling. In contrast to the continuous distribution of colour in Cardueline finches, colour patches in queleas are distributed bimodally as discrete red and

Ostrich 2006, 77(3&4): 164 169 165 orange morphs, suggesting that they function in individual recognition. By identifying the features of plumage colour in a species and interpreting them in terms of this model, we may thereby gain some understanding of its function. We applied the concepts of Dale et al. s (2001) model to the plumage colour of red fodies (Foudia madagascariensis), a little-studied weaver finch. Red fodies are highly sexually dichromatic during breeding; females are olive and drab brown, while males vary continuously from bright red to drab yellow over their entire bodies, excluding the tail and wings (Langrand 1990). This sexual dichromatism suggests that bright plumage colours in this species may have evolved through sexual selection (reviewed in Badyaev and Hill 2003). Furthermore, the low variation in patch size (Langrand 1990 in contrast to that of widowbirds) and continuous unimodal distribution of colour (this study, in contrast to that of queleas) suggests that the colour could function as an honest signal of quality. The signal content of colour in this species is, to our knowledge, entirely unstudied. By examining associations between plumage colour and measures of male body condition and reproductive success, we tested the hypothesis that the red colouration in this species could serve as a signal of individual quality. Procedures Data collection We studied red fodies from January April 2004 on their breeding and foraging ground in Ranomafana National Park, along the eastern escarpment of Madagascar. We centred our study site on the rice paddy that is directly adjacent to the village of Vohiparara, which is used by flocks of red fodies for foraging (21 14 S, 47 23 E). We captured males in mist-nets set up along the perimeters of rice paddies from 07:00 to 11:00 and from 14:00 to 18:00. Upon capture, we banded males with unique combinations of one numbered aluminum band (Band and Tag Corporation) and three coloured plastic leg bands. We recorded standard morphological measurements (wing chord, tail length, tarsus length, bill length and mass) for all individuals. We pulled breast feathers from approximately the same area on each male and stored them in envelopes for future colour analyses. We did not measure breast patch size because the red colouration of all males used in this study extended from under the bill to the tip of the undertail coverts (Langrand 1990). Data concerning the dates of the onset of breeding for this species in the Ranomafana region were not available prior to our study, and full details of our data on the basic breeding biology of this species will be published separately elsewhere. Briefly, we intensively searched for nests in the area surrounding the central rice paddy complex and checked nests every three days. We began our surveys for nests in mid-december, but did not discover nests until mid-january, suggesting that this was the beginning of the breeding season. Mated pairs were never observed utilising the same nest for multiple clutches, suggesting that fodies change location with each new breeding attempt. Thus, we took the date at which a male was first associated with a nest to be the date of his first breeding attempt of the season. We recorded the number of eggs or nestlings at each visit. On some breeding territories, we captured the male of the pair with mist-nets set up in the breeding territory at least m from the nest location. Males were highly territorial, chasing other males and singing from regularly-used perches (LK Estep, pers. obs.); thus, we were certain that the captured male was the social mate. Males captured on the territories were processed following the same procedure used for males captured on the rice paddy. Because of high depredation rates and flooding of nests containing nestlings (57.1%), we used final clutch size as our measure of reproductive success (Badyaev et al. 2000, Doucet et al. 2004). Colour analyses We stacked and taped five feathers from each male to gloss-free black construction paper and recorded spectral data from them using an Ocean Optics S2000 Spectrometer (range of 250 880nm, Dunedin, FL, USA) and a UV (deuterium bulb) and visible (tungsten-halogen bulb) light source. We used a bifurcated fibre-optic measuring probe (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA), which provided illumination from the lamp and transferred light reflected from the feather sample back to the spectrometer. This probe was held at a 90 angle to the feather surface. Ambient light was excluded with a block sheath that held the tip at a fixed distance of 5mm from the feather surface, providing a reading area of 2mm. All reflectance data were generated relative to a white standard (WS-1, Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA). The probe was lifted and replaced, and a new measurement taken, five times on each feather sample. Each of these five measurements was an average of 20 sequential spectra generated by 00Ibase software, a spectra acquisition software package (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL). We then averaged these five measurements for each feather sample. We calculated standard indices for three different elements of colour from these reflectance spectra (Hailman 1977, Norris et al. 2004). These indices were restricted to wavelengths between 300nm and 700nm, as evidence suggests that this is the range of sensitivity for the avian visual system (Jacobs 1981, Cuthill et al. 2000). Red chroma, or spectral purity in the red region, is the proportion of light reflected in the range from 575 700nm, and UV chroma is the proportion of light reflected in the range from 300 400nm (Norris et al. 2004). Brightness is the summed reflectance from 300 700nm (Endler 1990). Repeatability of measurements from the same individual male was 0.67 for brightness (F = 11.33, df = 81,328, p <.0001), 0.73 for UV chroma (F = 14.95, df = 81,328, p <.0001) and 0.75 for red chroma (F = 15.95, df = 81,328, p <.0001) (Lessells and Boag 1987). We chose these variables based on the shape of the reflectance curve (see Figure 1), which exhibited a peak in the UV and a long plateau at longer wavelengths. Furthermore, red chroma has been found to accurately indicate the amount of carotenoids present in feathers (Inouye et al. 2001, Saks et al. 2003a). To determine whether colour variables were unimodally or bimodally distributed, we visually examined histograms of UV chroma, red chroma and brightness for modality patterns.

166 Estep, Shawkey and Hill PERCENT REFLECTANCE (%) 50 40 30 20 300 400 500 600 700 WAVELENGTH (nm) FREQUENCY 20 15 5 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 1920 21 BRIGHTNESS (%) Figure 1: Representative spectrometric curve of red breast feathers of a male Madagascar red fody Verification of the presence of carotenoids We used an acidified pyridine extraction method (McGraw et al. 2005) to verify the presence of carotenoids in feathers. The acidified pyridine was coloured bright red following incubation, and the feathers from which pigments were extracted were white. These results suggest that carotenoids are primarily responsible for the red colour of this plumage (McGraw et al. 2005). Statistical analyses We used linear regression to compare first egg date and male body condition to the three measured colour variables. We used the residuals of mass on tarsus length as our measure of body condition, as is common for studies of live birds (for review, see Blem 1990, Brown 1996). Although this approach to measuring body condition has attracted some criticism (Green 2001), recent research suggests that such criticism may be unwarranted (Schulte-Hostedde et al. 2005). Both response variables of first egg data and male body condition were normally distributed (n = 12, D = 0.224, p = 0.094 and n = 13, D = 0.068, p > 0.150, respectively, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test). We used backwards elimination to remove variables, until arriving at a final model (alpha-todrop = 0.05). Because the response variable of final clutch size was not normally distributed (n = 13, D = 0.394, p <.0, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test), we used non-parametric correlations with a Bonferroni correction to compare the number of eggs to the three colour variables. Results We captured and processed 82 males in and around the rice paddy. The reflectance spectra of the red feathers was similar to that of the feathers of other bird species with carotenoid-based red colouration (Eaton and Lanyon 2003), with a small peak in the UV wavelengths and a long plateau in the red wavelengths (Figure 1). Histograms indicated that all three colour variables were unimodally distributed (Figure 2). Bins for the histograms were evenly distributed across the range of values for each variable (Figure 2). To test the hypothesis that plumage colouration is related to body condition, we compared saturation and brightness FREQUENCY FREQUENCY 40 35 30 25 20 15 5 16 14 12 8 6 4 2 <70 70 75 80 85 90 95 RED CHROMA (%) 0 2 4 6 8 12 14 16 UV CHROMA (%) Figure 2: Histograms for the colour variables UV chroma, red chroma and overall brightness for the red breast feathers of Madagascar red fodies (n = 82) to body condition. The final linear regression model arrived at for male body condition included only feather brightness (Figure 3, linear regression, n = 82, r 2 =.084, β = 0.290, F = 7.371, p = 0.008). UV chroma dropped out of the model at the first step in the selection process (β = 6.19, t = 1.12, p = 0.266), and red chroma dropped out in the second step (β = 2.57, t = 1.29, p = 0.201). As an indirect test of female mate preference and male plumage colouration, we compared nest initiation date to male colouration. We monitored the nests of 13 males for which we had plumage colour data. Data for exact nest initiation date were unavailable for one nest. Neither red nor UV chroma nor brightness of breast feather colouration predicted nest initiation date. Feather brightness was dropped from the model first (β = 3.53, t = 0.54, p = 0.602), followed by UV

Ostrich 2006, 77(3&4): 164 169 167 RESIDUALS OF MASS/TARSUS 2.0 1.0 0.0 1.0 2.0 Linear regression n = 82, r 2 = 0.84 ß = 0.290 F = 7.371, p = 0.008 12.5 15 17.5 20 22.5 25 MALE BREAST BRIGHTNESS (%) Figure 3: Scatterplot of male body condition (residuals of mass/tarsus) and male breast brightness for male red fodies captured on the rice paddy and on breeding territories FINAL CLUTCH SIZE 5 4 3 2 Spearman Rank Correlation N = 13 r s = 0.797 p = 0.001 13.75 15.00 16.25 17.50 MALE BREAST BRIGHTNESS (%) Figure 4: Scatterplot of number of eggs for the first nest of the breeding season and the brightness of male breast colouration chroma (β = 1 739.48, t = 1.61, p = 0.141), then red chroma (β = 35.72, t = 0.39, p = 0.7013). However, there was a significant positive relationship between male colouration and final clutch size (Figure 4, Spearman rank correlation, N = 13, r s = 0.797, p = 0.001) and this pattern remained significant after Bonferroni correction (adjusted alpha = 0.017). Clutch size was not correlated with UV chroma or red chroma (Spearman Rank Correlation, N = 13, r s = 0.491, p = 0.089; N = 13, r s = 0.343, p = 0.251, respectively). Discussion Brightness of breast plumage colour was positively associated with male body condition and clutch size, but not first egg date for male fodies. Only one other study (Saks et al. 2003b) has shown a significant correlation between overall brightness of carotenoid-based ornamental colouration and male quality. These first two patterns are thus not consistent with most studies of plumage colouration in Cardueline finches and members of the family Paridae, in which hue and saturation tend to be correlated with male quality (reviewed in Hill 2002). Hue and saturation reflect the composition and concentration of carotenoid pigments, respectively (Inouye et al. 2001, Saks et al. 2003a), while brightness has no clear relationship to the carotenoid content of feathers (Saks et al. 2003b). Of the three colour variables, brightness is most likely to be affected by the underlying white structural colour as well as carotenoid content (Shawkey and Hill 2005). In both experimental and field correlational studies, carotenoid pigmentation of feathers has been clearly shown to be condition-dependent (reviewed in Hill 2002, 2006a). The hypothesis that structural colouration is condition-dependent is not as strongly supported (Prum 2006), but some studies have shown a relationship between structural colour variables and condition (Doucet 2002, Doucet and Montgomerie 2003, Hill et al. 2005). Thus, both deposition of carotenoids and formation of the underlying white structural tissue may be condition-dependent in fodies. Experimental tests of the effects of condition on these two processes will be needed to test this hypothesis. However, a recent experimental study demonstrated that carotenoid deposition, but not white structural colour, was dependent on condition during molt in American Goldfinches Carduelis tristis (Shawkey et al., in press). Our examination of male plumage colouration in relation to fitness measures produced mixed results. Brighter males did not breed earlier than duller males, but females mated to males with brighter plumage colouration laid more eggs. Breeding early may not confer the same advantages to tropical species such as fodies as it does to temperate species (Stutchbury and Morton 2001), so nest initiation date may not be a relevant gauge of male success in tropical environments. The larger clutch size in the nests of brighter males suggests that females may produce larger clutches when mated to brighter males or that brighter males may be mated to higher-quality females, suggesting that colour may function as a sexual signal in fodies in the same manner as in Cardueline finches (Hill 2002). Alternatively, brighter males may acquire higher-quality territories, and it is the resources held by a male rather than his colouration per se that determines the number of eggs a female lays. It should be noted that this specific result applies to the entire male fody population we studied, contingent upon the assumption that events such as flooding and depredation that resulted in clutch loss were stochastic, with respect to male quality. Otherwise, our results apply specifically to the subset of the males who were less likely to lose clutches during such events. The nature of fody plumage colour suggests that it serves as an indicator of individual quality. Dale et al. (2001) propose that such indicators should be relatively less variable, condition-dependent, unimodally distributed, positively correlated with fitness measures, and have positively correlated component characters. While we cannot address the final condition of the model, the remainder of the conditions fit the case of the fody well. Carotenoid colouration in fodies is largely restricted to a UV-red colour, appears to be condition-dependent, is unimodally distrib-

168 Estep, Shawkey and Hill uted and is positively correlated with some, but not all, fitness measures. By contrast, the colour of the quelea, which has been argued to function in individual recognition, is highly variable and bimodally distributed into two discrete colour morphs, and is not correlated with fitness measures. Thus, by the terms of this model, the red colour of the fody likely functions as an indicator of individual quality. Why the colour of the widowbirds does not clearly fit either set of conditions is not clear, although it appears that the size, rather than the colour, of the ornament may function as an indicator of quality (Pryke and Andersson 2003a, 2003b). Conclusions We showed that red colour in fodies reflects body condition and a measure of reproductive success and thus could potentially be used as a sexual signal. Whether red colour functions in female choice or male-male competition could not be determined in this study; however, the fit of this species plumage characteristics to the model in Dale et al. (2001) suggests that carotenoid colouration in fodies could function as an indicator of individual quality. Future studies should explicitly examine whether females assess this colour when making mate-choice decisions, or if males use it in competition with one another. Acknowledgements We are grateful to Solohery Rasamison for field assistance and to MICET/ICTE Madagascar and Aristide Andrianarimisa at the Peregrine Fund Madagascar for assistance in Madagascar. We thank ANGAP, the Ranomafana National Park Service, the Centre Valbio, and local officials in the village of Vohiparara for helping us with the logistics of establishing our field site. Herman L Mays Jr and Lynn Siefferman provided helpful comments and insight during revision of this manuscript. This study was done under the Auburn University IACUC (Permit # PRN 2003-0615) and was financially supported by an Alabama Space Grant Consortium Fellowship to LKE and National Science Foundation grants (IBN 0235778 and DEB 0077804) to GEH. References Andersson M 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, USA Badyaev AEV and Hill GE 2003. Avian sexual dichromatism in relation to phylogeny and ecology. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics 34: 27 49 Badyaev AEV, Hill GE, Stoehr AM, Nolan PM and McGraw KJ 2000. The evolution of sexual dimorphism in the house finch: II. Population divergence in relation to local selection. Evolution 54: 2134 2144 Blem CR 1990. Avian energy storage. In: Power DM (ed) Current Ornithology. pp 59 113. Plenum Press, New York Brown ME 1996. Assessing body condition in birds. In: Nolan V and Ketterson ED (eds) Current Ornithology. pp 67 135. Plenum Press, New York Cuthill IC, Bennett ATD, Church SC, Hart NS and Hunt S 2000. Ultraviolet vision in birds. In: Slater PJB, Rosenblatt JS, Snowdon CT and Roper TJ (eds) Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol. 29. pp 159 214. Academic Press, San Diego Dale J 2000. Ornamental plumage does not signal male quality in Red-billed Queleas. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 267: 2143 2149 Dale J 2006. Intraspecific variation in bird colors. In: Hill GE and McGraw KJ (eds) Bird Coloration, Vol. 2: Function and Evolution. pp 36 86. Harvard University Press, Cambridge Dale J, Lank DB and Reeve HK 2001. Signaling individual identity versus quality: a model and case studies with ruffs, queleas, and house finches. American Naturalist 158: 75 86 Doucet SM 2002. Structural plumage coloration, male body size, and condition in the Blue-black Grassquit. Condor 4: 30 38 Doucet SM and Montgomerie R 2003. Multiple sexual ornaments in Satin Bowerbirds: ultraviolet plumage and bowers signal different aspects of male quality. Behavioral Ecology 14: 503 509 Doucet SM, Mennill DJ, Montgomerie R, Boag PT and Ratcliffe LM 2004. Achromatic plumage reflectance predicts reproductive success in male Black-capped Chickadees. Behavioral Ecology 16: 218 222 Eaton MD and Lanyon SM 2003. The ubiquity of avian ultraviolet plumage reflectance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 270: 1721 1726 Endler JA 1990. On the measurement and classification of color in studies of animal color patterns. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 41: 315 352 Grafen A 1990. Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology 144: 517 546 Green AJ 2001. Mass/length residuals: measures of body condition or generators of spurious results? Ecology 82: 1473 1483 Hailman JP 1977. Optical Signals: Animal Communication and Light. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, USA Hill GE 1991. Plumage coloration is a sexually-selected indicator of male quality. Nature 350: 337 339 Hill GE 2002. A Red Bird in a Brown Bag: the Function and Evolution of Ornamental Plumage Coloration in the House Finch. Oxford University Press, New York Hill GE 2006a. Environmental regulation of ornamental coloration. In: Hill GE and McGraw KJ (eds) Bird Coloration, Vol. 1: Mechanisms and Measurements. pp 507 560. Harvard University Press, Cambridge Hill GE 2006b. Female mate choice for ornamental coloration in birds. In: Hill GE and McGraw KJ (eds) Bird Coloration, Vol. 2: Function and Evolution. pp 137 200. Harvard University Press, Cambridge Hill GE and Montgomerie R 1994. Plumage colour signals nutritional condition in the House Finch. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 258: 47 52 Hill GE, Doucet SM and Buchholz R 2005. The effect of coccidial infection on iridescent plumage coloration in wild turkeys. Animal Behaviour 69: 387 394 Hõrak P, Ots I, Vellau H, Spottiswoode C and Møller AP 2001. Carotenoid-based plumage coloration reflects hemoparasite infection and local survival in breeding Great Tits. Oecologia 126: 166 173 Houde AE 1997. Sex, Color, and Mate Choice in Guppies. Princeton University Press, Princeton Inouye CY, Hill GE, Stradi RD and Montgomerie R 2001. Carotenoid pigments in male House Finch plumage in relation to age, subspecies, and ornamental coloration. Auk 118: 900 915 Jacobs GH 1981. Comparative Color Vision. Academic Press, New York Kodric-Brown A and Brown JH 1984. Truth in advertising: the kinds of traits favored by sexual selection. American Naturalist 124: 309 323 Langrand O 1990. Guide to the Birds of Madagascar. Yale University Press, New Haven Lessells CM and Boag PT 1987. Unrepeatable repeatabilities: a common mistake. Auk 4: 116 121 Lindström K and Lundström J 2000. Male green finches

Ostrich 2006, 77(3&4): 164 169 169 (Carduelis chloris) with brighter ornaments have higher virus infection clearance rate. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 48: 44 51 McGraw KJ, Hudon J, Hill GE and Parker RS 2005. A simple and inexpensive chemical test for behavioral ecologists to determine the presence of carotenoid pigments in animal tissues. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57: 391 397 McGraw KJ, Stoehr AM, Nolan PM and Hill GE 2001. Plumage redness predicts breeding onset and reproductive success in the House Finch: a validation of Darwin s theory. Journal of Avian Biology 32: 90 94 Norris DR, Marra PP, Montgomerie R, Kyser TK and Ratcliffe L 2004. Reproductive effort, molting latitude, and feather color in a migratory songbird. Science 306: 2249 2250 Prum RO 2006. Anatomy, physics, and evolution of avian structural color. In: Hill GE and McGraw KJ (eds) Bird Coloration, Vol. 1: Mechanisms and Measurements. Harvard University Press, Cambridge Pryke SR and Andersson S 2003a. Carotenoid-based epaulettes reveal male competitive ability: experiments with resident and floater Red-shouldered Widowbirds. Animal Behaviour 66: 217 224 Pryke SR and Andersson S 2003b. Carotenoid-based status signalling in Red-shouldered Widowbirds (Euplectes axillaris): epaulet size and redness affect captive and territorial competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 53: 393 401 Pryke SR, Lawes MJ and Andersson S 2001. Agonistic carotenoid signalling in male Red-collared Widowbirds: aggression related to the colour signal of both the territory owner and model intruder. Animal Behaviour 62: 695 704 Saks L, McGraw K and Horak P 2003a. How feather colour reflects its carotenoid content. Functional Ecology 17: 555 561 Saks L, Ots I and Horak P 2003b. Carotenoid-based plumage coloration of male green finches reflects health and immunocompetence. Oecologia 134: 301 307 Schulte-Hostedde AI, Zinner B, Millar JS and Hickling GJ 2005. Validating body condition indices. Ecology 86: 155 163 Shawkey MD and Hill GE 2005. Carotenoids need structural colours to shine. Biology Letters 1: 121 125 Shawkey MD, Hill GE, McGraw KJ, Hood WR and Huggins KL in press. An experimental test of the relative contribution and condition-dependence of microstructure and carotenoids in yellow plumage coloration. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B Stutchbury BJ and Morton ES 2001. Behavioral Ecology of Tropical Birds. Academic Press, San Diego Thompson CW, Hillgarth N, Leu M and McClure HE 1997. High parasite load in House Finches (Carpodacus americanus) is correlated with reduced expression of a sexually-selected trait. American Naturalist 149: 270 294 Tschirren B, Fitze PS and Richner H 2003. Proximate mechanisms of variation in the carotenoid-based plumage coloration of nestling Great Tits (Parus major L.). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16: 91 0 Zahavi A 1975. Mate selection a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53: 205 214 Received January 2006, accepted June 2006 Editor: WRJ Dean