for Assistance Elise R. Shore a, Charles Burdsal a & Deanna K. Douglas b a Psychology Department, Wichita State University

Similar documents
The Value of Data Gary Patronek & Stephen Zawistowski Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Hsin-Yi Weng a & Lynette A. Hart b a Department of Pathobiology, College of Veterinary

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Distressed Animal Behaviors and Some Recommendations for Improvements at the Kuala Lumpur Zoo, Malaysia Amber Haque Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Long-Term Outcome After Treatment of Feline Inappropriate Elimination Amy R. Marder & Joan M. Engel Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Departments, Iowa State University, Ames b Department of Population Medicine, Ontario Veterinary College, University of Guelph,

Amelia J. Cook a & Emily McCobb a a Center for Animals and Public Policy, Cummings

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Nicole Cottam a, Nicholas H. Dodman a, Alice A. Moon-Fanelli a & Gary J. Patronek b a Department of Clinical Science, Tufts Cummings

Published online: 05 Dec 2007.

The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Dog Surrender Profile

Birth and Death Rate Estimates of Cats and Dogs in U.S. Households and Related Factors

Behaviour of dogs adopted from an animal shelter. Svatava Vitulová, Eva Voslářová, Vladimír Večerek, Iveta Bedáňová

Dog Breeding in New Providence, The Bahamas, and Its Potential Impact on the Roaming Dog Population II: The Fate of Puppies

Carin Wittnich a b & Michael Belanger b a Canadian Veterinary Medical Association, Ottawa,

Reducing Surrenders. Dayna Kennedy Shelter Manager Upper Peninsula Animal Welfare Shelter

Creating an Effective Shelter Intake Form to Reduce Owner Surrender

30/04/2014. Why people keep pets and why we need to change how we breed them. Who I am. What are pets? What I plan to talk about

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Dog Profile. Dog s Information: About your Dog s History: Date: / / Animal ID (Staff Use Only): Dog s Name: Breed: Sex: (Check Box) Male Female

Outcome of Cats Adopted From a Biomedical Research Program Brian A. DiGangi, P. Cynda Crawford & Julie K. Levy Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

To cite this article: Emily Weiss & Sandra Wilson (2003) The Use of Classical and

Rocky s Retreat Boarding/Daycare Intake Form

Cat Profile. Animal ID (Staff Use Only) Cat s Name: Breed: Spayed or Neutered: (Check Box) Yes No Unknown Age: Date of Birth (If Known):

Conflict-Related Aggression

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

INCOMING DOG HISTORY SHEET

INCOMING CAT PROFILE

The Journal of Veterinary Medical Science

Daycare Application Form

Owner Surrender & Relinquishment Dog

Shawn Jen-Lung Peng a, Lisa Yu-Ting Lee b & Andrew Chang-Young Fei a a Institute of Animal Welfare, School of Veterinary

MEMBERSHIP APPLICATION

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

CANINE SURRENDER PROFILE

Canine Questionnaire

An Argument against Breed Specific Legislation

Adoption Application/Contract

TITLE: The effects of video footage versus photographs on perception of dog behavioral

Cat Surrender Information & Profile

PAW PRINTS PET RESORT GUEST APPLICATION FORM

Connecticut Humane Society Canine Pet Personality Profile

Tug Dogs Canine History Form

CONTRACT & HEALTH WARRANTY PET DOG

RE-HOMING PACKET. Dear Pet Guardian: Enclosed is a packet of information to help you re-home your pet(s). You should find the following:

Name: Spouse/Partner s Name: Address: Home Phone: City/State/Zip: Work Phone: Address: Cell Phone: TX DL # : Employer:

Chapter 13 First Year Student Recruitment Survey

Dog Breeding in New Providence, The Bahamas, and Its Potential Impact on the Roaming Dog Population I: Planned and Accidental

Owner Relinquish Profile - Cats

Canine Intake Profile. Owner s name: Owner s Phone#: Owner s Address Number: Street Name: Apt/Unit Postal Code: City:

Surrendered Misc. Pet Information

The Myth of Pet Overpopulation. Is there really a pet overpopulation crisis?

BEHAVIOR QUESTIONNAIRE FOR DOGS

An Evaluation of Respondent Conditioning Procedures to Decrease Barking in an Animal Shelter

ADOPTION POLICIES AND FEES PLEASE READ CAREFULLY BEFORE COMPLETING ADOPTION APPLICATION

Other people in your household Name Relationship to you Age

PUPPY TRAINING REGISTRATION FORM 6-Week Session Thursdays, July 13 - August 24, 2017

Sex, age and size as factors affecting the length of stay of dogs in Czech shelters. Jiří Žák, Eva Voslářová, Vladimír Večerek, Iveta Bedáňová

BEHAVIOR QUESTIONNAIRE FOR DOGS

Information Guide. Find a rescue dog.

CANINE BEHAVIOR HISTORY FORM. Household Information. Pet Info. List all other family members (names): Adults: Children: age age

Age: All dogs must be at least 16 weeks or older. Puppies and shelter dogs must have been at home for 2 weeks prior to coming to daycare.

S.A.F.E.R. 1 TRAINING MANUAL. (Showing Animals: Friendly, Effectively, and Responsibly)

Owner/Guardian SURRENDER Contract

TRAINING & BEHAVIOR QUESTIONNAIRE

Sterilization of Companion Animals: Exploring the Attitudes and Behaviors of Latino Students in South Texas

Incoming Dog Profile

Housetraining Your Adopted Dog

Owner Surrender Intake Interview Form

1740 W. Gordon St., Valdosta, GA ADOPTION CONTRACT PET INFORMATION

Evaluation of association between retention in the home and attendance at puppy socialization classes

New Client Questionnaire For multiple dog owners please complete one questionnaire for each dog.

OWNER SURRENDER CAT QUESTIONNAIRE

2013 AVMA Veterinary Workforce Summit. Workforce Research Plan Details

Adoption Questionnaire

TOMPKINS COUNTY SOCIETY FOR THE PREVENTION OF CRUELTY TO ANIMALS

1) First Name: Last Name: 2) First Name: Last Name: Street Address: City: Postal Code: address: Home Phone: Mobile phone:

Effects of Differing Traits in Dogs on Perceived Adoptability. Dogs are a mainstream part of American life. While many pet dogs are mutts, 35% of

A survey of the management of inter-dog aggression by animal shelters in Canada

SEPARATION ANXIETY. Why Do Dogs Suffer From Separation Anxiety?

Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by owners of dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter

Dog Evaluation Forms

Power Paws Assistance Dogs

John Reilly. Canisius College. March 17, 2018

Incorporating Behavioral Medicine Into General Practice

Biting, Nipping & Jumping Up

LABRADOR RETRIEVER CLUB of Qld Inc. RESCUE & RE-HOME SERVICE

Adaptil is clinically proven to help dogs deal with stressful situations.

Mile High Weimaraner Rescue Surrender Packet

Adopting a Dog. The New Arrival

Incoming Dog Profile Revised 3/23/2016

Desensitization and Counter Conditioning

Insider's Guide To The Cavalier King Charles Spaniel - The Dog Barking Helper HOW TO MANAGE DOGGY PROBLEMS. Dog Barking Help

ADOPTION APPLICATION

DuPage County Animal Care & Control Cat Behavior & Health Profile

ADOPTION APPLICATION. Please fill out this form completely. Completion of this application does not guarantee adoption.

Transcription:

This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 07:28 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/haaw20 Pet Owners' Views of Pet Behavior Problems and Willingness to Consult Experts for Assistance Elise R. Shore a, Charles Burdsal a & Deanna K. Douglas b a Psychology Department, Wichita State University b Psychology Department, Bethany College Published online: 25 Jan 2008. To cite this article: Elise R. Shore, Charles Burdsal & Deanna K. Douglas (2008) Pet Owners' Views of Pet Behavior Problems and Willingness to Consult Experts for Assistance, Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 11:1, 63-73, DOI: 10.1080/10888700701729221 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888700701729221 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages,

and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL WELFARE SCIENCE, 11:63 73, 2008 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1088-8705 print/1532-7604 online DOI: 10.1080/10888700701729221 Pet Owners Views of Pet Behavior Problems and Willingness to Consult Experts for Assistance Elsie R. Shore and Charles Burdsal Psychology Department Wichita State University Deanna K. Douglas Psychology Department Bethany College Prior research has found that dog obedience training and the receipt of advice regarding companion animal (pet) behavior reduce the risk of nonhuman animal relinquishment to an animal shelter and increase human-companion animal interactions, but research also finds that only a minority of pet guardians participates in such activities. The researchers for this study asked 170 dog and cat guardians to assess the seriousness of common problem behaviors and how likely they were to seek help. The researchers also asked them to rate different methods of obtaining expert advice. The dog and cat guardians saw behaviors directly affecting humans as most severe; those involving the destruction of property were next in severity; and those affecting the animal more than the human were least severe. There were no significant differences in ratings by participants gender or income. Participants indicated they were more likely to use free help options than fee-based services. The lack of a clear correlation between the seriousness of a behavior problem and likelihood of seeking help suggests that other factors may play a role in pet guardians interest in obtaining expert advice. Research spanning several decades demonstrates that guardian (owner)-perceived behavior problems place cats and dogs at increased risk of relinquishment to a shelter for nonhuman animals. Arkow and Dow (1984) found dog Correspondence should be sent to Elsie R. Shore, Wichita State University, Department of Psychology, 1845 Fairmount, Wichita, KS 67260. Email: elsie.shore@wichita.edu

64 SHORE, BURDSAL, DOUGLAS behavioral problems to be a predominant reason for relinquishment at 13 U.S. shelters, accounting for more than 26% of the dogs surrendered. Behavior was cited in 30% of dog relinquishments and 14% of cat relinquishments among 130 owners surveyed at an Ohio humane society (Miller, Staats, Partlo, & Rada, 1996). Patronek, Glickman, Beck, McCabe, and Ecker (1996a, 1996b) compared households that had relinquished dogs or cats with a control group of dog-or cat-owning households. Behavior problems were found to increase the risk of relinquishment for both species, the risk rising with increasing frequency of the behaviors. The National Council on Pet Population Study and Policy s Regional Shelter Study collected information from 12 shelters in four regions of the United States (New et al., 2000; Salman, et al., 1998, 2000). The more than 3000 relinquishers interviewed gave 71 reasons for relinquishing; 24 of these were classified as behavioral, with at least 1 behavioral problem cited for 40% of dogs and 28% of cats. A study of dogs in three Melbourne, Australia, shelters found that 14% of the dogs had been relinquished for dog behavior or aggression (Marston, Bennett, & Coleman, 2004). Pet behavior is also a factor in the return of recently adopted animals. Shore (2005) found that pet behavior accounted for more than one third of the reasons given for returning dogs or cats to the shelter from which they had been adopted. Obedience training and behavioral counseling may be one way to reduce the relinquishment of companion animals displaying behaviors their guardians find objectionable. Patronek et al. (1996a) found that risk of dog relinquishment to an animal shelter was reduced among owners who had received behavior advice they considered very helpful and among owners who had participated in training activities after acquiring the dog. Clark and Boyer (1993) and Jagoe and Serpell (1996) reported that obedience training was associated with reduced prevalence of selected behavior problems reported by dog owners. The potentially beneficial effects of obedience classes, expert advice, and behavioral counseling can, of course, be attained only if pet owners choose to avail themselves of such options. Coren (1999) reported that only 24% of people participate in formal obedience classes. Relinquishers in a study by DiGiacomo, Arluke, and Patronek (1998) reported tolerating the animal s behavior or looking for a new home for the pet but often stated that they had neither the time nor the money to invest in training. Bennett and Rohlf (2007), who found that training engagement was negatively correlated with a number of behavior problems and positively correlated with the owners involvement in shared activities with their pets, also noted that slightly more than 40% of the committed owners in their study had never attended formal obedience training. The authors point out that although it seems logical to assume that those with problem dogs would engage in training activities, it may be that owners with well-behaved dogs might be more likely to do so because the activity would be more pleasant for them. Such findings point to the need to understand pet owners views of their pets behavior and ways to address behavior

PET BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS 65 problems. The purpose of the current study was to investigate cat and dog owners assessments of the seriousness of common pet behavior problems and the likelihood that they would ask for help with these problems. Owners were also asked their opinion of a variety of options for obtaining expert advice or assistance with the behavior. METHOD Eighty-five cat owners and 85 dog owners were recruited from among visitors at an open-admissions Humane Society shelter in a moderately sized midwestern U.S. city. To quickly distinguish themselves from shelter staff, the interviewers wore names tags indicating they were from a local university. The interviewers approached people looking at animals available for adoption, introduced themselves, and explained that they were conducting a research study. The visitors were asked whether they already had a pet dog or cat at home. The interviewers briefly described the study to those who had pets, stated that the interview took between 5 and 10 minutes, and asked if they would like to participate. Those who agreed were shown to an office supplied by the shelter for the interview. Participants with both dogs and cats were asked to choose to respond to questions about dogs or about cats. Dog owners were asked to rate 13 problem behaviors; cat owners rated 9. Each list (Tables 1 and 2) is comprised of frequently given behavioral reasons for relinquishment, as reported in a variety of studies (Miller et al., 1996; New et al., 2000; Patronek et al., 1996a, 1996b; Salman et al., 2000). The participant was asked to rate each behavior in terms of the severity of the problem ( Would you consider this a problem? ), and the likelihood that the person would ask for advice ( How likely would you be to ask for advice or help from an expert? ). Participants were handed a sheet that presented each question, followed by a scale of numbers ranging from 1 to 10. Both ends were anchored with a short phrase, such as Not a problem at all and A very serious problem. The problem behaviors were presented verbally, in random order. Randomization was attained through the use of a computerized random number generator (Research Randomizer; http://www. randomizer.org). In the second part of the survey, the interviewers presented 12 services that could be used to obtain assistance with a behavior problem. The 12 (Table 3) included free and for-pay services. Participants were asked, How likely you would be to use the following services? As with the behavior problems, participants were handed a sheet that presented the rating visually, again on a scale of 1 (Never would) to10(definitely would). In order to make the free versus for-pay distinction clear, these services were not presented in random order.

66 SHORE, BURDSAL, DOUGLAS TABLE 1 Dog Owners Mean Ratings of Behaviors: Seriousness and Likely to Ask for Advice From an Expert Seriousness a Ask for Advice b Item M SD M SD The dog is aggressive toward people, growling and/or 9.23 1.7 7.62 3.2 biting. The dog doesn t get along with your children, or the 8.65 2.6 7.54 3.3 children are afraid of the dog. The dog escapes from the house or yard. 8.38 2.1 5.62 3.5 The dog is aggressive toward other dogs or cats. 7.96 2.3 6.75 3.3 The dog does not get along with your other pets. 7.89 2.6 6.42 3.6 The dog chews furniture, shoes, or other things in the 7.79 2.5 6.65 3.2 house. When no one is home, the dog gets upset and 7.28 3.0 6.73 3.4 destructive. The dog destroys things in the yard or garage. 7.12 2.6 6.13 3.3 The dog is not completely housebroken and 6.81 3.0 5.89 3.5 sometimes urinates or defecates (pees or poops) in the house. The dog is disobedient, doesn t listen. 6.78 2.7 6.42 3.3 The dog barks a lot. 6.51 2.6 6.56 3.1 The dog is hyper, running and jumping too much. 5.09 2.8 5.56 3.4 The dog is afraid of people, storms, or loud noises. 4.12 2.7 5.87 3.5 a Seriousness: 1 (not a problem at all)to10(a very serious problem). b Likely to ask for advice or help from an expert: 1 (not at all likely) to 10 (extremely likely). Finally, participants were handed a sheet presenting four demographic questions (age group, current living arrangement, race/ethnicity, annual household income group) and listing categories within each. For each question, they were asked to provide the number of the category that applied to them. RESULTS Most of the 170 participants were female (128; 75.3%); almost all (154; 90.6%) were White; and most all were in their 20s (30.2%) or 30s (27.8%). Participants were given a list of seven household- income groupings, from less than $20,000 per year to greater than $75,000 per year, and asked to indicate which group applied to them. Incomes were relatively evenly spread, and no group dominated. The largest number reported annual incomes greater than $75,000 (41, 24.6%) or between $30,000 and $40,000 (39; 23.3%).

PET BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS 67 TABLE 2 Cat Owners Mean Ratings of Behaviors: Seriousness and Likely to Ask for Advice From an Expert Seriousness a Ask for Advice b Item M SD M SD The cat sprays in the house. 8.79 2.1 8.01 3.2 The cat is aggressive toward people. 8.24 2.3 7.59 3.1 The cat doesn t always use its litter box and 8.07 2.3 7.82 2.9 sometimes urinates of defecates (pees or poops) in the house. The cat damages or destroys things by scratching, 7.28 2.6 7.24 3.2 biting, or chewing. The cat does not get along with your children, or they 7.22 3.2 7.51 2.9 are afraid of the cat. The cat is aggressive toward other cats or dogs. 6.61 2.7 7.31 3.1 The cat does not get along with other pets. 6.18 2.9 6.86 3.2 The cat is not friendly. 6.04 3.0 6.34 3.2 The cat is timid or fearful or spends a lot of time hiding. 4.84 2.7 6.38 3.2 a Seriousness: 1 (not a problem at all)to10(a very serious problem). b Likely to ask for advice or help from an expert: 1 (not at all likely) to 10 (extremely likely). Half the respondents (cat owners) answered questions about cat behaviors and half (dog owners) answered questions about dog behaviors. Only five of the 170 participants kept only one animal; 40% of the cat owners also had dogs and 36.5% of the dog owners also had cats. Assessment of Pet Problem Behaviors Tables 1 and 2 present the respondents ratings of dog and cat behaviors, respectively. Each provides means for the seriousness of each behavior and shows the respondents estimation of how likely they think they would be to ask for advice from an expert in dealing with the behavior. Among dog owners, aggression toward people was viewed as the most serious problem, followed by not getting along with, or frightening, the children in the family. Fear of people, storms, and loud noises was seen as least serious, with the mean falling below the halfway point on the 1 to 10 scale. This item is not the same as separation anxiety, which was described as the dog s becoming upset and destructive when no one is at home (seriousness mean = 7.28). Among cat owners, spraying in the house was seen as the most serious problem, followed by aggression toward people, and not reliably using the litter box. As

68 SHORE, BURDSAL, DOUGLAS TABLE 3 Factor Analysis: Seriousness of Dog Problem Behaviors and Factor Correlation Matrix Item Factor 1 Destructiveness Factor 2 Unsociability Factor 3 Overactivity The dog chews furniture, shoes, or other things.706.063.085 in the house. The dog destroys things in the yard or garage..632.170.332 When no one is home, the dog gets upset and.519.178.058 destructive. The dog escapes from the house or yard..511.045.139 The dog is aggressive toward people, growling.185.843.020 and/or biting. The dog doesn t get along with your children,.072.590.028 or the children are afraid of the dog. The dog does not get along with your other.299.404.185 pets. The dog is hyper, running and jumping too.190.079.932 much. The dog barks a lot..039.312.374 The dog is aggressive toward other dogs or.235.237.036 cats. The dog is disobedient, doesn t listen..214.113.226 The dog is not completely housebroken and.184.270.273 sometimes urinates or defecates (pees or poops) in the house. The dog is afraid of people, storms, or loud noises..134.216.166 Dog Problem Behavior Factors Factor 1 Destructiveness Factor 2 Unsociability Factor 3 Overactivity 1 Destructiveness 1.000.532.549 2 Unsociability 1.000.396 3 Overactivity 1.000 with dogs, timidity and fearfulness were viewed as least serious, falling just below the halfway point on the scale. On the other hand, problems relative to the children in the family were not considered as serious as they were relative to dogs. The seriousness data were also factor analyzed. Cattell s Scree and Kaiser- Guttmann tests indicated three factors that were extracted using an iterative Principal Axis solution followed by a Promax Oblique rotation with Kaiser normalization (Gorsuch, 1983). Loadings were considered salient at.35 or above. The three dog-behavior problem factors, which accounted for 50.2% of the variance before rotation, were labeled Destructiveness, Unsociability, and Overactivity (Table 3). The three cat-behavior problem factors, which accounted for 63.4% of

PET BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS 69 TABLE 4 Factor Analysis: Seriousness of Cat Problem Behaviors and Factor Correlation Matrix Item Factor 1 Destructiveness Factor 2 Unsociability Factor 3 Timidity The cat sprays in the house. 1.017.085 -.187 The cat doesn t always use its litter box and.540.041.189 sometimes urinates of defecates (pees or poops) in the house. The cat damages or destroys things by.533.146.480 scratching, biting, or chewing. The cat does not get along with other pets..006.738.034 The cat is aggressive toward other cats or.081.602.006 dogs. The cat is aggressive toward people..065.478.068 The cat is not friendly..152.404.391 The cat does not get along with your.034.377.261 children, or they are afraid of the cat. The cat is timid or fearful or spends a lot of time hiding..051.061.559 Cat Problem Behavior Factors Factor 1 Destructiveness Factor 2 Unsociability Factor 3 Timidity 1 Destructiveness 1.000.265.307 2 Unsociability 1.000.498 3 Timidity 1.000 the variance before rotation, were labeled Destructiveness, Unsociability, and Timidity (Table 4). One item, The cat is not friendly, loaded almost equally on two factors, Unsociability and Timidity. The three dog-problem behavior factors were moderately correlated, and the three cat-problem behavior factors were relatively independent. Male and female pet owners ratings were compared by discriminant analyses, using the three factors as predictors and gender as the grouping variable. There were no significant gender differences for cat or for dog owners. It should be noted that the low number of male respondents in the two samples may have affected the results. A linear regression analysis, using the three factors as predictors, yielded no significant differences for dog or cat owners by income. Participants assessments of the seriousness of the problem were compared with their likelihood of asking for expert help with the problem. Among dog owners, 6 of the 13 correlations between seriousness of problem and likelihood of asking for help were statistically significant (with Bonferroni correction, p #.004): Aggression toward people, r =.324, p =.003; Escapes house/yard, r =.316, p =

70 SHORE, BURDSAL, DOUGLAS TABLE 5 Ratings of Ways to Get Help With Pet Behaviors by Dog and Cat Owners Dog Owners Cat Owners Item M SD M SD Visit a free animal behavior Web site. 8.85 2.3 8.91 2.1 Call a veterinarian for free advice. 8.68 2.5 8.82 2.2 Buy a book on how to solve animal behavior 7.76 2.8 8.06 2.1 problems. Call an animal behavior helpline for free advice. 7.45 3.0 7.22 3.5 Call an animal behaviorist for free advice. 7.41 3.0 7.28 3.4 Call an animal shelter for free advice. 7.12 3.3 7.60 3.2 Pay for a class on solving animal behavior problems 6.85 3.0 6.25 3.2 at the Humane Society. Pay for a visit to the vet to discuss the problem. 6.75 3.3 7.47 2.6 Pay an animal behaviorist to work with you and 5.44 3.0 5.14 3.3 your pet. Pay for a class on solving animal behavior problems 5.26 3.3 5.48 3.3 at a pet store like PetSmart. Call an animal behavior helpline that charges for its 4.29 3.0 4.86 3.3 services. Visit an animal behavior website that charges for its services. 3.47 2.6 3.98 3.0 Note. 1 = Never would; 10 = Definitely would..003; Aggression toward other dogs or cats, r =.421, p <.001; Destructive to yard or garage, r =.371, p <.001; Barks a lot, r =.434, p <.001; Afraid of people, storms, loud noises, r =.388, p <.001). Among cat owners, 4 of the 9 correlations between seriousness of problem and likelihood of asking for help were statistically significant (with Bonferroni correction, p #.006): Aggression toward other cats or dogs, r =.325, p =.002; Damages or destroys things, r =.313, p =.004; Does not get along with other pets, r =.426, p <.001; Timid, fearful, hiding, r =.03, p =.001. Ratings of Ways to Get Help Table 5 presents mean ratings of the likelihood that participants would use various ways of obtaining assistance with pet behavior problems. With a few exceptions (all participants: buying a book; cat guardians: paying for a visit to a veterinarian), all the options involving fees or payments received lower scores than did those presented as free. Visiting an animal behavior Web site was rated highest when the visit was free; adding a charge to that service resulted in the

PET BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS 71 rating dropping to last place. Calling an animal behavior helpline, rated 4th by dog owners and 7th by cat owners, dropped to 11th for both when a charge was added. There were no statistically significant differences in the ratings by gender for either cat or dog owners. Similarly, there were no differences in the ratings by lower versus higher annual income. DISCUSSION The participants in this study were pet owners visiting an animal shelter in a moderately large midwestern U.S. city. Although income and age varied, almost all were White, and slightly more than three quarters were female. Data were collected through face-to-face interviews with one of two female interviewers. As with other studies, these demographic and methodological factors limit the generalizability of the results. The majority of the problems asked about were rated as quite serious. This is not unexpected, given that the behaviors were compiled from studies of common reasons owners relinquish their pets to animal shelters. The order of the ratings points to an underlying theme: how much the human is directly affected, or endangered, by the behavior. This is clearest among dog owners, where aggression and not getting along with children were rated as most serious. Behaviors characterized by destruction of property appeared next. At the bottom were fearfulness and high energy, behaviors that may not greatly affect the humans lifestyle or require an immediate response. A similar trend was found among cat owners responses. Aggression toward people was second in seriousness, but animal-to-animal aggression was not as highly ranked as it was among dog owners. The inability of the cat and children to get along, or children s fear of the cat, was mid-range in the rankings. The perception that cat aggression is less serious may be related to cats smaller size. Spraying, which received the highest mean rating, can be seen as destruction of property but also could be a problem which, by virtue of its frequency and the odor and mess produced, more immediately and directly affects the humans in the household. As with dogs, fearfulness ranked lowest. The factor analyses provided support for the aforementioned. Among both dogand cat-owning respondents, factors indicative of aggression and of destructiveness emerged. The third factor timidity for cat owners and overactivity for dog owners reflects behaviors that affect the humans less immediately or seriously. Participants gender and income were not related to their ratings of the seriousness of the behaviors or the likelihood that the person would seek expert assistance. The similarity in response suggests that programs to prevent or treat pet behavior problems could be equally attractive to men and women and to people of different income levels. The results for gender may have been affected by the low

72 SHORE, BURDSAL, DOUGLAS number of male pet owners in the sample; further study of male and female views of pet behavior problems is needed as is additional research to confirm the other findings and to consider other human demographic variables. As might be expected, participants indicated that they were more likely to use free help options than for-fee services. Consulting an animal behavior Web site was rated highest if the use of the Web site was without cost; adding a charge dropped this option to last place. A similar reaction to the use of a helpline suggests that pet owners may not be willing to pay for less personal assistance, even if they are comfortable with the technology involved. Respondents rated buying a book on how to solve animal behavior problems relatively highly, and some reported that they had already purchased such books. Patronek et al. (1996a) found that reading a book was not related to decreased risk of relinquishment among dog owners; however, Patronek et al. (1996b) found it was protective for cats. The relationship between perceptions of the seriousness of a behavior problem and the person s reported willingness to seek expert help is less clear. Likelihood of seeking help was positively correlated with perceived seriousness in somewhat fewer than half of the cases, and problems for which there was a positive correlation were not necessarily the most serious ones. More research is needed to determine what leads pet owners to obtain or accept expert assistance with pet problems, as insight into such factors could result in programs to reduce relinquishment and improve human-companion animal relationships. ACKNOWLEDGMENT We thank the director and staff of the Kansas Humane Society for their cooperation and assistance with this project. REFERENCES Arkow, P. S., & Dow, S. (1984). The ties that do not bind: A study of the human-animal bonds that fail. In R. K Anderson, B. L. Hart, & L. A. Hart (Eds.), The pet connection: Its influence on our health and quality of life (pp. 348 354). Minneapolis: Center to Study Human-Animal Relationships and Environments, University of Minnesota. Bennett, P. C., & Rohlf, V. I. (2007). Owner-companion dog interactions: Relationships between demographic variables, potentially problematic behaviours, training engagement and shared activities. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 102, 65 84. Clark, G. I., & Boyer, W. N. (1993). The effects of dog obedience training and behavioural counselling upon the human-canine relationship. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 37, 147 159. Coren, S. (1999). Psychology applied to animal training. In A. M Stec & D. A. Bernstein (Eds.), Psychology: Fields of application (pp. 199 217). Boston: Houghton Mifflin. DiGiacomo, N., Arluke, A., & Patronek, G. J. (1998). Surrendering pets to shelters: The relinquisher s perspective. Anthrozoös, 11(1), 41 51.

PET BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS 73 Gorsuch, R. (1983). Factor analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jagoe, A., & Serpell, J. (1996). Owner characteristics and interactions and the prevalence of canine behaviour problems. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 47, 31 42. Marston, L. C., Bennett, P. C., & Coleman, G. J. (2004). What happens to shelter dogs? An analysis of data for 1 year from three Australian shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 7, 27 47. Miller, D. D., Staats, S. R., Partlo, C., & Rada, K. (1996). Factors associated with the decision to surrender a pet to an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 209, 738 742. New, J. C., Salman, M. D., King, M., Scarlett, J. M., Kass, P. H., & Hutchison, J. M. (2000). Characteristics of shelter-relinquished animals and their owners compared with animals and their owners in U.S. pet-owning households. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 3, 179 201. Patronek, G. J., Glickman, L. T., Beck, A. M., McCabe, G. P., & Ecker, C. (1996a). Risk factors for relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 209, 572 581. Patronek, G. J., Glickman, L. T., Beck, A. M., McCabe, G. P., & Ecker, C. (1996b). Risk factors for relinquishment of cats to an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 209, 582 588. Salman, M. D., Hutchinson, J., Ruch-Gallie, R., Kogan, L., New, J. C., Kass, P. H., et al. (2000). Behavioral reasons for relinquishment of dogs and cats to 12 shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 3, 93 106. Salman, M. D., New, J. G., Scarlett, J. M., Kass, P. H., Ruch-Gallie, R., & Hetts, S. (1998). Human and animal factors related to the relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 1, 207 226. Shore, E. R. (2005). Returning a recently adopted companion animal: Adopters reasons for, and reactions to, the failed adoption experience. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 8, 187 198.