Observations on the incubation and post-hatching behaviour o f the G reenland W hite-fronted G oose

Similar documents
Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Vigilance Behaviour in Barnacle Geese

Inland and saltmarsh feeding of wintering Brent Geese in Essex

Autumn staging behaviour in Pink-footed Geese; a similar contribution among sexes in parental care

Spring migration of Greenland White-fronted Geese through Iceland

EIDER JOURNEY It s Summer Time for Eiders On the Breeding Ground

Winning with warts? A threat posture suggests a function for caruncles in Ross s Geese

SOME PHOTOGRAPHIC STUDIES OF THE PINK-FOOTED GOOSE

WWT/JNCC/SNH Goose & Swan Monitoring Programme survey results 2015/16

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Wildfowl & Wetlands Trust Pink-footed Goose Anser brachyrhynchus Project: a report on the first three seasons

What is the date at which most chicks would have been expected to fledge?

GOOSE POPULATION STUDIES,

The story of Solo the Turnbull National Wildlife Refuge Male Swan

4. OTHER GOOSE SPECIES IN THE WILLAMETTE VALLEY AND LOWER COLUMBIA RIVER

Subject: Preliminary Draft Technical Memorandum Number Silver Lake Waterfowl Survey

Citation for published version (APA): Prop, J. (2004). Food finding: On the trail to successful reproduction in migratory geese. Groningen: s.n.

THE HOLLINGWORTH GREENLAND WHITE-FRONTED GEESE

The feeding behaviour of Greylag and Pink-footed Geese around the Moray Firth,

SOME OBSERVATIONS ON WILD GEESE IN SPITSBERGEN

Anas clypeata (Northern Shoveler)

Breeding success of Greylag Geese on the Outer Hebrides, September 2016

In the summers of 1977 and 1978, at Gibraltar Point, Lincolnshire, I

The behaviour of a pair of House Sparrows while rearing young

T HE recent and interesting paper by Alexander F. Skutch (1962) stimulated

ANSER BRACHYRHYNCHUS AN D G REYLAG A. ANSER

EVALUATION OF A METHOD FOR ESTIMATING THE LAYING RATE OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS

Survey of the feeding areas, roosts and flight activity of qualifying species of the Caithness Lochs Special Protection Area; 2011/12 and 2012/13

Naturalised Goose 2000

Diurnal time-activity budgets and habitat use of Lesser Snow Geese Anser caerulescens in the middle Missouri River valley during winter and spring

Scottish Natural Heritage Diversionary feeding of hen harriers on grouse moors. a practical guide

PREDATION, BODY SIZE, AND ENERGETICS

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1996) provides a comprehensive account.

( 142 ) NOTES ON THE GREAT NORTHERN DIVER.

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

FITNESS CONSEQUENCES OF PARENTAL BEHAVIOR IN RELATION TO OFFSPRING NUMBER IN A PRECOCIAL

PEREGRINE FALCON HABITAT MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES ONTARIO MINISTRY OF NATURAL RESOURCES

VARIATION IN BROOD BEHAVIOR OF BLACK BRANT

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

Studies on the effects of disturbances on staging Brent Geese: a progress report

She is best known for her Newbery Medal-winning novel for young adults, Hitty, Her First Hundred Years, published in 1929.

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

The hen harrier in England

(170) COURTSHIP AND DISPLAY OF THE SLAVONIAN GREBE.

Distribution and abundance of Tule Geese in California and southern Oregon

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL

(135) OBSERVATIONS IN A ROOKERY DURING THE INCUBATION PERIOD C. M. OGILVIE.

(261) THE INCUBATION PERIOD OF THE OYSTER-CATCHER

Nesting behaviour of male and female Whistling Swans and implications of male incubation

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Chapman (1999) provides a comprehensive account.

EFFECTS OF MALE REMOVAL ON FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE BIOLOGY IN ROSS AND LESSER SNOW GEESE

You may use the information and images contained in this document for non-commercial, personal, or educational purposes only, provided that you (1)

ACTIVITY BUDGETS OF CANADA GEESE DURING BROOD REARING

The Kongsfjorden colony of barnacle geese: Nest distribution and the use of breeding islands

Chloroceryle americana (Green Kingfisher)

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

The Long-term Effect of Precipitation on the Breeding Success of Golden Eagles Aquila chrysaetos homeyeri in the Judean and Negev Deserts, Israel

The distribution, fall migration routes and survival of Ross s Geese

Animal Biodiversity. Teacher Resources - High School (Cycle 1) Biology Redpath Museum

Swans & Geese. Order Anseriformes Family Anserinae

POPULATION STUDY OF GREATER SNOW GEESE ON BYLOT ISLAND (NWT) IN 1998: A PROGRESS REPORT

( 162 ) SOME BREEDING-HABITS OF THE LAPWING.

Egg-laying by the Cuckoo

EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Egg laying Late May to early June Mid-May to mid-july 3 to 10

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

BUILDING A HOME (NESTS) VOLUNTEER DIRECTIONS

University of Canberra. This thesis is available in print format from the University of Canberra Library.

Canada Goose Management Practices Jake Nave

Waterfowl Along the Road

NOTES ON THE SPRING TERRITORY OF THE BLACKBIRD

Activity 4 Building Bird Nests

OBSERVATIONS ON SWALLOWS AND HOUSE- MARTINS AT THE NEST. BY

The Recent Nesting History of the Bald Eagle in Rondeau Provincial Park, Ontario.

Population Study of Canada Geese of Jackson Hole

ISLAY SUSTAINABLE GOOSE MANAGEMENT STRATEGY OCTOBER 2014 APRIL 2024

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

Trunk Contents. Crane Flight Feathers (3)

Short Report Key-site monitoring on Hornøya in Rob Barrett & Kjell Einar Erikstad

ESTIMATING NEST SUCCESS: WHEN MAYFIELD WINS DOUGLAS H. JOHNSON AND TERRY L. SHAFFER

Wayne E. Melquist Idaho Cooperative Wildlife Research Unit University of Idaho Moscow, Idaho 83843

Western Snowy Plover Recovery and Habitat Restoration at Eden Landing Ecological Reserve

The impact of disturbance on the behaviour and energy budgets of Brent Geese Branta b. bernicla

POPULATION STUDY OF GREATER SNOW GEESE ON BYLOT ISLAND (NUNAVUT) IN 2000: A PROGRESS REPORT

6 Month Progress Report. Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa. VulPro NPO

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Incubation feeding in snow buntings: female manipulation or indirect male parental care?

How to Raise Healthy Geese for the Backyard Farm

REPORT OF ACTIVITIES TURTLE ECOLOGY RESEARCH REPORT Crescent Lake National Wildlife Refuge 31 May to 4 July 2017

Influence of supplementary food on the behaviour of Greylag Geese Anser anser in an urban environment

Oecologia. Environmental change and the cost of philopatry: an example in the lesser snow goose. Oecologia (1993) 93: Springer-Verlag 1993

GNARALOO TURTLE CONSERVATION PROGRAM 2011/12 GNARALOO CAPE FARQUHAR ROOKERY REPORT ON FINAL RECONNAISSANCE SURVEY (21 23 FEBRUARY 2012)

He was a year older than her and experienced in how to bring up a brood and survive.

Rock Wren Nesting in an Artificial Rock Wall in Folsom, Sacramento County, California

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist

A POSSIBLE FACTOR IN THE EVOLUTION OF CLUTCH SIZE IN ROSS GOOSE JOHN P. RYDER

OBSERVATIONS OF PEMBROKE PINES BALD EAGLE NEST - FWC ID# BO-002

Biological notes on the Em peror Goose in north-east Siberia

Transcription:

Observations on the incubation and post-hatching behaviour o f the G reenland W hite-fronted G oose DAVID A. STROUD Introduction The breeding behaviour of the dispersed nesting Greenland W hite-fronted Goose Anser albifrons flavirostris was studied in May and June 1979 as part of an investigation of W hitefront summer ecology in west Greenland (Fox & Stroud 1981a). The aim was to com pare the behaviour of the W hitefront during incubation with that of the better known, m ore densely nesting Pink-footed Goose A. brachyrhynchus (Inglis 1977; Lazarus & Inglis 1978). There have been several behavioural studies of colonially and densely nesting arctic geese, but less attention has been given to dispersed breeders. Ely (1979) briefly discusses the behaviour of the Pacific W hitefront A. albifrons frontalis as it relates to breeding on the Yukon-K uskokwim Delta, but the nesting habitats and densities there are very different to those in west Greenland, and it seems likely that many aspects of Greenland W hitefront summer ecology may be peculiar to this race (Fox & Stroud 1981b). The range of the W hite-fronted Goose in G reenland extends from 64-72 N in the ice-free regions of the west. The small population size (c. 15,000) and low summer densities of birds have resulted in little published inform ation on summer biology; neither Fencker (1950) nor Salomonsen (1950) discuss breeding behaviour. that in favourable areas the nest density did not exceed one per 2 km2. An estimate of total numbers based on a partial census in July 1979 suggested that only 16 pairs nested successfully here. O ne nest was observed throughout the incubation period; continuous observations starting a week after incubation commenced. The marsh near which the nest was situated, was surrounded by heathherb slopes dominated by Calamagrostis purpurescens. These merged into wetter communities along the sides of the marsh, and the nest was situated in this transitional vegetation zone. The central regions of Í0 mil» Study area and methods The study area of Eqalungmiut Nunât (67 50'N) is a discrete area of 750 km 2 close to the ice-cap and in the southern central part of the range of the W hitefront in Greenland (Figure 1). Conditions are thought to be typical of large areas of the breeding grounds although densities may be slightly higher here close to the ice-cap than in the immediate surrounding areas (V. Holthe in litt., Stroud 1981a). Nesting pairs of W hitefronts were found predom i nantly near Eriophorum angustifolium dom inated marshes around 50-250 m altitude. Although impossible accurately to count nests over a large area, it is thought Figure 1. The breeding range of the Whitefronted Goose in west Greenland showing the location of the study area, Eqalungmiut Nunât. \ 4«W 63 Wildfowl 33 (1982):63-72

64 D avid A. Stroud the marsh contained a mixture of sedges, grasses and mosses, with numerous pools, still frozen in May. H ere, Eriophorum angustifolium and Carex rariflora were important food items for the geese (Madsen & Fox 1981), although the area was also characterized by Salix arctophila, Sphagnum squarrosum and Hippuris vulgaris. Observations were made from a wooden hide, 173 m from the nest, situated on a nearby hill and using x30 and x60 telescopes. M ethods were based on Inglis (1977); the marsh was scanned every five minutes and behaviour and position of all geese recorded. Behaviour was recorded as activities and postures: the former being largely locomotory states whilst the latter generally related to the degree of alertness of the bird (see Inglis 1977 for details). Hourly meteorological records were also taken. In presentation of diurnal rhythm data, those hours when a bird was out of sight for more than ten minutes have been excluded, as have data for the female for 36 hours and the male for 4 hours prior to hatching, since their behaviour changed significantly during this period. Presentation of long-term changes in behaviour is directly com parable to Inglis (1977). Results The nest was situated 500 m from Base Camp. Although the male moved further from the camp in the middle of the day (when presumably disturbance from this source would have been greatest), it is not thought that the normal behaviour of the geese was otherwise greatly altered, and indeed one afternoon the male was observed feeding only 30 m from the camp. Breeding chronology Clutch initiation by Greenland W hitefronts in Eqalungmiut Nunât in 1979 took place during 19-26 May. The studied goose probably laid her first egg on 22 May; the full clutch was six eggs, although only five successfully hatched. Incubation commenced on 27 May and the eggs hatched on 22 June, a period of 25 days. The nest was observed continuously (except for a break of 24 hours on 12 June), from the afternoon of 4 June until the goslings were led away to higher altitudes 1 0 hours after they hatched. A nother nest was observed during hatching and over the period 26-29 June (Stroud 1981b). H ere, the family stayed for longer at the nest site and were observed for 26 hours until they too, moved to upland lakes to feed. All results presented here are from the first nest unless otherwise stated. Female time budget The incubating female spent most of the day in one of two postures: head on back (48-7%) and head low (41-5% of 4,731 scans). O ther postures made a small contribution to the total (Figure 2): 2-3% of the tim e was spent head up, followed in order of decreasing importance by drinking droplets of water from the flanks, nest adjusting, grazing, preening, egg-rolling, concealment behaviour during predator presence, flying to feed, down-plucking and others. Long term trends infernale behaviour Feeding departures, always accompanied by the male, were regular in duration and timing (mean 24 minutes, range 15-35, n = 14), although they increased in frequency from once every other day, to occur daily between 13-19 June. She fed twice during 20 June then rem ained on the nest until the eggs hatched in the early hours of 22 June. Thus, over the observation period as a whole, she showed a significant increase in the time spent feeding (Figure 3). Male time budget During egg-laying, the male stood close to the nest and he also accompanied the female back whenever she had fed on the marsh. The rem ainder of the incubation period was spent on the marsh, generally about 300-500 m from the nest, but moving to within 2 m during the hours preceding the hatch. His time budget is shown in Figure 2. U p to seven other geese were present on the marsh for short periods but generally most frequent in early incubation between 02.00 and 10.00 hours. A nother pair was thought to have nested close by, but this nest apparently failed between 6 and 9 June. Hence although three, possibly four, nests were known to have been initiated near the marsh, only the study nest was successful. The small flock of non-breeding geese

G reenland W hite-fronted G oose observations 65 MALE P R E - HATCH PE R C E N T A G E P O S T -H A T C H P R E -HATCH PERCENTAGE 100 100 S w im W alk S it F lo a t Fly Figure 2. Time budgets for male and female geese pre- and post-hatch. In each diagram, left hand columns are the proportion of time spent before hatching and right hand columns are the proportion of time spent after hatching. U pper diagrams are postures, lower are activities. The data for female pre-hatching has been further sub-divided into behaviour in the absence of predators (upper, open columns, n = 4,661 scans), and behaviour in the presence of predators (lower, shaded columns, n = 70 scans). (r> so LU z o < CL 3 Q 16 20 JUNE Figure 3. Total time spent feeding each day by female Greenland Whitefront during incubation. No observations were made on 12 June.

66 D avid A. Stroud was tolerated by the male and few aggressive interactions were observed. He fed amongst them and occasionally flew with them to roost on a nearby hillside. N onbreeding geese rarely approached closer than 300 m from the nest; however, one young bird was chased from the nest area by the male after following the breeding pair back to the nest after feeding. Both sexes tried to hide when geese flew near the nest, generally more intense in response than to a predator such as Raven Corvus corax at a similar distance. A t the later hatching nest, the breeding pair was accompanied by an immature goose. It usually stood 2-20 m from the nest, most frequently c. 7 m. On one occasion the breeding male chased the immature from 6 to 2 0 m, despite closer approach previously. The male spent more time alert, less time in non-vigilant postures than the immature (14-4% of time non-viligant com pared to 50-8%). head up increased as incubation proceeded (Figure 4). There was no similar increase in time spent in the extreme head up posture, which was most frequently seen in the presence of predators and after the goslings hatched. Feeding declined (Figure 4), probably as the corollary of increased alertness. Time spent roosting showed little variability but was greatly reduced once the chicks had hatched. O ther activities and postures rem ained more or less constant but combined activities of floating and swimming decline significantly. Diurnal rhythms The nest was in the shadow of hills for part of the day, and there were distinct diurnal cycles of tem perature and light intensity at the nest and these affected behaviour (Figure 5). Long term trends in male behaviour The proportion of time the male spent Female behaviour Between the hours of 23.00 and 07.55, the tu 8 0-1 CD < LU o, CE 60 - Ui Q. 4 0-20 - I I i i i i J i J- - - - - - - - - 1 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 DAYS FROM START OF INCUBATION Figure 4. Long-term changes in male behaviour; grazing and extreme head up combined with head up. Values are the proportion of all scans for each day.

G reenland W hite-fronted G oose observations 67 T e m p e ra tu re»-------------------------------«0. 0 0 1. ' p «0. 0 0 ] p< 0. 0 ]»... «0. 01 < p» 0. 05 x > 0.0 5 < -p Figure 5. Correlations of diurnal behaviour. L in es jo in in g b e h a v io u rs in d ic a te th a t th e y a re sig n ific a n tly c o rre la te d (S p e a rm a n R a n k C o rre la tio n ). T y p e o f lin e in d ic a te s d e g re e o f s tatistic a l sig n ific a n ce as in d ic a te d, sign in d ic a te s p o s itiv e o r n e g a tiv e c o rre la tio n s. 75 HOUR Figure 6. Diurnal changes in female alertness (head low). V a lu e s a re m e a n h o u rly p r o p o r tio n o f tim e s p e n t o v e r th e in c u b a tio n ± s ta n d a rd e r r o r o f m e a n. H o riz o n ta l b a r is th e p e rio d th a t th e n e st w as in th e sh a d o w o f th e hills to th e n o rth, i.e., n ig h t'.

68 D avid A. Stroud female spent most of the time with head on back (Figure 6). The proportion spent with head low showed a close inverse relationship. The timing of the female's departures from the nest were mainly between 19.00 and 01.00 hours and these tended to be in the period of least predator activity (Figure 7). Male behaviour Changes in the behaviour of the male are shown in Figure 8. There was a peak of grazing between 22.00 and 05.00 hours while a high proportion of time between 06.00 and 18.00 hours was spent sitting either head up or extreme head up. Roosting (head on back) occurred throughout the day, but falling to a minimum between 16.00-19.00 hours. Preening occurred infrequently although usually between feeding and roosting. Although the sexes alternated vigilant behaviour during the pre-breeding period (Fox & Madsen 1981), the time of day at which the female was alert (head low) was the same as that of the male (head up and extreme head up) during incubation. This alert period of both sexes correlated significantly with the time of highest predator activity and tem perature (Figure 5). Post-hatching behaviour Immediately after hatching the female showed a significant decrease in time spent sleeping (head on back) and an increase in all vigilant postures (head low, head up and extreme head up) (Figure 2). She also started to eat intensively. Preening took up a greater proportion of her time and she frequently brooded the young. The male spent more time alert, and markedly less time in other activities, whilst close to the nest before the brood left for higher lakes (Figure 2). Family behaviour during the period from 48 hours after hatching to fledging is described by M adsen (1981). Discussion It has been noted that some geese nest at a wide range of densities, from solitary to quasi-colonial in other areas (Ely 1979). There are several reports of W hitefronts nesting at high densities, or with some degree of gregariousness, and Table 1 shows that the breeding density varies considerably, even given that only the assessments of Ely (1979) and Mickleson (1975) are likely to be accurate. This may imply that breeding density is affected by local factors (such as predator density, habitat OC 12 Z3 o x,o tr UJ 8 If) œ g 6. < Q LU OC 4 û. LU Cn 4-1 - J LU < C/) 5 CO - U J H IO O O co O AO CM (N HOUR Figure 7. Relationship between abundance of predators (summed hourly occurrence of all records in the observation period) and the feeding periods of the female away from the nest. P r e d a to r re c o rd s w ere o f o n e G y rfalco n Falco rusticolus, five arctic foxes, a n d 41 R avens.

G reenland W hite-fronted G oose observations 69 HOUR Figure 8. Diurnal changes in male behaviour; grazing and head up and extreme head up combined. Presentation as Figure 6. Table 1. Nesting densities of White-fronted Geese. Subspecies Anser albifrons frontalis Anser albifrons flavirostris Densities of Study pairs area (per km2) (km2) Size 0-4 S I800 Alaska 2 10-4 Yukon-Kuskokwim Delta, Alaska 3-0 9-8 Yukon-Kuskokwim Delta, 1977 4-7 9-8 Yukon-Kuskowkim D elta, 1978 5-7? Old Crow Flats, Alaska 14 1-3 Yukon-Kuskokwim Delta, Alaska 23-30? Point Barrow, Alaska <0-5 10 Eqalungmiut Nunât, Greenland Dzubin et al. 1964 Mickelson 1975 Ely 1979 Ely 1979 Dzubin et al. 1964 Michelson 1975 Dzubin et al. 1964 Stroud 1981a availability or topography), and that aspects of breeding behaviour may also vary with density. There was no evidence of any aggregation amongst nesting geese in Eqalungmiut N unât, and the smallest m easured internest distance of 1,170 m was very much greater than even the dispersed Pacific W hitefront (mean of 170 m, Ely 1979). Such dispersed breeding probably relates to the patchiness of the feeding marshes, predation and the small proportion of geese breeding (Fox & Stroud 1981b). In Alaska the nesting habitat utilized is the dominant vegetation over vast areas of tundra (e.g. the Yukon-Kuskokwim Delta: Ely 1979; Dzubin et al. 1964; Mickleson 1975), whilst in Greenland the marshes and wetlands are discrete. In the last days of laying (27/28 May) the

70 D avid A. Stroud male remained very close to the nest for 20 hours. This may represent a defence against intra-specific nest parasitism or rape, which has been reported for the Pacific W hitefront by Ely (1979), Lesser Snow G eese A nser caerulescens caerulescens by M ineau & Cooke (1979) and Barnacle Geese Branta leucopsis by Owen & Wells (1979). The male spent most of the day more than 300 m from the nest during incubation, much further than reported distances for other species. The one occasion when the nest was visited by another goose, the male was present and the intruder flew off after threats. A t the second nest, three immature geese were seen less than four metres from the nest a few hours before hatching; however, despite a lack of any overt aggression the male positioned himself between the nest and the geese. Inglis (1976) points out that it is difficult to envisage any evolutionary advantage in harrassm ent of colonially nesting geese by conspecifics, other than perhaps rape or the eating of eggs. For solitary breeders such an advantage is even more rem ote, and in almost 500 hours of observation no such attacks were noted. Both sexes did, however, hide when strange geese flew near the nest. If a predator attacked the nest, the male could be present within a few seconds, dem onstrated by the almost instantaneous response to the call of the female when she left the nest to feed. The female is camouflaged against a background of dead grasses and sedges, and when Ravens approached within 200 m of the nest, she frequently placed her head low on the ground reducing her silhouette and hiding the conspicuous white forehead in the vegetation (Fowles 1981). Two geese attem pting to hide in this way would be more conspicuous than one, and thus it may be of advantage for the male to be some distance from the nest so that he does not advertise its presence to predators after the clutch is complete. The time spent incubating each day by the single G reenland W hitefront studied (99.99%) is greater than both dispersed Pacific W hitefronts (97.3% ; Ely 1979) and densely nesting Pinkfeet (96-2%; Inglis 1977). There is a m arked increase in the time spent off the nest in the later stages of incubation (Figure 3). During incubation reserves of food accumulated over-winter and on arrival become progressively depleted (Ankney & M aclnnes 1978), and the increased time spent feeding is consistent with a greater need. The greater time spent on the nest by both races of W hitefront compared with Pinkfeet can be explained by the protective function of the colony. The female Pinkfoot never feeds far from the nest (Inglis 1977), and if a predator approaches, neighbouring geese provide sufficient alarm to allow her to return to the nest. W hitefronts do not have this protection and thus leave the nest for very short periods when there are fewest predators active. It is surprising that the female did not always cover the eggs with down, since uncovered and unprotected eggs would seem to be at some risk from Ravens. The reasons for the m ale s protection of his mate rather than the nest, presumably derive from the relatively small risk of nest predation com pared with the importance of allowing the female to feed undisturbed by possible harrassm ent from other geese. The total time spent grazing declined over incubation, probably because grazing is of less importance than altertness which correspondingly increases. This situation is very different from that of the male Pinkfoot which shows an increase in feeding and thus a low level of alertness until five days before the hatch, after which feeding declines and alertness markedly increases (Inglis 1977). A fter hatching the male is largely responsible for the protection of the family, allowing the female to feed intensively (Figure 2; M adsen 1981). Thus it is im portant that the male should be in good condition at hatching, and surprising that he tolerated other geese on the marsh, presumably com petitors for food. It may be that the marsh was large enough so that the feeding of other geese had little overall effect on the quantity of grazing. These non-breeding birds were free to move between feeding sites as the thaw released marshes throughout June, and probably followed a gradient of plant growth through the study marsh and onto the plateau areas (Madsen & Fox 1981). The breeding male however, is restricted to feeding in a relatively small area. N onbreeders are likely to be tolerated by the male because of their transitory com petition at any one site. W hen the immediate post-hatching budgets are com pared with those during incubation in the presence of predators (Figure 2), it can be seen that they are closely similar. Thus the behaviour shown immediately after hatching, presumably the period of greatest risk to the goslings, may be anticipatory of predators.

G reenland W hite-fronted G oose observations 71 The association of an immature goose with the breeders of the second nest was only observed after hatching, although the bird was present throughout incubation. The role of the immature W hitefronts in families after hatching is described by Madsen (1981) and Stroud (1981b). It is possible that this behaviour, previously noted by Ely (1979) in Pacific W hitefronts, is a form of alloparental care, whereby non-breeding birds in their first or second summers remain with their parents and this aspect deserves further study. The marked rhythm of both male and female behaviour described here has only rarely been reported in arctic nesting geese. Inglis (1977) found that Pink-footed Geese showed no diurnal rhythms during a daily observation period of 05.00-23.00 hours, although a few observations carried out outside those hours found that females were invariably on the nest with the male sitting close by. It is probable that the geese are responding to environmental cues such as tem perature and light intensity when changing behaviour. Ultimately, the geese probably respond to the activity of predators, being m ore alert when there is greatest chance of attack from either arctic fox A lopex lagopus or Raven. If the frequency of fox calls is an accurate reflection of their activity, then from counts made over 24 hour periods they seem to be less active during the early hours of the day. Thus vigilant behaviour seems to be timed in both sexes as to be greatest when there is most risk of predation. Prop et al. (1978) found that nesting Barnacle Geese in high-arctic Svalbard (77 N) showed a distinct diurnal rhythmicity in the timing of their feeding. Males left the nesting island to feed on the nearby mainland most frequently at night (18.00-03.00 hours) whilst the females fed more often in the day (09.00-17.00 hours). The timing of feeding by the female was attributed to two factors: the smaller energetic costs of rewarming eggs on return, and the higher concentrations of carbohydrates available in forage typical of arctic plants in the day (Wilson 1954). Both these factors will apply in Eqalungmiut N unât, so the fact that the female Greenland W hitefront chooses a later time of day to feed may indicate that these benefits are outweighed by the greater predation risk to a solitary nesting goose. Acknowledgements The Greenland W hite-fronted Goose Study was supported by generous contributions from many sources, in particular extensive support from the D epartm ent of Fisheries and Forestry, Dublin, the N A TO Eco-Sciences Panel, W AGBI and the W orld Wildlife Fund, D enm ark. O ther valuable support and advice was received from the Royal Geographical Society, the University College of W ales, Aberystwyth, and the Wildfowl Trust; the R A F gave logistical support and Racal-Tacticom Ltd loaned high quality radio equipm ent. The observations reported here were made by John Bell, John Floyd, A drian Fowles, Tony Fox, Alison and William Higgs and Jesper M adsen, and all m em bers of the expedition made useful contributions to draft manuscripts. The study was aided by m any people, fully acknowledged in Fox & Stroud (1981a), but in particular help was received from D r I. R. Inglis, D r M. Owen and M r A. Jennings, whilst D r J. W arnes, S. Newton and especially Tony Fox gave their time in improving early drafts. Summary A behavioural study during the sum m er of 1979 in west G reenland of the incubation and imm ediate post-hatching period of two Greenland W hite-fronted Goose Anser albifrons flavirostris nests is reported, totalling about 500 hours of continuous observation. A t the time of clutch completion the male stood a few m etres from the nest, but later moved away and spent the rest of the incubation period feeding on a nearby marsh. A t hatching he moved closer to the nest once more. Both sexes show strong diurnal rhythmicity, being most alert when predator activity is at a maximum. The significance of these cycles is discussed with reference to other arctic diurnal cycles. The male shows a significant increase in time spent vigilant over the incubation period which affects the time apportioned to other activities such as grazing. The female left the nest roughly once a day at a constant time (low predator activity) and for a constant period; however, the frequency of these recesses increased throughout incubation. A t the time of hatch, both sexes markedly changed their behaviour, the male becoming more attentive allowing the female to spend longer feeding. The significance of this change is discussed and the association of an immature goose with a nesting pair is m entioned. The results of the study are com pared with previous work undertaken on the densely nesting Pinkfooted Goose A. brachyrhynchus.

72 D avid A. Stroud References Ankney, C. D. & M aclnnes, C. D. 1978. Nutrient reserves and reproductive perform ance of female Lesser Snow Geese. A u k 95: 459-71. Dzubin, A., Miller, H. W. & Schildman, G. I. 1964. W hitefronts. Pp. 135-43 in J. P. Linduska (ed.) Waterfowl Tomorrow. W ashington: US Govmt. Printing Office. Ely, C. R. 1979. Breeding biology of W hite-fronted Geese. Unpub. M.Sc. thesis, University of California, Davis. Fencker, H. 1950. Den gr0nlandske Blisgâs (Anser albifrons flavirostris, Scott & Dalgety) og dens ynglebiologi. Dansk orn. Foren. Tidsskr. 44: 61-5. Fowles, A. P. 1981. Behavioural responses of breeding W hite-fronted Geese Anser albifrons flavirostris to the proximity of predators. Pp. 103-7, Fox, A. D. & Stroud, D. A. 1981a. Fox, A. D. & M adsen, J. 1981. The pre-nesting behaviour of the Greenland W hite-fronted Goose. W ildfowl 32: 48-54. Fox, A. D. & Stroud, D. A. 1981a. (eds.) Report o f the 1979 Greenland White-fronted Goose Study Expedition to Eqalungmiut Nunât, west Greenland. Aberystwyth. Fox, A. D. & Stroud, D. A. 1981b. The summer ecology and natural history of the Greenland W hite-fronted Goose. Pp. 148-55, in Fox, A. D. & Stroud, D. A. 1981a. Inglis, I. R. 1976. Agnostic behaviour of breeding Pink-footed geese with reference to Ryders Hypothesis. Wildfowl 27: 95-9. Inglis, I. R. 1977. The breeding behaviour of the pink-footed goose: behavioural correlates of nesting success. Anim. Behav. 25: 747-64. Lazarus, J. & Inglis, I. R. 1978. The breeding behaviour of the pink-footed goose: parental care and vigilant behaviour during the fledging period. Behaviour 65: 62-88. M adsen, J. 1981. Post-hatching behaviour of families and non-breeding G reenland W hite-fronted Geese. Pp. 116-22, in Fox, A. D. & Stroud, D. A. 1981a. M adsen, J. & Fox, A. D. 1981. The summer diet of the Greenland W hite-fronted Goose. Pp. 108-15 in Fox, A. D. & Stroud, D. A. 1981a. Mickleson, P. G. 1975. Breeding biology of Cackling geese and associated species on the Yukon-K uskokwim delta, Alaska. Wildlife Monographs 45: 1-35. M ineau, P. & Cooke, F. 1979. Rape in the lesser Snow Goose. Behaviour 70: 280-91. Ow en, M. & W ells, R. 1979. Territorial behaviour in breeding geese a re-examination of R yder s hypothesis. Wildfowl 30: 20-26. Prop, J., Van E erdan, M. R., D aan, S., D rent, R. H., Tinbergen, J. M. & St. Joseph, A. M. 1978. Ecology of the Barnacle Goose (Branta leucopsis) during the breeding season. Preliminary results from expeditions to Spitzbergen in 1977 & 1978. Pp. 51-112 in Spitzbergen Symposium. Holland: University of Groningen. Salomonsen, F. 1950. Gr0nlands fugle. K0benhavn. Stroud, D. A. 1981a. The distribution and abundance of the Greenland W hite-fronted Goose in Eqalungm iut Nunât. Pp. 51-62, In Fox, A. D. & Stroud, D. A. 1981a. Stroud, D. A. 1981b. The breeding behaviour of the Greenland W hite-fronted Goose. Pp. 82-102, in Fox, A. D. & Stroud, D. A. 1981a. Wilson, J. W. 1954. The influence of the midnight sun conditions on certain diurnal rhythms in Oxyria digyna. J. Ecol. 42: 81-94. D. A. Stroud, School of Biological Sciences, University College of Wales, Penglais, Aberystwyth, Dyfed, SY23 3D A.