Movement correlates of lizards dorsal pigmentation patterns

Similar documents
CAMBRIDGE, MASS. 26 MARCH 2010 NUMBER 519 CRUISE FORAGING OF INVASIVE CHAMELEON (CHAMAELEO JACKSONII XANTHOLOPHUS) IN HAWAI I

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour

MA41 Colour variability and the ecological use of colour in the chameleons and geckos of Mahamavo

Foraging by the Omnivorous Lizard Podarcis lilfordi: Effects of Nectivory in an Ancestrally Insectivorous Active Forager

Colorful tails fade when lizards adopt less risky behaviors

Do the traits of organisms provide evidence for evolution?

Supplementary Fig. 1: Comparison of chase parameters for focal pack (a-f, n=1119) and for 4 dogs from 3 other packs (g-m, n=107).

Objectives: Outline: Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles. Characteristics of Amphibians. Types and Numbers of Amphibians

8/19/2013. What is convergence? Topic 11: Convergence. What is convergence? What is convergence? What is convergence? What is convergence?

Introduction to phylogenetic trees and tree-thinking Copyright 2005, D. A. Baum (Free use for non-commercial educational pruposes)

CLADISTICS Student Packet SUMMARY Phylogeny Phylogenetic trees/cladograms

Modern Evolutionary Classification. Lesson Overview. Lesson Overview Modern Evolutionary Classification

LAB. NATURAL SELECTION

SOAR Research Proposal Summer How do sand boas capture prey they can t see?

8/19/2013. Who eats herps? Topic 20: Predators. Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps?

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

Evolution of Birds. Summary:

Introduction to the Cheetah

The evolution and function of pattern diversity in snakes

Impact of colour polymorphism and thermal conditions on thermoregulation, reproductive success, and development in Vipera aspis

Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Typical Snakes Part # 1

Dipsas trinitatis (Trinidad Snail-eating Snake)

Pre-lab Homework Lab 8: Natural Selection

Is it better to be bigger? Featured scientists: Aaron Reedy and Robert Cox from the University of Virginia Co-written by Matt Kustra

Bio homework #5. Biology Homework #5

Interpreting Evolutionary Trees Honors Integrated Science 4 Name Per.

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

Higher National Unit Specification. General information for centres. Unit code: F3V4 34

Life Cycle of a Leopard

5 State of the Turtles

Comparative Zoology Portfolio Project Assignment

The Effect of Aerial Exposure Temperature on Balanus balanoides Feeding Behavior

SHEEP SIRE REFERENCING SCHEMES - NEW OPPORTUNITIES FOR PEDIGREE BREEDERS AND LAMB PRODUCERS a. G. Simm and N.R. Wray

A COMPARATIVE TEST OF ADAPTIVE HYPOTHESES FOR SEXUAL SIZE DIMORPHISM IN LIZARDS

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

muscles (enhancing biting strength). Possible states: none, one, or two.

INQUIRY & INVESTIGATION

Lecture 11 Wednesday, September 19, 2012

APPLICATION OF BODY CONDITION INDICES FOR LEOPARD TORTOISES (GEOCHELONE PARDALIS)

Supporting Online Material for

Plestiodon (=Eumeces) fasciatus Family Scincidae

8/19/2013. Topic 14: Body support & locomotion. What structures are used for locomotion? What structures are used for locomotion?

THE EFFECTS OF MORPHOLOGY AND PERCH DIAMETER ON SPRINT PERFORMANCE OF ANOLIS LIZARDS

[ Post a Response Precious Fids Chat ] Novel Chemistry at Work To Provide Parrot's Vibrant Red Colors.

The impact of the recognizing evolution on systematics

A tail of two scorpions Featured scientists: Ashlee Rowe and Matt Rowe from University of Oklahoma

Title: Phylogenetic Methods and Vertebrate Phylogeny

Seasonal Shifts in Reproductive Investment of Female Northern Grass Lizards ( Takydromus septentrionalis

STUDY BEHAVIOR OF CERTAIN PARAMETERS AFFECTING ASSESSMENT OF THE QUALITY OF QUAIL EGGS BY COMPUTER VISION SYSTEM

Activity 1: Changes in beak size populations in low precipitation

Sources And Consequences of Ecological Intraspecific Variation In The Florida Scrub Lizard (Sceloporus Woodi)

These small issues are easily addressed by small changes in wording, and should in no way delay publication of this first- rate paper.

Impact of colour polymorphism in free ranging asp vipers

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

Comparing DNA Sequence to Understand

Fact Sheet: Oustalet s Chameleon Furcifer oustaleti

HIGLEY UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT INSTRUCTIONAL ALIGNMENT. Zoology Quarter 3. Animal Behavior (Duration 2 Weeks)

Shooting the poop Featured scientist: Martha Weiss from Georgetown University

A Conglomeration of Stilts: An Artistic Investigation of Hybridity

Squamates of Connecticut

Longevity of the Australian Cattle Dog: Results of a 100-Dog Survey

Texas Quail Index. Result Demonstration Report 2016

University of Canberra. This thesis is available in print format from the University of Canberra Library.

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

TESTING AND TRAINING FOR PROPER DEFENSE AGGRESSION

Result Demonstration Report

Rules of the Game. Lab Report - on a separate sheet

Correlated evolution of thermal characteristics and foraging strategy in lacertid lizards

Color On, Color Off Multidisciplinary Classroom Activities

NATURAL SELECTION SIMULATION

DECREASED SPRINT SPEED AS A COST OF REPRODUCTION IN THE LIZARD SCELOPORUS OCCIDENTALS: VARIATION AMONG POPULATIONS

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

Reptile Identification Guide

Texas Quail Index. Result Demonstration Report 2016

A Population Analysis of the Common Wall Lizard Podarcis muralis in Southwestern France

Lab 7. Evolution Lab. Name: General Introduction:

ANTHR 1L Biological Anthropology Lab

Geo 302D: Age of Dinosaurs LAB 4: Systematics Part 1

The relationship between limb morphology, kinematics, and force during running: the evolution of locomotor dynamics in lizardsbij_

reproductive life History and the effects of sex and season on morphology in CRoTALus oreganus (northern PaCifiC RATTLESNAKES)

Question Set 1: Animal EVOLUTIONARY BIODIVERSITY

B-Division Herpetology Test. By: Brooke Diamond

Pilot study to identify risk factors for coprophagic behaviour in dogs

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

Prof. Neil. J.L. Heideman

EFFECTS OF BODY SIZE AND SLOPE ON ACCELERATION OF A LIZARD {STELLJO STELLIO)

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Revell et al., Supplementary Appendices 1. These are electronic supplementary appendices to: Revell, L. J., M. A. Johnson, J. A.

Care For Us Re#culated Python (Python re/culatus)

Biol 160: Lab 7. Modeling Evolution

A record of a first year dark plumage Augur Buzzard moulting into normal plumage.

Bio 1B Lecture Outline (please print and bring along) Fall, 2006

Animal Adaptations. Structure and Function

Global comparisons of beta diversity among mammals, birds, reptiles, and amphibians across spatial scales and taxonomic ranks

Point of Care Diagnostics: the Client vs. Veterinary Perspective Andrew J Rosenfeld, DVM ABVP

Gulf and Caribbean Research

LIZARD ECOLOGY. D ONALD B. MILES is Professor in the Department of Biological Sciences at Ohio University.

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

Transcription:

Functional Ecology 2016 doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12700 Movement correlates of lizards dorsal pigmentation patterns Topaz Halperin 1,2, Liran Carmel 3 and Dror Hawlena*,1,2 1 Risk-Management Ecology Lab, Department of Ecology, Evolution & Behavior, The Alexander Silberman Institute of Life Sciences, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Edmond J. Safra Campus, Jerusalem 91904, Israel; 2 Herpetological Collection, National Natural History Collections, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Edmond J. Safra Campus, Jerusalem 91904, Israel; and 3 Department of Genetics, Faculty of Science, The Alexander Silberman Institute of Life Sciences, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Edmond J. Safra Campus, Givat Ram, Jerusalem 91904, Israel Summary 1. Understanding the ecological function of an animal s pigmentation pattern is an intriguing research challenge. We used quantitative information on lizard foraging behaviour to search for movement correlates of patterns across taxa. 2. We hypothesized that noticeable longitudinal stripes that enhance escape by motion dazzle are advantageous for mobile foragers that are highly detectable against the stationary background. Cryptic pigmentation patterns are beneficial for less-mobile foragers that rely on camouflage to reduce predation. 3. Using an extensive literature survey and phylogenetically controlled analyses, we found that striped lizards were substantially more mobile than lizards with cryptic patterns. The percentage of time spent moving was the major behavioural index responsible for this difference. 4. We provide empirical support for the hypothesized association between lizard dorsal pigmentation patterns and foraging behaviour. Our simple yet comprehensive explanation may be relevant to many other taxa that present variation in body pigmentation patterns. Key-words: antipredator behaviour, camouflage, cryptic coloration, disruptive patterns, foraging behaviour, longitudinal stripes, motion dazzle, movement detection, pigmentation patterns, predation Introduction Animal body pigmentation patterns are remarkably diverse and include longitudinal stripes, reticulation, spots, cross bars and blotches. In spite of numerous attempts to explain this diversity (e.g. Cott 1940; Jackson, Iii & Campbell 1976; Caro 2009), the ecological function of pigmentation patterns remains an intriguing research challenge (Kelley & Kelley 2014). We took advantage of the recent upsurge in published information about lizard foraging behaviour to search for movement correlates of pigmentation patterns across taxa. It has been suggested that dorsal pigmentation patterns interact with movement behaviour to reduce animal vulnerability to predation (Jackson, Iii & Campbell 1976; Stevens et al. 2011). Cryptic body patterns (sensu Stevens & Merilaita 2009) that blend with the natural background (e.g. reticulation, spots, blotches, uniform) may benefit stationary prey by reducing the probability of detection by *Correspondence author. E-mail: dror.hawlena@mail.huji.ac.il predators. Yet, these patterns are no longer advantageous when the prey moves (Cott 1940; Thayer 1909). Motionsensitive visual circuits of a stationary predator can instantly detect movement against a stationary background, regardless of the prey s dorsal pattern (Stevens et al. 2011; Hall et al. 2013). Conversely, longitudinal stripes that can be considered conspicuous in many environments may be beneficial when the prey moves (e.g. Jackson, Iii & Campbell 1976; Brodie 1992; Allen et al. 2013). This is because longitudinal stripes may deceive the predator s motion perception creating a motion dazzle that hampers its ability to intercept a moving prey (Jackson, Iii & Campbell 1976; Brodie 1992; Allen et al. 2013; Kelley & Kelley 2014; Rojas, Devillechabrolle & Endler 2014; H am al ainen et al. 2015; but see Hughes, Magor-Elliott & Stevens 2015; von Helversen, Schooler & Czienskowski 2013). Consequently, low mobility species are predicted to have cryptic patterns that decrease their probability of being detected, and highly detectable mobile species should have longitudinal striped pattern to increase their chances of 2016 The Authors. Functional Ecology 2016 British Ecological Society

2 T. Halperin, L. Carmel & D. Hawlena escaping inevitable predator attacks. We further predicted that an intermediately active prey should benefit from mixed patterns that include longitudinal stripes and motifs of cryptic patterns. Mixed patterns might be less detectable than stripes when stationary and more efficient in generating motion dazzle than cryptic patterns when the prey moves. Several studies have found association between pigmentation patterns and movement behaviour (Lizards: Carretero & Vasconcelos 2006; Hawlena et al. 2006; Hawlena 2009; Snakes: Brodie 1992; Frogs: Rojas, Devillechabrolle & Endler 2014; but see Creer 2005; Ortega, Lopez & Martın 2014); yet only few studies have used a comparative approach to search for this association across species. Jackson, Iii & Campbell (1976) found an association between defence behaviour and dorsal patterns, using qualitative scoring of snake escape behaviour that was based on expert opinions. Allen et al. (2013) essentially used a similar qualitative approach but applied modern statistical techniques to account for phylogenetic non-independence. They too found an association between aspects of defence and foraging behaviours and the snake s pigmentation patterns. Using broad behavioural categories that are based on expert opinions is unavoidable when rigorous quantitative data do not exist; yet this approach impairs detailed hypothesis testing. For example, it requires splitting continuous variables (e.g. time spent moving or sprint speed) into subjective groups and prevents exploration of complex behavioural interdependencies using coarse behavioural approximations. Fortunately, since the classic study of Jackson, Iii & Campbell (1976), detailed quantitative measurements of lizard movement behaviour have become routine (Perry 2007). This wealth of quantitative behavioral data provides an excellent opportunity to advance our understanding of the possible links between mobility and pigmentation patterns. Lizards spend much of their active time foraging. Thus, movement characteristics of lizard foraging behaviour may determine their detectability to predators. Lizard foraging behaviour is commonly characterized by two continuous indices: the percentage of the foraging time spent moving (PTM) and the number of movements per minute (MPM). Using both indices complementarily produces a quantitative depiction of the forager s movement patterns (Butler 2005). Lizards with high PTM and low MPM move continuously in search for food ( widely foragers ), lizards with low PTM and low MPM are sit-and-wait forgers and lizards with moderate PTM and relatively high MPM move frequently between ambush sites ( stop-and-go ) or combine elements of the two distinct foraging strategies. We assume that high PTM and high MPM make lizards more detectable by visual predators (Fig. 1). Other movement correlates of lizard foraging behaviour for which quantitative data is readily available are foraging speed and maximal sprint speed. Cooper (2007) has suggested that widely foraging lizards continually search Fig. 1. Hypothetical relationship between the foraging behaviours depicted by PTM and MPM, and dorsal pigmentation patterns. At low PTM and MPM values, the probability of detection by predators is relatively low, making cryptic patterns that improve camouflage a preferred strategy. At high PTM and MPM values, the probability of detection is high due to movement that exposes location, making striped patterns which cause motion dazzle a preferred strategy. At intermediate PTM and MPM values, a mixed pattern may be the most useful. Note that the expected MPM values on the MPM-PTM two-dimensional space are bounded by a triangle. This is because lizards with low or high PTM values cannot have many movement or stop bouts, respectively. for prey using a slower foraging speed to maximize searching efficiency. Ambush foragers, however, must use bursts of high speed to intercept moving prey. Huey et al. (1984) found that maximal sprint speed is associated with foraging behaviour in a few Kalahari lizards. Later, a more comprehensive study suggested that these relationships are more complicated, probably because maximal sprint speed is also influenced by the lizard s escape strategy (Miles, Losos & Irschick 2007). We meticulously collected published information on lizard dorsal patterns, body length, foraging indices, foraging speed and maximal sprint speed. Using phylogenetically controlled analyses, we tested the following predictions (Fig. 1): (i) striped lizards have high PTM, relatively high MPM and slow foraging speed; (ii) lizards with cryptic patterns have low PTM, low MPM and high foraging speed; (iii) lizards with mixed patterns of stripes and cryptic motifs have intermediate PTM, MPM and foraging speed. In addition, we explored the association between pigmentation and maximal sprint speed with no a priori predictions. Materials and methods DATA COLLECTION AND CATEGORIZATION We conducted an extensive literature survey to collect numerical data regarding lizard movement indices including: PTM, MPM, foraging speed and maximal sprint speed. Foraging speed, also known as moving speed, is the speed measured while lizards forage in the field (Cooper 2007). Therefore, this index is clearly indicative of lizard foraging behaviour. Unfortunately, only a small number of studies have reported foraging speed in lizards.

Ecological function of pigmentation patterns 3 Maximal sprint speed (i.e. the highest speed recorded during escape bursts in the laboratory) is a more common movement index in lizards; yet its validity as a foraging index requires further confirmation (Miles, Losos & Irschick 2007). To test whether maximal sprint speed can be used as a foraging index, we explored the association between the two speed indices. We used the average PTM, MPM, foraging speed and maximal sprint speed as quantitative indices of lizard movement behaviour. If more than one study examined the same species, we used the results that were based on a bigger sample size and longer focal observations. When index averages were not reported in the original study, we used the value of a single measurement or the reported maximal values. We assumed that all measurements related to adult individuals. Data explicitly related to juveniles were excluded from our data set. We assigned a pigmentation pattern and a numerical value of snout-vent length (SVL) to each species using published descriptions. We collected this information from scientific books, field guides and scientific papers. When this information was not available, we described the dorsal pigmentation ourselves based on multiple pictures from books or papers. We defined the dorsal pigmentation pattern as the lizard s mid-dorsal markings (markings of the flanks and tail were not recorded). All patterns were divided into three major categories: stripes, cryptic and mixed (Fig. 2). The striped category includes only patterns of plain longitudinal stripes; the cryptic category includes all unstriped patterns such as uniform, reticulation, patches, spots, dots and cross bars; the mixed category includes patterns of longitudinal stripes with cryptic motifs in between. Basing our pattern categorization mostly on existing expert descriptions is advantageous since it minimizes identification mistakes and avoids using non-representative morphs as our references. This conservative approach is credible for our specific purposes because of the coarse classification that we used, in that we have assigned lizards to pattern categories based on whether or not they have longitudinal stripes and whether the longitudinal striped pattern is pure or includes elements of cryptic patterns. We excluded information from Gekkonidae and Chamaeleonidae species due to their nocturnal activity and ability to change body patterning, respectively. We also excluded from all analyses 61 species that exhibit polymorphism or sexual dimorphism, and 6 species for which pattern information was not available. After removing those species, our data included 219 species from 17 families. During data exploration, we noticed large variations in the quantification of behavioural indices. Different researchers used different measurement techniques and various sample sizes, took measurements at different times of the day or at different seasons and had various measurement durations. Such inconsistencies added noise to our analyses (Butler 2005; Perry 2007) and turned our results to a conservative depiction of the associations between foraging behaviour and pigmentation patterns. STATISTICAL ANALYSIS Our general approach was to first explore whether lizards with different pigmentation patterns differ in their overall foraging behaviour as described by the combination of all quantitative variables. Following that, we searched for a functional explanation by testing our predictions using smaller numbers of explanatory variables each time. To correct for phylogenetic nonindependence, we repeated the analyses that revealed significant differences using phylogenetically controlled tests (Felsenstein 1985). In spite of large efforts to assign the four movement indices to every species on the list, there were many missing values. Thus, as the number of variables included in an analysis increased, the effective sample size substantially decreased. We failed to determine the exact phylogenetic position of some of the species for which behavioural information was available. This difficulty led to even smaller sample sizes in the phylogenetic analyses. Consequently, we conducted each analysis on a different subset of species that included the largest sample size possible. The first step was to establish the validity of maximal sprint speed as a foraging index by testing its correlation to foraging speed using Pearson correlation tests. We then performed principal component analysis (PCA) to test whether lizard pigmentation-pattern categories were discernible based on the movement variables PTM, MPM and foraging speed, and we tested for significant differences between categories using MANOVA. We tested for differences between pigmentation-pattern categories based on single movement indices (PTM or MPM) using ANOVA along with Tukey HSD post hoc tests. We used ANCOVA to correct for the Fig. 2. Body pigmentation-pattern categories. (a) stripes; (b) cryptic: uniform, dots, spots, patches, reticulation and crossbars; (c) mixed. The individual patterns within each category depict a large variation of patterns.

4 T. Halperin, L. Carmel & D. Hawlena lizard s SVL when testing for differences based on foraging speed and maximal sprint speed. Tukey HSD post hoc tests for ANCOVA were conducted using the R package multcomp (Harmon et al. 2008). We used phylogenetically corrected analyses to test whether the significant differences between groups hold after precluding the effect of phylogenetic relatedness among species (Garland Jr. et al. 1993). Phylogenetic ANOVA and MANOVA tests were conducted using the function aov.phylo in the Geiger package in R (Harmon et al. 2008). We added slight modifications to this function to conduct phylogenetic ANCOVA. Phylogenetic Hochberg post hoc tests were conducted using the function phylanova in the package phytools in R (Revell 2012). We assumed a Brownian motion model of evolution. All simulations included branch length information. The scaled phylogenetic tree used in the simulations of the phylogenetic corrected analyses was adopted from Pyron, Burbrink & Wiens (2013). This generally well-supported phylogenetic estimate for squamates (i.e. 70% of nodes have Shimodaira Hasegawa-like values >85) is based on molecular data and included up to seven nuclear genes and five mitochondrial genes. We altered the tree according to the sample of each analysis so it would contain only the examined species, and the species not included in our data base were removed from the tree. Results Sprint speeds were about 10-fold faster than foraging speeds (sprint speeds: 236 m/s 017; foraging speeds: 024 m/s 003, Fig. S1). We found no correlation between foraging and sprint speeds (Pearson correlation test: r = 018, P = 029, n = 37). We therefore did not consider it a valid index for foraging behaviour. PCA revealed that lizards with striped and cryptic patterns are discernible based on their overall foraging behaviour (Fig. 3, n = 47). Interestingly, lizards with mixed patterns mostly fell inside the two other pattern groups. Ameiva exsul was the only outlier, being the sole cryptic-patterned species that fell inside the striped-pattern cluster. The separation between the cryptic and the striped patterns was mostly captured by the first principal component, which accounts for 627% of the total variability, and has a major loading contribution from PTM (Table 1). PTM Table 1. Loading of the first two principal components SD (MPM, movement per minutes; PTM, percentage time spent moving; FS, foraging speed; SVL, snout-vent length) MPM PTM FS SVL PC1 048 007 077 005 043 009 002 007 PC2 017 043 033 027 083 009 044 017 and MPM were positively associated with PC1, whereas foraging speed was negatively associated with it. Thus, species with cryptic patterns are less active and use faster foraging speed than species with stripes. While MPM and foraging speed also have significant contributions to this principal component, the contribution of SVL to the separation of the pigmentation patterns is negligible (Table 1). MANOVA revealed that foraging behaviour varied between species with different dorsal patterns (F = 68 d.f. = 2, P < 0001, n=49; PTM: F = 4595, P < 0001; MPM: F = 1316, P < 0001; foraging speed: F = 29, P = 006). The result held after controlling for phylogenetic non-independence (phylogenetic MANOVA: P < 0001, n = 47; PTM P < 0001; MPM P = 0015; foraging speed P = 046). The lizard foraging mode on its own, that is the measured PTM and MPM values, differed according to the dorsal patterns (Fig. 4) (F = 1090, d.f. = 2, P < 0001, n = 115; PTM: F=2779, P < 0001; MPM: F = 373, P = 003). When tested separately, both PTM and MPM varied between pigmentation patterns (PTM: F = 2818, d.f. = 2, P < 0001, n = 123; MPM: F = 321, d.f. = 2, P = 004, n = 123). Tukey post hoc comparisons indicated that species with cryptic patterns had lower PTM than species with striped and mixed patterns (P < 0001 in both cases), and lower MPM only than striped species (P = 004). After correcting for the phylogenetic non-independence, MPM was no longer different between lizard dorsal patterns, but the rest of the results remained the same (Table 2). Snout-vent length-adjusted foraging speed differed according to dorsal patterns (Fig. 5a) (F = 32, d.f. = 2, Fig. 3. PCA using PTM, MPM, foraging speed and SVL as lizard-characterizing features. Yellow (squares) indicates striped patterned species, blue (circles) cryptic-patterned species and green (triangles) mixed-patterned species. The outlying cryptic species is Ameiva exsul. Fig. 4. Comparison of mean foraging mode associated with each dorsal pattern category (Mean SE). n-stripes = 13; n-mixed = 21; n-cryptic = 81.

Ecological function of pigmentation patterns 5 Table 2. Significance levels and sample sizes of phylogenetic ANOVA and MANOVA tests for the association between foraging mode and dorsal pigmentation patterns. Bolded P-values indicate significance and consistencies with the parallel standard ANOVA or MANOVA test Foraging variable/s P = 005, n = 50). This result did not hold after correcting for phylogenetic non-independence (phylogenetic ANCOVA: P = 012, n = 48). SVL-adjusted sprint speed did not differ between pigmentation patterns (Fig. 5b) (F = 09, d.f. = 2, P = 04, n = 171; phylogenetic ANCOVA: P = 076, n = 150). Discussion Pattern categories Test P value n PTM and All categories Phylogenetic MPM MANOVA PTM All categories Phylogenetic ANOVA Cryptic Stripes Phylogenetic Hochberg t-test Cryptic Mixed Phylogenetic Hochberg t-test MPM all categories Phylogenetic ANOVA <0001 108 <0001 113 <0001 002 034 115 Our findings provide the first quantitative support to the hypothesized relationships between pigmentation patterns and movement aspects of lizard foraging behaviour. Using phylogenetic corrected comparative analyses, we have shown that lizards with longitudinal stripes have a very different foraging behaviour than species with cryptic patterns. As predicted, striped lizards were substantially more mobile than lizards with cryptic patterns. This difference was largely driven by variation in the percentage of the foraging time the lizards spent moving. Mixed-patterned lizards were divided between the two behavioural strategies and did not present an intermediate behaviour. It has been suggested that pigmentation patterns are associated with different behaviours (Snakes: Jackson, Iii & Campbell 1976; Mammals: Ortolani 1999; Fish: Seehausen, Mayhew & Van Alphen 1999; Price et al. 2008; Fig. 5. Comparison of SVL-adjusted mean speeds associated with each dorsal pattern category (Mean SE). (a) Mean foraging speed. n-stripes = 7; n-mixed = 9; n-cryptic = 34;. (b) Mean sprint speed, n-stripes = 20; n-mixed = 21; n-cryptic = 130. Ruell et al. 2013; Cephalopods: Zylinski, Osorio & Shohet 2009; Lizards: Hawlena et al. 2006; Frogs: Rojas, Devillechabrolle & Endler 2014). For example, Allen et al. (2013) found an association between various snake patterns and individual aspects of either foraging or antipredator behaviours. Indeed, pigmentation patterns may influence the effectiveness of foraging and defence behaviours in different ways. However, those different outcomes should be considered jointly to account for the overall influence on fitness. Moreover, foraging and defence behaviours are expected to be tightly intertwined due to morphological and physiological trade-offs. Our hypothesis overcomes these apparent difficulties by focusing on the ways in which pigmentation patterns and foraging behaviour interact to affect the probabilities to be detected and to survive predator attacks. Lizards spend much of their active time foraging. Thus, the integrative expression of their foraging behaviour and pattern should determine their detectability to visual predators. We hypothesized that lizards that spend much of their foraging time moving (i.e. high PTM) are highly detectable regardless of their pattern (Edmunds 1974; Hall et al. 2013). These highly visible lizards may benefit from having conspicuous stripes that may enhance their chances to survive inevitable predator attacks by means of motion dazzle (Jackson 1979; Carretero & Vasconcelos 2006; Kelley & Kelley 2014; H am al ainen et al. 2015). PTM is traditionally regarded as the main index for distinguishing foraging strategies (i.e. active vs. ambush foragers, Whiting & Cooper 1999; Miles, Losos & Irschick 2007). Indeed, PTM was found to be the major behavioural index that differs between striped and cryptic lizards, with striped lizards having substantially higher PTM than cryptic lizards. The complementary foraging index, MPM, represents the frequency of discrete movement bouts (Perry et al. 1990). The lizard movement incidence is expected to be positively associated with detectability. We found that lizards with cryptic patterns had lower MPM than lizards with striped patterns, but this effect was not independent of phylogenetic relatedness. MPM, when considered alone, cannot capture the full extent of variation in foraging behaviours. This is because species that rarely move (low PTM) and species that rarely stop (high PTM) are bound to have similar low MPM values (see Fig. 2). Thus, the association between pigmentation patterns and MPM must be interpreted with caution. Lizards with mixed patterns that include longitudinal stripes and motifs of cryptic patterns were not clustered separately in the PCA, but were divided between the stripes and cryptic clusters. Mixed-pattern lizards had higher PTM than lizards with cryptic patterns, but did not differ in MPM from lizards with striped or cryptic patterns. These results do not seem to reflect a genuine intermediate foraging strategy as we predicted. Instead, mixed-patterned lizards behaved either more like cryptic or striped lizards. We failed to identify phylogenetic,

6 T. Halperin, L. Carmel & D. Hawlena morphological or environmental variables that might explain this division. Possibly, our purposely crude classification ignored important nuances of mixed patterns that might bear ecological significances. For example, mixedpatterned lizards with pronounced stripes may behave more similarly to striped lizards than mixed-patterned lizards with definite cryptic elements. We predicted that species with cryptic patterns will have higher foraging speed than striped lizards. We based our predictions on the well-acknowledged notion that ambush foragers use burst attacks and hence have higher foraging speeds to intercept a moving prey (Cooper 2007). Lizards that use short bouts of high-speed movements are expected to be less detectable, favouring cryptic patterning. As expected, foraging speed substantially contributed to discriminate pattern groups in the PCA, whereas higher speeds were associated with cryptic patterns. We also found a weak association between dorsal patterns and foraging speed that met our predictions. However, this association was not independent of phylogenetic relatedness. It is important to note that the foraging speed data set is relatively small and therefore the results should be treated as such. It has been suggested that ambush foragers have high maximal sprint speed (Huey et al. 1984). This hypothesis is based on the widespread expectation that species that use burst attacks as part of their foraging strategy should have higher sprint capacities. This implies a tight association between foraging speed and maximal sprint speed. Our results have rejected this association. Moreover, we found that the realized foraging speeds were much slower than sprint capacity. These findings suggest that lizards use only a small fraction of their maximal running capacity while foraging, seriously questioning the hypothesized association between foraging behaviour and maximal sprint speed. Indeed, we found no differences in maximal sprint speed between striped, mixed and cryptic patterns. It is widely acknowledged that stripes can create motion dazzle, but the perceptual effects underlying it are still not well understood (Kelley & Kelley 2014 for potential mechanisms). The common assumption is that stripes create motion dazzle while the prey engages in high-speed escape from its predator. Support for this idea is mostly correlative (e.g. Jackson, Iii & Campbell 1976; Brodie 1992; Allen et al. 2013). Attempts to explore this assumption using computer games in which human predators target escaping objects have revealed inconclusive results (Stevens, Yule & Ruxton 2008; Scott-Samuel et al. 2011; Stevens et al. 2011; Hall et al. 2013; von Helversen, Schooler & Czienskowski 2013; Hughes, Troscianko & Stevens 2014; Hughes, Magor-Elliott & Stevens 2015). Another overlooked option is that stripes may hamper the predator s ability to assess the striped-prey form and speed. Consequently, ambush predators may miscalculate the planned point of interception, allowing the striped prey an extra split second to initiate a successful escape. Our study was not designed to tease apart these options. Yet, the strong association between active foraging behaviour and dorsal stripes should supply the impetus to expand the research into the perceptual effects of stripes in hindering and misguiding directional movements. Our comparative results coincide with focal studies that found association between pigmentation patterns and movement correlates of foraging behaviour. Rojas, Devillechabrolle & Endler (2014) found that aposematic frogs (Dendrobates tinctorius) bearing elongated patterns move on average faster and more directional while foraging than individuals with interrupted patterns. Acanthodactylus beershebensis lizards that undergo ontogenetic changes in pigmentation patterns from stripes to blotches exhibit reduction in both PTM and MPM (Hawlena et al. 2006). We urge researchers to critically test our hypothesis by comparing quantified indices of foraging behaviour between individuals or populations of polymorphic species, between sexes in sexually dimorphic species, or between ontogenetic stages in species that undergo pattern changes. Our movement-patterns hypothesis aims to explain the functional differences between longitudinal stripes and cryptic patterning. This hypothesis does not intend to explain variation among cryptic patterns. We expect environmental conditions to govern this variation. Unfortunately, in spite of an extensive literature search, we could not find comparative information about environmental variables (e.g. habitat, substrate or land cover) that was detailed enough to allow meaningful testing of this hypothesis. We emphasize that our hypothesized relationship between foraging behaviour and pigmentation patterning does not rule out the possibility that in certain environments stripes can be cryptic (Van der Winden, Strijbosch & Bogaerts 1995; Sherbrooke 2002). Our data base includes two striped lizards with relatively low PTM (Chamaesaura anguina and Podarcis peloponnesiacus). Future studies should focus on characterizing the striped lizard habitats to untangle the possibly two very different functions of this patterning. Unravelling these confounded functions may reveal the actual strength of the association between stripes and mobility in lizards. In summary, our study provides a simple yet comprehensive explanation for the association between pigmentation patterns and lizard foraging behaviour. Using quantitative behavioural data coupled with a phylogenetic comparative approach, we have shown that striped lizards were more mobile than lizards with cryptic patterns. We attribute these differences to alternative antipredatory strategies. Highly detectable mobile lizards may benefit from motion-dazzle patterns that enhance escape, while less-mobile lizards may profit from patterns that reduce detectability. This functional link between foraging behaviour and pigmentation pattern might be applicable to many other taxa and serve as a starting hypothesis for more detailed exploration. We hope that future studies will test our movement-patterns hypothesis, ultimately applying meticulous manipulations to tease apart, and test its mechanistic details.

Ecological function of pigmentation patterns 7 Acknowledgements We thank M. Mandel for his most valuable statistical advices and N. Shamir for drawing pattern illustrations in much talent. This work was supported by Gans Collections and Charitable Fund donation to D.H. Data accessibility Data for this paper can be found as supporting information. References Allen, W.L., Baddeley, R., Scott-Samuel, N.E. & Cuthill, I.C. (2013) The evolution and function of pattern diversity in snakes. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 1237 1250. Brodie, E.D. (1992) Correlational selection for color pattern and antipredator behavior in the garter snake Thamnophis ordinoides. Evolution, 46, 1284 1298. Butler, M.A. (2005) Foraging mode of the chameleon, Bradypodion pumilum: A challenge to the sit-and-wait versus active forager paradigm? Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 84, 797 808. Caro, T. (2009) Contrasting coloration in terrestrial mammals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364, 537 548. Carretero, M. & Vasconcelos, R. (2006) Escape tactics of two syntopic forms of the Lacerta perspicillata complex with different colour patterns. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 84, 1594 1603. Cooper, W.E. (2007) Foraging modes as suites of coadapted movement traits. Journal of Zoology, 272, 45 56. Cott, H. (1940) Adaptive Coloration in Animals. Methuen, London, UK. Creer, D.A. (2005) Correlations between ontogenetic change in color pattern and antipredator behavior in the racer, Coluber constrictor. Ethology, 111, 287 300. Edmunds, M. (1974) Defence in Animals: A Survey of Anti-Predator Defences. Longman, Harlow, UK. Felsenstein, J. (1985) Phylogenies and the comparative method. The American Naturalist, 125, 1 15. Garland, T. Jr, Dickerman, A.W., Janis, C.M. & Jones, J.A. (1993) Phylogenetic analysis of covariance by computer simulation. Systematic Biology, 42, 265 292. Hall, J.R., Cuthill, I.C., Baddeley, R., Shohet, A.J. & Scott-samuel, N.E. (2013) Camouflage, detection and identification of moving targets. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280, 1 7. H am al ainen, L., Valkonen, J., Mappes, J. & Rojas, B. (2015) Visual illusions in predator-prey interactions: birds find moving patterned prey harder to catch. Animal Cognition, 18, 1059 1068. Harmon, A.L., Weir, J., Brock, C., Challenger, W., Hunt, G., Fitzjohn, R. et al. (2008) GEIGER: investigating evolutionary radiations. Bioinformatics, 24, 129 131. Hawlena, D. (2009) Colorful tails fade when lizards adopt less risky behaviors. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64, 205 213. Hawlena, D., Boochnik, R., Abramsky, Z. & Bouskila, A. (2006) Blue tail and striped body: Why do lizards change their infant costume when growing up? Behavioral Ecology, 17, 889 896. von Helversen, B., Schooler, L.J. & Czienskowski, U. (2013) Are stripes beneficial? Dazzle camouflage influences perceived speed and hit rates. PLoS One, 8, e61173. Huey, R.B., Bennett, A.F., John-Alder, H. & Nagy, K.A. (1984) Locomotor capacity and foraging behaviour of Kalahari lacertid lizards. Animal Behaviour, 32, 41 50. Hughes, A.E., Magor-Elliott, R.S. & Stevens, M. (2015) The role of stripe orientation in target capture success. Frontiers in Zoology, 12, 17. Hughes, A.E., Troscianko, J. & Stevens, M. (2014) Motion dazzle and the effects of target patterning on capture success. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 14, 201. Jackson, J.F. (1979) Effects of some Ophidian tail displays on the predatory behavior of Grison (Galictis sp.). Copeia, 1979, 169 172. Jackson, J.F., Iii, W.I. & Campbell, H.W. (1976) The dorsal pigmentation pattern of snakes as an antipredator strategy: a multivariate approach. The American Naturalist, 110, 1029. Kelley, L.A. & Kelley, J.L. (2014) Animal visual illusion and confusion: the importance of a perceptual perspective. Behavioral Ecology, 25, 450 463. Miles, D.B., Losos, J.B. & Irschick, D.J. (2007) Morphology, performance and foraging mode. Lizard Ecology: The Evolutionary Consequences of Foraging Mode (eds S.M. Reilly, L.B. McBrayer & D.B. Miles), pp. 49 93. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, USA. Ortega, J., Lopez, P. & Martın, J. (2014) Conspicuous blue tails, dorsal pattern morphs and escape behaviour in hatchling Iberian wall lizards (Podarcis hispanicus). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 113,1094 1106. Ortolani, A. (1999) Spots, stripes, tail tips and dark eyes: predicting the function of carnivore colour patterns using the comparative method. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 67, 433 476. Perry, G. (2007) Movement patterns in lizards: measurement, modality, and behavioral correlates. Lizard Ecology: The Evolutionary Consequences of Foraging Mode (eds D.B. Miles, S.M. Reilly & L.B. McBrayer), pp. 13 48. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, USA. Perry, G., Lampl, I., Lerner, A., Rothenstein, D., Shani, E., Sivan, N. et al. (1990) Foraging mode in lacertid lizards: variation and correlates. Amphibia-Reptilia, 11, 373 384. Price, A.C., Weadick, C.J., Shim, J. & Rodd, H. (2008) Pigments, patterns, and fish behavior. Zebrafish, 5, 297 307. Pyron, R., Burbrink, F. & Wiens, J. (2013) A phylogeny and revised classification of Squamata, including 4161 species of lizards and snakes. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 13, 93. Revell, M.L.J. (2012) phytools: An R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 3, 217 223. Rojas, B., Devillechabrolle, J. & Endler, J.A. (2014) Paradox lost: variable colour-pattern geometry is associated with differences in movement in aposematic frogs. Biology Letters, 10, 20140193. Ruell, E.W., Handelsman, C.A., Hawkins, C.L., Sofaer, H.R., Ghalambor, C.K. & Angeloni, L. (2013) Fear, food and sexual ornamentation: plasticity of colour development in Trinidadian guppies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280, 20122019. Scott-Samuel, N.E., Baddeley, R., Palmer, C.E. & Cuthill, I.C. (2011) Dazzle camouflage affects speed perception. PLoS One, 6, 2 6. Seehausen, O., Mayhew, P.J. & Van Alphen, J.J.M. (1999) Evolution of color patterns in East African cichlid fish. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 12, 514 534. Sherbrooke, W.C. (2002) Do vertebral-line patterns in two horned lizards (Phrynosoma spp.) mimic plant-stem shadows and stem litter? Journal of Arid Environments, 50, 109 120. Stevens, M. & Merilaita, S. (2009) Defining disruptive coloration and distinguishing its functions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364, 481 488. Stevens, M., Yule, D.H. & Ruxton, G.D. (2008) Dazzle coloration and prey movement. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 275, 2639 2643. Stevens, M., Searle, W.T.L., Seymour, J.E., Marshall, K.L.A. & Ruxton, G.D. (2011) Motion dazzle and camouflage as distinct anti-predator defenses. BMC Biology, 9, 81. Thayer, G.H. (1909) Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom: An Exposition of the Laws of Disguise Through Color and Pattern: Being a Summary of Abbott H. Thayer s Discoveries. Macmillan, New York, NY, USA. Van der Winden, J., Strijbosch, H. & Bogaerts, S. (1995) Habitat related disruptive pattern distribution in the polymorphic lizard Mabuya vittata. Acta Oecologica, 16, 423 430. Whiting, M.J. & Cooper, W.E. (1999) Foraging modes in lacertid lizards from southern Africa. Amphibia-Reptilia, 20, 299 311. Zylinski, S., Osorio, D. & Shohet, A. (2009) Cuttlefish camouflage: contextdependent body pattern use during motion. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276, 3963 3969. Received 16 January 2016; accepted 3 June 2016 Handling Editor: Kevin McGraw Supporting Information Additional Supporting Information may be found online in the supporting information tab for this article: Table S1. Lizards movement behavior and dorsal pigmentation database. Appendix S1. Database references. Fig. S1. The relationship between average values of maximal sprint speed and foraging speed in different species.