Received March 25, 1963

Similar documents
The Hypostatic Genotype of the Recessive White Prat Breed of Chickens

REGRESSION IN EGG PRODUCTION IN THE DOMESTIC FOWL WHEN SELECTION IS RELAXED1

I yellow, a great assortment of shades of red and yellow being known. The

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite

INHERITANCE OF BODY WEIGHT IN DOMESTIC FOWL. Single Comb White Leghorn breeds of fowl and in their hybrids.

THE MASKING OF SEPIA BY WHITE, TWO RECESSIVE

Genetics Department, Iowa State Uniuersity, Anies, Iowa. Received September 14, 1959

LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE'

Research Into Sex Linked Control of Bodyweight in Poultry and Rabbits

F same general plumage color pattern but differ in details of color are recognized,

Today: Mendel s Technique: What Mendel Observes: Mendelian Genetics: Consider this. Mendelian Genetics and Problems (In-Class 6)

Jakaria*, Maria Ulfah, & Desha Anandya Putri

MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection

Fruit Fly Exercise 2 - Level 2

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Sex-Influenced (Autosomes) P Horned x Hornless HH H'H' H H' F 1 Horned x Hornless HH' HH' 1/2 H 1/2 H' 1/2 H 1/2 H' F 2 Genotypes Phenotypes

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Incomplete Dominance and Codominance

P to be a heritable characteristic, although in some instances the mode of

Mendelian Genetics Problem Set

HEREDITY BEYOND MENDEL INCOMPLETE DOMINANCE CODOMINANCE: Heredity Activity #3 page 1

ON COMMERCIAL poultry farms during

I the BUSSEY INSTITUTION of HARVARD UNIVERSITY, it was found that

Assorted Guinea Brown egg layers: Black Sex Link Golden Sex Link Red Sex Link

T heritance in mice begun some years ago at the Bussey Institution of

Worksheet for Morgan/Carter Laboratory #9 Mendelian Genetics II: Drosophila

Question 3 (30 points)

Genetics and Probability

Mendelian Genetics SI

A "New" Allele of the Mallard Plumage Pattern in Ducks

Summary. investigation programs. Estimation of general and specific combining abilities from a diallel cross of three inbred lines of Fayoumi chicks

BEYOND MENDEL. Incomplete Dominance: Blue (BB) Red (RR) F 1 hybrids have appearance in between 2 parents Purple (BR)

9-2 Probability and Punnett. Squares Probability and Punnett Squares. Slide 1 of 21. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Biology 201 (Genetics) Exam #1 120 points 22 September 2006

Here are some ground rules that you should ALWAYS follow when tackling an Inheritance Problem:

AS91157 Demonstrate understanding of genetic variation and change. Punnett Squares

IT HAS been well established that

C. W. Knox Iowa State College

PHYSIOLOGIC AND GENETIC STUDIES OF CROOKED KEELS IN CHICKENS

Sections 2.1. and 2.2. (Single gene inheritance, The chromosomal basis of single-gene inheritance patterns)

Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1

We are learning to analyze data to solve basic genetic problems

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below.

Studying Gene Frequencies in a Population of Domestic Cats

13. Cell division is. assortment. telophase. cytokinesis.

Mendel s Laws: Their Application to Solving Genetics Problem

Mendelian Genetics 1

Cow Exercise 1 Answer Key

Two-Factor Crosses. All of the resulting F 1 offsrping had round yellow peas (RrYy).

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING.

17 Inherited change Exam-style questions. AQA Biology

Genetics Problems. Character Dominant Recessive

Genetics Review Name: Block:

WILLIAM H. EYSTER University of Maine, Orono, Mahe. Received December 15, 1924 TABLE OF CONTENTS

The response to selection for body weight and egg weight in the fowl

Basic color/pattern genetics. Heather R Roberts 3 November 2007

THE ASSOCIATION OF SIZE DIFFERENCES WITH SEED-COAT PATTERN AND PIGMENTA- TION IN PHASEOLUS VULGARIS

Name Date Hour Table # 1i1iPunnett Squares

Station 1. Using the cards, match the vocabulary word with its definition. If there are any words you do not know, write them down if you have time!

HERITABILITY ESTIMATES OF HATCHING

Breeding Icelandic Sheepdog article for ISIC 2012 Wilma Roem

GENETICS PRACTICE 1: BASIC MENDELIAN GENETICS

Baby Face Activity. Name: Date: Per:

Genome 371; A 03 Berg/Brewer Practice Exam I; Wednesday, Oct 15, PRACTICE EXAM GENOME 371 Autumn 2003

6. Show the cross for one heterozygous short hair cat and a long haired cat. What percentage of the offspring will have short hair?

Sexing Day-Old Chicks on Small and Backyard Flocks

Genetics Practice Problems. 1. For each genotype, indicate whether it is heterozygous (HE) or homozygous (HO) AA Bb Cc Dd.

Incomplete Dominance, Co-Dominance, and Sex-linked dominance NON-MENDELIAN GENETICS

Cross Application Problems

Polymorphism of egg white proteins

Inheritance of coat and colour in the Griffon Bruxellois dog

PLUMAGE COLOR ALLELISM IN THE RED JUNGLE FOWL (GALLUS GALLUS) AND RELATED DOMESTIC FORMS

Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD

B- indicates dominant phenotype

Name Period G eni G ames Worksheet Packet 1

Student Exploration: Mouse Genetics (One Trait)

Genetics & Punnett Square Notes

Bio 111 Study Guide Chapter 14 Genetics

Heredity and Genetics Notes- Enriched

Mendelian Inheritance Practice Problems

Inheritance of qualitative traits in two populations of Nigerian local chicken ecotypes.

SEX LINKAGE AND AUTOSEXING IN WATERFOWL. CONTENTS Page. The principles of sex-linkage Sex-linkage in the common duck... 3

NON MENDELIAN INHERITANCE PART III

Questions from last week. You have a mouse with red eyes and a mouse with blue eyes. How could you determine which is the dominant trait?

Genetics #2. Polyallelic Traits. Genetics can be very complicated.

EVOLUTIONARY GENETICS (Genome 453) Midterm Exam Name KEY

Layer/Egg Breeds. This presentation is sponsored by the Beginning Farmer and Rancher Development Program

Evolution and Gene Frequencies: A Game of Survival and Reproductive Success

Biology 100. ALE #8. Mendelian Genetics and Inheritance Practice Problems

THE INFLUENCE OF SOME FACTORS ON THE HATCHABILITY OF THE HEN S EGG

Page 2. Explain what is meant by codominant alleles (1) Male cats with a tortoiseshell phenotype do not usually occur. Explain why. ...

RELATIONSHIPS AMONG WEIGHTS AND CALVING PERFORMANCE OF HEIFERS IN A HERD OF UNSELECTED CATTLE

Some Problems Concerning the Development of a Poultry Meat Industry in Australia

Summary. Plymouth Rock (PP), Light Sussex (SS) and their recriprocal Crosses. Sixteen

Genetics Intervention

Biology 164 Laboratory

THE INTERACTIONS BETWEEN GENOTYPES AND HOUSING ENVIRONMENTS IN THE DOMESTIC HEN

What is Genetics? Genetics is the scientific study of heredity

Transcription:

STUDIES ON DOMINANCE AND PLEIOTROPY USING SEGREGATING INBRED LINES OF FOWL1 L. T. SMITH2 and A. W. NORDSKOG Poultry Science Department, Iowa Siate Uniuersity, Ames, Iowa Received March 25, 1963 R. A. FISHER (1930) developed a phylogenetic and statistical theory of dominance of wild type over mutants involving a selection of modifiers that shift the phenotype of the heterozygous mutant toward the wild type. The wild type has a selective advantage which also is conferred upon heterozygotes. FISHER (1934,1935,1938) noted that many common mutations in the domestic fowl were dominant in contrast to those of Drosophila. The fowl, therefore, provided material to test a crucial point in the dominance theory. If domestic dominants had evolved through artificial selection during the process of domestication, this could be demonstrated by showing that, in wild stocks not previously subjected to artificial selection, dominance is incomplete; i.e., the heterozygote is intermediate between two homozygous types. FISHER S experiment consisted of introducing several supposedly dominant genes into a stock of wild jungle fowl. This was done by backcrossing the heterozygote five successive generations to the jungle fowl stock followed by interbreeding the heterozygotes. Thus, all three genotypes were obtained in a relatively pure jungle-fowl genetic background. Dominance was found to be incomplete in the case of three mutations influencing pigmentation. These were, Barring (B), pile (dominant white) (I), and black internal pigment. In fact, barring appeared to be more nearly recessive than dominant. Later, FISHER (1949) in his book, Theory of Inbreeding, suggested that segregating inbred lines might be useful in studies of dominance and linkage and that What is needed in practice is the gene and at least one of its allelomorphs-segregating together in a uniform genetic background, and this is supplied by a segregating inbred line.-the time consumed in preparing the genetic material for making an adequate comparison is often considerable and it is a great advantage to possess stocks of prepared material available for future use. DUNN S (1942) investigation of two reduced tail-length mutations (T) and (Sd) in the mouse is a notable example of segregating inbred lines and dominance 1 Journal paper No. 5-4557 of the Iowa Agricultural and Home Economics Experiment Station, Amzs, Iowa. Project No. 1039, in cooperation with the North Central Regional Poultry Breeding Praject NC-47. This is part of a thesis submitted by the senior author to the Graduate School of Iowa State University in partial fulfillment of the requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Present address: Poultry Science Department, Kansas State University, Manhattan, Kansas. Genetics 48: 1141-1152 September 1963

1142 L. T. SMITH AND A. W. NORDSKOG modifiers. One stock which had modifiers increasing tail length for T also had modifiers which decreased tail-length for Sd. Thus, the same inbred line contained genes modifying two similar phenotypes in opposite directions. The modifiers appeared to be specific for a particular mutation rather than a general phenotype. The objectives of the present study are: (1) to examine the dominance relationships in different genetic backgrounds of three mutant genes of the fowl; and (2) to measure the effects of the genes on body weight, egg production and egg weight. MATERIALS AND METHODS A. Genes introduced: The present study is concerned with three recognized single gene effects in the domestic fowl: dominant white, generally thought to produce the white plumage color common to White Leghorns (given the symbol Z-Inhibitor of black pigment) ; Rose comb, determined by a dominant gene R (characterized by a low flat head appendage covered with smooth even papillae) ; and sex-linked Barring (B), which restricts the deposition of melanin to bars in the feathers that would otherwise be a solid color. The gene symbolism used is that suggested by JAAP and HOLLANDER (1954). Genes of the standard wild type are designated with a L +77 superscript. B. General mating procedure: Segregating inbred lines were developed by first outcrossing an already established inbred line to bring in the desired gene and then by successively backcrossing the heterozygote to the inbred. A segregating inbred line is defined as a line heterozygous for a specific single-locus character. These lines are symbolized as follows: A x B Where A is the parent inbred line backcrossed n generations after outcrossing to line B to introduce the desired gene. Populations including the three genotypes are established by inter se matings of the segregating inbred lines. These populations were then performance tested. C. Genetic stocks: Line Breed Date of origin L-9 White Leghorn 1939 I? I1 L-19 1944 If L-BA 1954 /r L-HN 1954 L-SP Spanish (Castellana Negra) 1954 L-SP (barred) 1956 L-WG White Wyandott 1953 Estimated inbreeding coefficient 93 % 68 % 40 % 40 % 25-30 % 2 7-42 % unknown Since 1956 L-9 and L-19 have been maintained by flock matings. L-SP is a black-plumaged Spanish breed closely resembling the Black Minorca and was obtained from the University of Minnesota in 1954. They have since been maintained as a closed flock.

DOMINANCE AND PLEIOTROPY 1143 Seven segregating lines were formed by the backcrossing procedure already described. These are presented in Table 1. Of the seven lines, four segregated for I, three for R, and one for B. D. Statistical methods: The quantkative traits measured on female test populations were body weight at three ages, egg production in two periods, and egg weight. Egg production was measured in Period 1 (September 6 to December 28) and in Period 2 (February 14 to April 19). The quantitative data were analyzed by the method of least squares based on the mathematical model: y = p +si + gij + h k + ezjkm, where y = performance of an individual bird, p =.general population mean, si = effect of the ith line, gij = effect of the jth genotype within the ith line, hk = effect of the kth hatch, e;jkm. = deviation of the mth bird from the mean of the ijth genotype of the kth hatch. A within-line preliminary analysis indicated that the hatch x genotype interaction was negligible, and therefore was not included in the model. Also the hatch x line effect was assumed to be negligible. Genotypes were analyzed on a within-line basis because of possible allelic differences between the introduced genes. Thus the genotype X line interaction is included in the genotype/line parameters and mean squares. However, preliminary inspection of the data did not indicate any genotype x line interaction. The analysis of variance with expectations of the mean squares is: Source Degrees of freedom Expected mean squares Hatches h-i U: + k, U: Lines s-1 U: + k, U: Genotypes/Lines t-i + ki g:,s Error N-h-S-2 0," where h = the number of hatches, s = the number of lines, t = the genotype within line subclasses, and N = total number of individuals. TABLE 1 Segregating lines formed Segregation lines 94 x SP BA4 x SP SP4 x 9 SP4 x BA Genes segregating I/i+ I/i' I/i+ I/i+ Source I from L-9; ' i from L-SP I from L-BA; i' from L-SP I from L-9; i+ from L-SP I from L-BA; i' from L-SP Year test population formed 1959 1959 1957f19.59 1957+1959 Genetic background L-9 L-BA L-SP L-SP 94 x WG 1g4 x WG HN4 x WG SP-barred R/r+ R/r+ R/r+ B/b+ R from L-WG; r+ from L-9 R from L-WG; r+ from L-19 R from L-WG; r+ from L-HN B from L-SP; b' from L-SP 1959 1959 1959 1959 L-9 L-19 L-HN L-SP

~ 1144 L. T. SMITH AND A. W. NORDSKOG A reduced mathematical model was used in the case of sex-linked barring; since only one line was involved, no "line effect" could be estimated. In those cases where the genotype within-line mean squares were statistically significant, the corresponding parameter estimates were tested by the t-test. Adjustment of datu:' The necessary progeny testing to identify genotypes was conducted during the first egg production test period. Rose comb females and all Leghorn dominant white line females were mated to a recessive tester. The minimum number of phenotypically-alike test progeny sucessively produced was arbitrarily set at seven to classify a genotype as a homozygote. The maximum probability of incorrectly classifying a heterozygous individual as a homozygous is i/z or 0.0078. This procedure introduced a bias into the Period 1 egg production results because it favored the definitive classification of the better egg-producing birds. Thus, the distribution of egg production records of individuals classified as homozygotes was truncated with a deficiency of individuals in the lower production levels. Special formulae (SMITH 1962) were developed to correct for this bias by estimating the probable number of individuals with misclassified genotypes from information on all individuals producing at least one test progeny. Least squares analyses were then conducted on the estimated populations. RESULTS A. Dominant white ( I) : The number of breeders in 1959 producing the segregating test populations is presented in Table 2 and the number of pullets in the 1960 segregated test populations is presented in Table 3. Dominance: A difference in dominance expression was noted between heterozygotes originating from the Leghorn and Spanish parental lines. The heterozygotes with the Spanish background became more pigmented with successive backcrosses. The three genotypes from the inter se mutings, Z/Z, Z/i+ and i+/i+, were respectively pure white, black-flecked and black. In the Spanish background, Z is, therefore, incompletely dominant. Heterozygotes of the first backcross to the Leghorn lines showed slight black TABLE 2 Number of female breeders in the heterozygous lines used in 1959 Phenulypes and genotypes Number of ~~ Lines female5 Rlales Fnnales 94 x SP 37 white I/i' white I/i+ BA4 x SP 25 white I/i+ white I/i' SP4 x 9 82 black flecked I/i+ 39 black flecked I/i' 43 black i+/i+ SP4 x BA 63 black flecked I/i+ 33 black flecked I/i+ 30 black i+/i+

DOMINANCE AND PLEIOTROPY 1145 TABLE 3 SPprqatd fentnlr tpst populations in ly60 for I flecking in the plumage. but those of the second and third backcross showed no black flecking and could be identified only by progeny testing. In Line 9, inter se matings of heterozygotes produced 21 6 white and six colored individuals. Progeny testing demonstrated that 36 pure white individuals were recessive i+/i- but further testing proved these to be recessive white, c/c. However. even with the I;I(;I:III< 1, -Iii(lividiiiil\ 4ion itig cottiplvtv cloiiiitiiiriw of / iii I.rghor~i gcwvtic. I~i~ckgrouti~l iititl inconiplrtr tlontiriii,icr of / in Spiiriish griirtir I)iirkgrniind. Upprr Idt: ///-I.rgIiorn gcwctic h ~ k - ground (Line 98 x SI ). Upprr right: //i--i,rgltnrn grnrtic 1)nrkgrnund (Ihr 91 x SI ). IAwrr left: ///-Spanish grnrtir hnrkgmund (Ihr SPI x 9). Imvrr right: I/i+ -Spanish grnrtic backgrountl (Linr SPI x 9).

1146 L. T. SMITH AATD A. W. NORDSKOG high gene frequency estimate of c (.93), no black-flecked birds were noted, SO that I in the Leghorn Line 9 was considered completely dominant. Segregating matings in Leghorn Line BA produced 81 white and 22 colored individuals in agreement with the expected ratio of 3 to 1. The colored individuals proved to be recessive i+/i+ by progeny test. However, three pure white individuals also proved to be i+/i+, and hence were probably recessive white (c/c). Since no black-flecked individuals were noted, I was completely dominant in Leghorn Line BA. (See Figure 1.) Effect on quantitative traits: Table 4 shows the analyses of variance for body weight, egg production and egg weight in the Spanish lines segregating for I. Differences between genotypes within lines were statistically significant for body weight at all ages. Estimates of mean body weights for genotypes within lines are presented in Table 5. Differences between genotypes within lines were not statistically significant for egg production or egg weight. Significant line differences were noted for eightweek weight and both egg production periods. Line SP" x 9 laid an average of 12.4 eggs in Period 1 ; the greatest difference between genotypes was 2.4 eggs. Line SP' x BA averaged 14.4 eggs, and the greatest difference between genotypes was 2.0 eggs. Differences between genotypes in egg weight for both Spanish lines SP4 x 9 and SP' x BA were small. Average egg weight for these lines was 58.7 and 60.0 g, respectively. Only pullets with genotypes classified by progeny test were used in the analyses of data from the segregating lines of Leghorn background. The analyses of variance of the quantitative traits are presented in Table 6. Differences between genotypes within lines were not significant except for TAELE 4 Analysis of uariance of quantitatiue traits of Spanish lines segregating for I l\iean squares d.f 8 ueeks 20 weeks 36 weeks Body weights (kg) Hatches 1.142* 219'.038 Lines 1.079+.001.224 Geno types/lines 4,026',137',285-t Error 502,004.052,075 Mean squares d f. Perivd 1 Period 2 Eaa weight (ai Egg production and egg weight Hatches 11 7.491 1.288 4+.859 Lines 1 291.86O-f 481.34.0-t 53.562 Genotypes/lines 4 70.955 13.105 3.025 Error 430$ 40.369 17.670 15.289 * Signilicant at.05 lerel. -; Significant at.01 lerel. : Error degree& (if freedom for Period 2 egg production and c'gg weight are 40i and 253 respectively.

DOMINANCE AND PLEIOTROPY 1147 TABLE 5 Least squares estimates of body weight (kilograms) for the Spanish lines segregating for I Genotype Lines I/[ I/i+ i+/i+ Eight weeks SP4 x 9 SP4 X BA 20 weeks SP4 x 9 SP4 x BA 36 weeks SP4 x 9 SP4 x BA * Significantly different from [/I. -1 Signilicantly different from [/it..689.656*.690+,636.645.664*t 1.592 1.642 1.714*+ 1.606 1.610 1.646 1.728 1.869* 1.914' 1.728 1.783 1.846* TABLE 6 Analysis of uariance of the quantitative traits in the Leghorn lines segregating for I Mean sauares d f. 8 weeks 20 weeks 36 weeks Body weights (kg) Hatches 4.0165.*st,056 Lines 1.0012 3.3341 1.014.1- Genatypes/lines 4.0052,026.019 Error 232.004,017.028 Mean squares d.f. Period 1 Period 2 Em weixht (9) Egg production and egg weight Hatches 4 116.084' 1.399 81.785t Lines 1 1088.29O-f 437.858* 643.560t Genotypes/lines 4 62.598 59.962' 5.093 Error 251$ 36.463 17.897 10.934 * Significant at.05 level. + Significant at.01 level. Z Error degrees of freedom for Period 2 egg production and egg weight are 190 and 180 respectively. Period 2 egg production, which showed a significant difference of 2.2 eggs in favor of Z/Z over I/;+ in Line g4 x SP. The significant line differences are not surprising since the Leghorn inbreds were of different origin. In body weight, Line g4 x SP averaged 1.238 and 1.619 kilograms, respectively, at 20 and 36 weeks. Line BA4 x SP averaged 1.510 and 1.778 kg for the corresponding periods. The average differences between genotypes Z/Z and i+/i+ for both lines were 20.7, 63.5 and 9.1 grams in favor of ;+/if for the three weigh periods of 8,20 and 36 weeks, respectively. Egg production in Lines 9" x SP and BA4 x SP averaged 13.39 and 18.52 eggs for Period 1, respectively. Egg weight

1148 L. T. SMITH AND A. W. NORDSKOG was 48.3 g for Line g4 x SP and 52.8 g for Line BA4 x SP. No effect of I could be demonstrated on egg production and egg weight in these Leghorn lines. B. Rose comb' (R): Progeny testing was required to distinguish the homozygous and heterozygous Rose comb genotypes in the segregating populations. Rose comb females were test-mated against recessive single comb male testers (r+/r+). The number of female heterozygous breeders was 87, 23 and 31 respectively for the lines g4 x WG, 19" x WG and HN4 x WG. The classified populations for all three lines are presented in Table 7. The analysis of Period 1 egg production was corrected for bias as indicated in the METHODS section. Matings of heterozygotes (R/r+ x R/r+) yielded the expected three Rose comb to one single comb in each of the three lines. Athough Rose comb remained essentially dominant with repeated backcrossing to each of the single comb lines, in the second backcross generation, the Rose comb spike became modified to a single comb-like blade. In extreme cases, even the papillae of the Rose comb were greatly reduced. Several cross-beaked, Rose combed individuals appeared in the Rose comb segregated populations. Four cross-beak individuals reached maturity in Line 19" x WG, of which two were classified as homozygous (R/R). In Line HN4 x WG six had cross beaks, of which three were (RIR) and one of the heterozygous (RJr+). Line g4 x WG had none. FISHER (1938) in introducing Rose comb into jungle fowl stock observed that the frontal bone between the orbits was affected. This sudden incidence of crossbeaks suggests that modifiers are present in breeds normally Rose combed, which permits normal skull development. Table 8 presents the analyses of variance for body weights. egg production and egg weight. No significant differences between genotypes within lines were found for any of the traits, although differences between the three inbred lines statistically differed significantly in all three traits. C. Sex-Linked Barring (B): The sex-linked Barred Spanish line originated from a single Barred male appearing in the SP line in 1956. The segregation of dominant sex-linked Barring from a solid black phenotype was unexpected and genetically is explained only on the basis of mutation. A remote possibility exists that Barring arose by contamination from the Barred Plymouth Rock. This scems un- TABLE 7 Test populations for three inbred Leghorn lines segregating for Rose comb Genotype 94 x WG 19-1 x WG HN4 x WG 4'7 109 62 35 84 36 20 64 46

DOMINANCE AND PLEIOTROPY 1149 TABLE 8 Analysis of variance of the quaniitatiue traits measured in three inbred Leghorn lines segregating for R Mean squares d.f 8 weeks 20 weeks 36 weeks Body weights (kg) Hatches 4.048.66+.094* Lines 2.023 1.465-f 1.0411 GenotypesAines 6.002.045,054 Error 490.004,025.031 LMean squares d.f. Period 1 Period 2 Egg weight (9) Egg production and egg weight Hatches 4 68.739 29.143 97.6131 Lines 2 281.403-f 113.619-f 628.8251 Genot ypes/lines 6 62.781 11.596 12.061 Error 5212 31.151 17.330 16.737 * Significant at.@5 level. -: Significant at,131 level. 2 Error degrees of freedom for Period 2 egg production and egg weight are 401 and 407 respectively. likely because the Barred progeny all showed the size and form characteristics of the Spanish breed, including an all-white ear lobe. The Barred Spanish line was inbred with planned heterozygosis at the Barring locus in 1957 and 1958. A flock mating of 28 non-barred females and two Barred males in 1959 produced the 1960 test flock of 58 Barred and 49 non-barred pullets. Table 9 presents the analyses of variance of body weights, egg production and egg weight. A statistically significant mean square was found for Barring versus non-barring in body weight at eight weeks. The non-barred pullets were 45.5 g heavier at eight weeks, which was statistically significant. Also the non-barred group was 77.1 g heavier at 20 weeks and 18.1 gat 36 weeks, but these were below statistical significance. Nevertheless, the Barring gene appears to be a growth depressant in the Spanish genetic background. No important differences between genotypes in egg production and egg weight could be determined. DISCUSSION A single gene that governs primarily a morphological trait may influence the performance of quantitative traits if such genes are pleiotropic or if the genes for both characters are linked. Strictly speaking, the single gene aspect of this study may be questioned. Three generations of backcrossing to an inbred line still allow for such linkages to exist. In the study of dominant white (I), two levels of dominance were found. In the Spanish segregating lines, SP4 x 9 and SP4 x BA, I was incompletely dominant. All three genotypes were phenotypically distinguishable, and the amount of black

1150 L. T. SMITH AND A. W. NORDSKOG TABLE 9 Analysis of uariance of quantitatiue traits measured in the Spanish line segregating for Burring hiean squares _ ~ _ ~ _ ~~ _ d.f. H weeks 20 wcelrs 36 weeks Body weights (kg) Hatches 3,013.237-/.141 Bar us. non-bar 1,063-f.I53.012 Error 101.006,043,045 d f. Pei iod 1 Period 2 Egg weight (g) Egg production and egg weight Hatches 3 182.57w 20.728 11.324 Bar us. non-bar 1 2.869 11.813 2.763 Error 49 41.110 19.964 16.553 pigment in the heterozygotes increased with successive backcrosses to the Spanish. This can be interpreted as a loss of modifiers because complete dominance characterized Leghorn backcross lines. Some possible modifiers present in the Leghorn lines, but not in the Spanish, would be sex-linked Barring and blue plumage. Hutt (1949) reported that both sex-linked Barring and blue enhances the inhibiting action of I on black in birds heterozygous for dominant white. An alternative interpretation of the two levels of dominance found in this study could be based on the possible differences of the two sources of the gene 1. Thus, the 1 from the Line 9 might not be the same allele as that from Line BA. However, since the two sources of the I gene gave similar results, this would seem to rule out the alternative interpretation. The present results substantiate FISHER S (1935) conclusion that much of the dominance exhibited by mutant types is due to the selection of modifiers during the period of domestication. A depressing effect of dominant white ( I) on body weight was first reported by JAAF and GRIMES (1956). Since then several investigators have presented evidence of the growth depressing effect of 1 (JEROME, SLINGER, HUNTSMAN and PEPPER 1956; COLLINS and HUBBARD 1957; MERAT 1959). WILLIAMS, KRUEGER and QUI- SENBERRY (1959), however, reported an increase in body weight when i+ was replaced by I. BLACKWOOD, BOHREN and MCKEAN (1962) found no evidence that growth rate in an inbred Leghorn line was affected by the I gene either through a pleiotropic action or by interaction with E, B, or S. Simple linkage of small body size genes with I could explain the superiority of the homozygote i+/i+ in the Spanish backcross lines since the Z gene came from smaller body lines. However, in the Leghorn backcross lines the superiority of the homozygote i+/i+ was not as evident even with the source of the if allele originating from the larger body line. When I was incompletely dominant, as in the Spanish genetic background, a

DOMINANCE AND PLEIOTROPY 1151 growth depression was noted. No growth depression was found when I was completely dominant as in the Leghorn lines. However, genotype differences in the segregating Leghorn lines in favor of the if allele approached statistical significance at eight and 20 weeks of age. HAMILTON (1940) reported that melanophores in the embryos of both dominant and recessive white breeds of chickens have a much lower viability and higher sensitivity to adverse environmental conditions than melanophores of breeds having black plumage. Thus, if cell environment is influenced by the presence of I, growth depression is a possible result. In an already white or almost white breed, a suboptimal condition may already exist in the cells, and the additional effect of dominant white would not be evident. MERAT ( 1961 ) reported that non-barred females were significantly heavier in body weight than Barred females in some strains. In the present study growth depression seemed also to be associated with the Barring allele in the Spanish background. Since both B and I influence melanophore development, the observation of JAAP and GRIMES (1956) that melanophore development is related to growth rate is substantiated. These workers also reported that the interaction of I with Extension of black (E), Barring (B), and possibly Silver (S) suppressed growth which is not supported in the present study. Growth depression was found in the Spanish genetic background where only E and S were present, while no depression was found in the Leghorn background where all three genes E, B, and S were present. Thus this study supports a pleiotropic action of I and B in reducing body growth and that this expression is associated with their action on the melanophores. SUMMARY Marker genes segregating in inbred lines were used to study dominance and interactions on quantitative traits. The gene loci studied were dominant white (I), Rose comb (R) and sex-linked Barring (B). The quantitative traits were body weight, egg production and egg weight. Segregating inbred lines were produced by outcrossing an already established inbred line to bring in a desired gene and backcrossing the heterozygote in three successive generations. By this procedure each particular locus was kept in a heterozygous condition and at the same time the genetic contribution of the inbred line was increased in each successive backcross. The three genotypes for each locus were obtained by inter se matings of the heterozygotes in the fourth generation. Individuals of these genotypes were then compared on the basis of the quantitative traits. Four segregating dominant white lines were produced from Leghorn x Spanish crossbred foundation stock. The parental lines were Leghorn inbred Lines 9 and BA and one Spanish line (SP). Two of the segregating lines differed in Leghorn genetic background, and MO had the same genetic background (Spanish), but differed as to source of I genes. In the Spanish background I was incompletely dominant and all three geno-

1152 L. T. SMITH AND A. W. NORDSKOG types were phenotypically distinguishable. In each of the Leghorn backgrounds I was completely dominant? for progeny testing was required to identify the heterozygote. I significantly depressed growth in the Spanish lines, but not in the Leghorn lines. The Rose comb gene (R) was introduced into the three Leghorn inbred Lines 9, 19, and HN, from which segregating populations were obtained. In each line Rose comb remained essentially dominant after three backcrosses but the spike of the Rose comb became modified into a blade. Genotype differences at the Rose comb locus appear not to influence the quantitative traits measured. The sex-linked Barring gene (B) was also tested for its possible influence on quantitative traits in the Spanish line. The "-Barred pullets were consistently heavier in body weight than the Barred pullets. Differences in egg production and egg weight were not statistically significant. LITERATURE CITED BLACKWOOD, C. A., B. B. BOHREN. and H. E. MCKEAN, 1962 A mutation at the I locus in an inbred line of White Leghorns and its effect on growth rate. Poultry Sci. 44: 48811.92. COLLINS, W. M., and W. HURBARD. 1957 Influence of plumage color in hatching ratio and growth rate in chickens. Poultry Sci. 37: 69-77. DUNN, L. C., 1942 Changes in the degree of dominance of factors affecting tail-length in the hcuse mouse. Ani. Naturalist 76: 552-569. FISHER, R. A., 1930 The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Oxford University Press, London. 1934 Crest and hernia in fowls due t3 a single gene without dominance. Science 80: 288-289. 1935 Dominance in poultry. Phil. Trans. of the Roy. Soc. London 255: 195-226. 1938 Dominance in poultry: feathered feet. rose comb, internal pigment. Proc. Roy. SOC. London B 125: 25-48. 1949 The Theory of Inbreeding. Oliver and Boyd, Edinburgh. HAMILTON, H. L., 19480 A study of the physiological properties of melanophores with special reference to their role in feather coloration. Anat. Record 78: 525-549. HUTT, R. B., 1949 Genetics of the Fowl. McGraw-Hill Book Co., Inc., New York, N. Y. JAAP, R. G., and W. F. HOLLANDER: 1954 Wild type as standard in poultry genetics. Poultry Sci. 33: 94-100. JAAP, R. G., and F. J. GRIMES, 1956 Growth rate and plumage color in chickens. Paultry Sci. 35: 1264-1269. JEROME, F. N., S. J. SLINGER, C. M. HUNTSMAN, and W. F. PEPPER, 1956 The relationship between dominant white and growth rate of chicks. Poultry Sci. 35: 488-489. MERAT, P., 1959 [Factorial genetics and production in poultry. I11 Dominant white and growth rat-.] (in French.) Ann. Zootech 8: 49-55. 2961 Factorial genetics and production in poultry. 11. Sex-linked barring and growth rate. World's Poultry Sci. Jour. 17: 44.. SMITH, L. T., 1962 The development and use of segregating inbred lines. Ph.D. thesis. On file, Iowa State University Library, Ames, Iowa. WILLIAM, J. D., W. F. KRUEGER: and J. H. QUISENBERRY, 1959 The effect of plumage color genes on growth in broilers. Poultry Sci. 38: 1260.