Microhabitat Use by Brown Treesnakes (Boiga irregularis): Effects of Moonlight and Prey

Similar documents
Byall, C., H. M. Smith, and D. Chiszar Response of Brown Tree Snakes (Boiga

MICROHABITAT AND PREY ODOR SELECTION IN THE FORAGING PIGMY RATTLESNAKE

A journal for the publication of original scientific research in the biology and management of wild native or feral introduced vertebrates

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

Erin Maggiulli. Scientific Name (Genus species) Lepidochelys kempii. Characteristics & Traits

CHOOSING YOUR REPTILE LIGHTING AND HEATING

Social Housing and Environmental Enrichment Policy

Invasive Species Videos

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

Call of the Wild. Investigating Predator/Prey Relationships

FORAGING BEHAVIOUR OF THE BROWN TREE SNAKE, BO/GA IRREGULARIS

Reptiles Notes. Compiled by the Davidson College Herpetology Laboratory

Writing: Lesson 31. Today the students will be learning how to write more advanced middle paragraphs using a variety of elaborative techniques.

The effect of invasive plant species on the biodiversity of herpetofauna at the Cincinnati Nature Center

Journal: International Journal of Pest Management. 31 The Lizard, Wymondham, Norfolk, NR18 9BH. UK +44 (0)

The Feasibility of Controlling the Brown Treesnake in Small Plots

SOAR Research Proposal Summer How do sand boas capture prey they can t see?

Journal of Zoology. Is it better in the moonlight? Nocturnal activity of insular cottonmouth snakes increases with lunar light levels.

Ssssneaky, Pesssky, S speciesss! Objectives: Sneak Peek Aligned with the following Sunshine State Standards and FCAT Benchmarks for grades 6-8:

Native lizards on the Kapiti Coast

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

4 Many species of mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians and fish 940L. Source 1 Habitats

Pre-lab homework Lab 8: Food chains in the wild.

The GB Invasive Non-native Species Strategy. Olaf Booy GB Non-native Species Secretariat

Objectives: Outline: Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles. Characteristics of Amphibians. Types and Numbers of Amphibians

Alligators. very long tail, and a head with very powerful jaws.

Effects of prey availability and climate across a decade for a desert-dwelling, ectothermic mesopredator. R. Anderson Western Washington University

Pre-lab Homework Lab 9: Food Webs in the Wild

Reptilian Physiology

by Andrew L. Shiels, Leader, Nongame and Endangered Species Unit

Brown treesnakes: a potential invasive species for the United States

Lab 9: Inventing Life Forms

Mice alone and their biodiversity impacts: a 5-year experiment at Maungatautari

The Effect of Aerial Exposure Temperature on Balanus balanoides Feeding Behavior

Social and Thermal Cues Influence Nest-site Selection in a Nocturnal Gecko, Oedura lesueurii

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF TWO SPECIES OF EGERNIA (SCINCIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

Title of Project: Distribution of the Collared Lizard, Crotophytus collaris, in the Arkansas River Valley and Ouachita Mountains

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

Turtle Research, Education, and Conservation Program

Author's personal copy. Available online at

We are adult American. Field Marks. We are the smallest falcons in North America. Like other falcons, we have long, pointed wings,

Motuora island reptile monitoring report for common & Pacific gecko 2016

Crested Gecko GUIDE TO. Introduction. Types of Crested Gecko

Provision of egg-laying sites for captive breeding of the endangered Fijian ground frog Platymantis vitianus

Night Life Pre-Visit Packet

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Vertebrate Pest Management

Doug Scull s Science and Nature

Bio4009 : Projet de recherche/research project

B-Division Herpetology Test. By: Brooke Diamond

THEMATIC UNIT Amphibians and Reptiles

Biota of the Lehigh Gap Wildlife Refuge Reptiles and Amphibians

COMPARING BODY CONDITION ESTIMATES OF ZOO BROTHER S ISLAND TUATARA (SPHENODON GUNTHERI) TO THAT OF THE WILD, A CLINICAL CASE

Managing Uplands with Keystone Species. The Case of the Gopher tortoise (Gopherus polyphemus)

HIGLEY UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT INSTRUCTIONAL ALIGNMENT. Zoology Quarter 3. Animal Behavior (Duration 2 Weeks)

PLAGUE. Dan Salkeld. Postdoc, Lane Lab Department of Environmental Science, Policy & Management UC Berkeley

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS

Lizard Park. Ages: 9 & up. Contributor: John Wiessinger, Artist & Naturalist

Community Cats and the Ecosystem

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

AN2.3 Curriculum: Animal Growth and Change (grade 2)

North Carolina Aquariums Education Section. Prepare to Hatch. Created by the NC Aquarium at Fort Fisher Education Section

Prey and predator in the amazon rainforest

Curriculum connections: Science: grade 2 Life Science Animal Growth and Change Art: grades 1-4 Patterns, Animal Portraits

Fundamentals to be considered when choosing your reptile pet.

SECTION 3 IDENTIFYING ONTARIO S EASTERN MASSASAUGA RATTLESNAKE AND ITS LOOK-ALIKES

Institutional Animal Care & Use Program - UTEP Title: Animal Enrichment Policy#: 014 Date in Effect: 27 February 2015

NAME: DATE: SECTION:

Consequences of Extended Egg Retention in the Eastern Fence Lizard (Sceloporus undulatus)

Time of Day. Teacher Lesson Plan Nocturnal Animals Pre-Visit Lesson. Overview

Egg laying site preferences in Pterostichus melanarius Illiger (Coleoptera: Carabidae)

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

Squamates of Connecticut

Bobcat Interpretive Guide

Amphibians and Reptiles Division B

Amphibians and Reptiles in Your Woods. About Me

Iguana Technical Assistance Workshop. Presented by: Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission

Home Range, Habitat Use, Feeding Ecology and Reproductive Biology of the Cuban Boa (Chilabothrus angulifer) at Naval Station Guantánamo Bay, Cuba

SLOW DOWN, LOVE WIZARD. HERE S WHAT YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT THE HORNED LIZARD.

Northern Copperhead Updated: April 8, 2018

Lizard Surveying and Monitoring in Biodiversity Sanctuaries

Biology. Slide 1of 50. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Recording Form. Part One: Oral Reading. Excerpt is taken from pages 4 7. Running words: 214

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

November 6, Introduction

Ontogenetic changes in tail-length and the possible relation to caudal luring in northeast Kansas Copperheads, Agkistrodon contortrix

Field Herpetology Final Guide

Lab 7. Evolution Lab. Name: General Introduction:

Coyote (Canis latrans)

THE concept that reptiles have preferred

Evolution. Evolution is change in organisms over time. Evolution does not have a goal; it is often shaped by natural selection (see below).

SKELETONS: Museum of Osteology Tooth and Eye Dentification Teacher Resource

VANCOUVER ISLAND MARMOT

Newsletter May Crested Geckos and our guide to decorating your vivarium.

Unit 7: Adaptation STUDY GUIDE Name: SCORE:

About Reptiles A Guide for Children. Cathryn Sill Illustrated by John Sill

The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis)

Biol 160: Lab 7. Modeling Evolution

Transcription:

Journal of Herpetology, Vol. 42, No. 2, pp. 246 250, 2008 Copyright 2008 Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles Microhabitat Use by Brown Treesnakes (Boiga irregularis): Effects of Moonlight and Prey STEVEN R. CAMPBELL, STEPHEN P. MACKESSY, AND JENNIFER A. CLARKE 1 School of Biological Sciences, University of Northern Colorado, Greeley, Colorado 80639, USA ABSTRACT. Microhabitat selection by reptiles can be affected by a complex interplay of abiotic and biotic factors. The Brown Treesnake (Boiga irregularis) is an efficient nocturnal predator that hunts in the tropical forest canopy and on the ground, using visual and chemical cues. To identify the factors influencing Brown Treesnake microhabitat use, we experimentally manipulated an abiotic factor, moonlight level, and a biotic factor, prey presence. We hypothesized that (1) moonlight would affect microhabitat use and (2) the presence of prey would alter microhabitat use in various moonlight levels. Trials were conducted in a large laboratory chamber with artificial trees in simulated new, half, and full moonlight. In each trial, the snake s location in canopy, subcanopy, or open ground was recorded at 60-sec intervals for 100 min. Treesnake microhabitat use was determined in three moonlight levels without prey present and in two moonlight levels with a mouse (adult Mus musculus) or a Mangrove Monitor (juvenile Varanus indicus) present. The treesnakes used open ground areas more as moonlight decreased, and they used the canopy more as moonlight increased. No significant differences existed within a moonlight level between trials with or without prey. Thus, moonlight appeared to supercede prey availability in affecting Brown Treesnake microhabitat use. Additionally, the effect of nocturnal illumination on Brown Treesnake habitat use may also have important conservation implications regarding trapping techniques and deterring the snakes from specific areas. Reptiles are often faced with the conflicting demands of simultaneously avoiding predation and locating food, both of which are influenced by abiotic factors. Relatively few studies have considered both biotic and abiotic factors and their effects on reptile behaviors, such as activity in particular microhabitats. In nature, the interplay between biotic and abiotic factors affecting microhabitat use can be exceedingly complex and impossible to control. Laboratory studies can increase our understanding of the selective pressures driving daily variations in habitat use by testing each variable that may be involved. In one such study, Garden Skinks (Lampropholis guichenoti) were found to prefer open areas for foraging and basking but avoided open areas if predator (snake) scent was present (Downes, 2001). Similarly, geckos (Oedura lesuerii and Nephrurus milii) chose retreat sites with warm temperatures that lacked predation scent in laboratory tests (Downes and Shine, 1998; Shah et al., 2004). The objective of our study was to test the influences of an abiotic and a biotic factor on microhabitat selection in the Brown Treesnake (Boiga irregularis) in an experimental setting. The Brown Treesnake is a primarily nocturnal, arboreal species that uses visual and chemical cues in hunting in the tropical forest canopy and 1 Corresponding Author: E-mail: jennifer.clarke@ unco.edu also on the ground (Cogger, 1975; Fritts et al., 1987; Chiszar, 1990; Rodda and Fritts, 1992). Brown Treesnakes were accidentally introduced on Guam around 1950 and have proved to be formidable predators, contributing to the decimation of the island s bats, birds, and reptiles (Savidge, 1987; Rodda and Fritts, 1992; Fritts and Rodda, 1998). Visual census data on Guam have indicated that moonlight may affect microhabitat use in Brown Treesnakes (Rodda and Fritts, 1992). Moonlight intensity is a critical factor affecting the activity and habitat choice of various other nocturnal snake species. The Habu (Trimeresurus flavoviridis) and the Lake Tanganyika Watersnake (Lycodonomorphus bicolor) both exhibit increased activity with decreased moonlight (Yamagishi, 1974; Madsen and Osterkamp, 1982). Adult Prairie Rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis viridis) increase activity in open ground (vs. areas with cover) as moonlight intensities decrease (Clarke et al., 1996). Prey availability also appears to affect the activity of Brown Treesnakes, as demonstrated by the effectiveness of prey (mouse) movements and scent in trapping the snakes (Shivik and Clark, 1997; Lindberg et al., 2000). Habitat use by various other snake species is similarly affected by prey availability. In laboratory experiments, Western Rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis) were shown to prefer microhabitats with prey odor and the type of cover that the prey would use in nature (Theodoratus and Chiszar,

MICROHABITAT USE BY BROWN TREESNAKES 247 2000). Pigmy Rattlesnakes (Sistrurus miliarius) preferred areas with prey (frog) scent cues (Roth et al., 1999), whereas Gray Rat Snakes (Elaphe obsolete spiloides) used visual cues of birds provisioning nestlings to focus arboreal activity on specific trees with prey (Mullin and Cooper, 1998). In the field, Water Pythons (Liasis fuscus) were observed to alter their habitat use to coincide with seasonal variation in habitat use by the Dusky Rat (Rattus colletti), their primary prey species (Madsen and Shine, 1996). The first objective of our study was to test the hypothesis that moonlight levels affected microhabitat use by Brown Treesnakes. We predicted that a direct relationship would be observed between increased moonlight level and Brown Treesnakes use of canopy vegetation. Our second objective was to test the hypothesis that the presence of terrestrial prey affected microhabitat use by Brown Treesnakes. We predicted that Brown Treesnakes would be observed significantly more often in open ground areas in all moonlight levels when terrestrial prey were present, compared to similar moonlight conditions when prey were absent. MATERIALS AND METHODS Animals. All animals used in this study were housed in animal holding rooms at the University of Northern Colorado s Animal Facility, Greeley, Colorado, in accordance with UNC IACUC protocol 0205, ASIH/SSAR guidelines (for reptiles), and ASM guidelines (for rodents) for humane treatment. The adult Brown Treesnakes used in this study (N 5 5) were wildcaught on Guam (U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service permit MA022452-0). Snout vent lengths (SVLs) were 1,210 1,790 mm, and body masses were 473.5 533.0 g. Light conditions were 12 : 12 light : dark photoperiod, and room temperatures were 25 28uC. We conducted trials during the snakes peak activity periods (2 h after lights were off). Snakes were housed separately: each snake was in a,57-liter (15-gallon) glass terrarium with paper covering the floor and equipped with a water dish, a hiding box, and artificial foliage in one quarter of the box. Snakes were fed two adult mice (domestic Mus musculus) every two weeks and tested one week after feeding to control for hunger level within and among snakes. The juvenile Mangrove Monitor, Varanus indicus (300 mm SVL), used in the tests was also wild-caught on Guam and maintained under a 12 : 12 light : dark photoperiod and room temperatures of 25 28uC and fed euthanized domestic mice (Mus musculus). The monitor lizard was housed in a,76-liter (20- gallon) glass terrarium with paper covering the floor and equipped with a water dish, artificial foliage, rocks for cover, and a heat lamp. The domestic mice, Mus musculus (30 g), used in the tests were maintained under a 12 : 12 light : dark photoperiod, 22 23uC and housed in standard lab boxes with hardwood shavings, rodent chow, and water ad libitum. Testing Conditions. The testing chamber was, essentially, a small room (1.76 3 3 3 2.03 m tall), constructed of wood and plexiglass. The chamber temperature was 25 28uC. The walls of the chamber were covered with plastic contact paper, and the floor was covered with hardwood shavings, which were mixed and redistributed between each test to prevent scent trailing from previous trials. Moonlight illumination was simulated using 35 light bulbs (0.05 W) in the ceiling of the chamber, and the intensity of illumination was varied using a rheostat. Simulated moonlight levels were achieved by measuring light reflectance values in the field using a standard 90% white card and a Gossen Luna-Pro light meter and then reproducing these levels in the test chamber. The snakes were tested under simulated new moon (0.05 lux, no moon, starlight only), half moon (0.35 lux), and full moon (2.10 lux). A different light level was used each night for each snake (selected using a randomized schedule) and each snake was tested only once per night at 2200 h, when they exhibited maximum activity. Brown Treesnakes have been observed to maintain nocturnal behavior in laboratory situations (Chiszar et al., 1985; Chiszar and Kandler, 1986). All snakes were tested under the situations of moonlight-only, moonlight and mouse prey, and moonlight and lizard prey. Two artificial trees (2 m tall, canopy diameter,0.75 m) were placed in diagonally opposite corners of the chamber such that the snakes could not travel from one treetop to the other without leaving the canopy microhabitat. Because trials were conducted during the dark hours of the snakes photoperiod, we videotaped behaviors using four Sanyo black/white video cameras with internal infrared lighting sources placed within the chamber. At the beginning of each trial, the snake was placed in the center of the chamber on the floor, and at the conclusion of each 100-min trial, the snake was returned to its home terrarium. Tapes were analyzed by scoring the location of the snake at 60-sec intervals for 100 min in (1) canopy, (2) subcanopy, and (3) open ground areas. Pilot tests revealed that the snakes were remarkably similar in their acclimation time (P. 0.05). After a minimum of 8 min and a maximum of 12 min, with a mean, median, and mode of 10 min, all began to move.

248 S. R. CAMPBELL ET AL. The effect of terrestrial prey on Brown Treesnake microhabitat use was tested in new and full moonlight conditions. During these trials, we used a live mammalian prey and a live reptilian prey. A domestic mouse or a juvenile Mangrove Monitor lizard was placed in a transparent plastic box (210 3 300 3 180 mm tall) that had numerous small holes drilled in the top and sides to allow for airflow and scent dispersal. The box was placed in the center of the chamber, equidistant from the trees, to allow equal viewing opportunities of the prey from either tree s canopy or subcanopy microhabitat. When the prey box was in place, the treesnake was released onto the floor in the center of the chamber. Trials were then conducted for 100 min under new or full simulated moonlight (hereafter, termed moonlight) Data Analyses. Spearman Rank correlations were used to determine microhabitat use relationships among moonlight levels. We documented the snakes presence in the canopy, subcanopy, and open ground to determine microhabitat use. Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to compare the snakes use of each microhabitat type between each moonlight level and between the three test situations (moonlight only, mouse prey, lizard prey). If Kruskal- Wallis tests revealed significant differences, Freidman two-way analyses of variance (a nonparametric repeated-measures test) were conducted in pairwise comparisons to identify between which conditions differences existed. Significant differences were determined at the level of P # 0.05. RESULTS Moonlight-Only Trials. We observed a positive relationship between moonlight level and the snakes use of canopy vegetation and a concurrent decrease in the snakes use of open ground areas with increased moonlight (r 2 5 0.88, P, 0.0001, N 5 5 snakes; Fig. 1A). In new moonlight, no significant differences existed in the snakes use of canopy, subcanopy, and open ground areas because they were observed equally in each area (P. 0.05, all cases; Fig. 1A). In half and full moonlight, the snakes used canopy areas significantly more than they used open ground areas (Q 5 5.000, df n-1 5 4, P 5 0.025, both cases). The snakes also used the canopy more in full moonlight than they did in new or half moon light (Q 5 4.00, df n-1 5 4, P 5 0.04, all cases). Prey Present Trials. In the trials conducted with either mouse or lizard present, the snakes were not observed significantly more in open ground areas compared to conditions when prey was absent (P. 0.10, all cases). In full FIG. 1. Microhabitat use by Brown Treesnakes under different nocturnal illumination levels. (A) Microhabitat use (median plus 1 SE) in open, subcanopy, and canopy microhabitats under new moon (0.05 lux), half moon (0.35 lux), and full moon (2.10 lux) light levels without prey present. (B) Microhabitat use (median plus 1 SE) in open, subcanopy, and canopy microhabitats under new moon (0.05 lux) and full moon (2.10 lux) light levels with a mouse or a monitor lizard present. Microhabitat use did not change significantly when potential prey were present compared to when prey were absent. moonlight trials with prey present, observations of the Brown Treesnakes in the canopy were not significantly greater than in trials without prey (P. 0.10, all cases). Also, the snakes microhabitat use did not differ between trials with mouse prey compared to trials with the Mangrove Monitor prey (P. 0.10, all cases). Similar to the moonlight-only trials, the snakes used canopy microhabitat more than open ground areas in full moonlight, even when prey was present on the ground (Q 5 5.00, P 5 0.025; Fig. 1B). The snakes appeared to be aware of the prey in all moonlight levels. In 60% (3/5) of the mouse trials and 40% (2/5) of the Mangrove Monitor trials conducted in new moonlight, the Brown Treesnakes wrapped their bodies around the prey box and actively tried to enter the box. This behavior was never seen in full moonlight. In full moonlight, the Brown Treesnakes extended their heads from the canopy foliage to peer at the mouse or

MICROHABITAT USE BY BROWN TREESNAKES 249 Mangrove Monitor in the box, but they did not descend from the canopy nor did they ever appear to seek a way out of the chamber. DISCUSSION Species that are both predator and prey, such as the Brown Treesnake, are confronted with a challenging array of factors when making foraging decisions. The demands of avoiding predation while locating food, both of which are influenced by abiotic factors, place a complex set of pressures on species in this trophic level. Our findings overwhelmingly indicated that moonlight is a dominant factor affecting Brown Treesnake microhabitat use, appearing to exceed terrestrial prey cues. With and without mouse or lizard prey present, the Brown Treesnakes in our tests preferred canopy vegetation and avoided open ground areas in bright moonlight. Thus, our prediction that Brown Treesnakes would exhibit an increased preference for canopy microhabitat with increased moonlight was supported. However, our prediction that prey cues would change this trend was not supported. Even in the darkest illumination (new moon 5 starlight only), the snakes were aware of the presence and location of the prey and they oriented toward the prey or actively tried to enter the box (in 50% of all new moon tests). However, in full moonlight, the snakes only extended their heads from the canopy vegetation and appeared to focus their gaze at the prey, demonstrating that they were aware of prey presence, but snakes never descended from the canopy microhabitat. Precisely why moonlight so strongly affects Brown Treesnake behavior is unknown. We posit three possibilities: (1) in bright moonlight, treesnakes may be more vulnerable to predation by visually hunting predators (e.g., owls; Clarke, 1983); (2) terrestrial prey may be less available because they are avoiding the same predators (Lockard and Owings, 1974; Lima and Dill, 1990; Brillhart and Kaufman, 1991; Kotler et al., 1991), or (3) the Brown Treesnakes are less effective predators without visual cues (Mullin and Cooper, 1998; Lindberg et al., 2000; Shivik et al., 2000). Because the treesnakes left the canopy in new moonlight but not in full moonlight, even when prey was detected, we propose that predator avoidance is the principle factor contributing to their preference for canopy microhabitats in bright moonlight. The native range of the Brown Treesnake is northern Australia, Papua New Guinea, parts of Indonesia, and nearby island chains where it has natural predators, including monitor lizards, cobras, owls, and several mammals (Rodda et al., 1992, 1999; Caudell et al., 2002). On Guam, the natural nocturnal predators of Brown Treesnakes are absent, although snakes may be killed by dogs, cats, and pigs (Rodda et al., 1999). Apparently, the avoidance of activity in open areas in bright moonlight, a behavior associated with evading detection by visually hunting predators, has been retained by Brown Treesnakes for nearly 50 yr in their introduced habitat on Guam. Our finding that moonlight is a major abiotic factor affecting microhabitat use by Brown Treesnakes may have practical application. Artificially increasing light levels in and around areas where the risk of snakes exiting the island (such as sea ports and airports) might be effective in preventing introgression of snakes to other tropical habitats. In those few areas on Guam where trapping is used to reduce snake numbers, capture success may be increased if capture efforts were focused in specific microhabitats during specific times (such as on the ground during new moon and in the canopy during full moon). Furthermore, the influence of nocturnal illumination on the foraging behavior of these snakes may have conservation implications regarding the effect of light pollution on the behavior of other nocturnal reptiles. In our study, Brown Treesnakes significantly altered nocturnal activity patterns with increased nocturnal illumination, and terrestrial movements were essentially eliminated; whether this change in movement pattern is affected by light pollution in the wild is unknown. The negative consequence of artificial light on hatching sea turtles is well documented (e.g., Witherington, 1997), but the impact of light pollution on other reptiles behavior and ecology is an area requiring further research. Acknowledgments. We thank G. H. Rodda, the Guam Department of Natural Resources, and P. D. Burns. We also thank our outstanding undergraduate assistants: J. Broda, M. Browning, T. Dillon, A. Malone, R. Wallace, and T. Whalen. This study was supported by the Katie Edmonds Behavioral Ecology Laboratory Fund. LITERATURE CITED BRILLHART, D. B., AND D. W. KAUFMAN. 1991. Influence of illumination and surface structure on space use by Prairie Deer Mice (Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii). Journal of Mammalogy 72:764 768. CAUDELL, J. N., M. R. CONOVER, AND J. WHITTIER. 2002. Predation of Brown Tree Snakes (Boiga irregularis) in Australia. International Biodeterioration and Biodegredation 49:107 111. CHISZAR, D. A. 1990. Behavior of the Brown Tree Snake, Boiga irregularis: a study in applied comparative psychology. In D. Dewsbury (ed.), Con-

250 S. R. CAMPBELL ET AL. temporary Issues in Comparative Psychology, pp. 101 123. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA. CHISZAR, D., AND K. KANDLER. 1986. Adjustment of Brown Tree Snakes (Boiga irregularis) to a reversed light cycle. Bulletin of the Maryland Herpetological Society 22:171 174. CHISZAR, D., D. CARRILLO,P.RAND,J.CHISZAR, AND H. M. SMITH. 1985. Nocturnal activity in captive Brown Tree Snakes Boiga irregularis. Bulletin of the Maryland Herpetological Society 21:115 118. CLARKE, J. A. 1983. Moonlight s influence on predator/ prey interactions between Short-Eared Owls (Asio flammeus) and Deermice (Peromyscus maniculatus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 13:205 209. CLARKE, J. A., J. T. CHOPKO, AND S. P. MACKESSY. 1996. The effect of moonlight on activity patterns of adult and juvenile Prairie Rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis viridis). Journal of Herpetology 30:192 197. COGGER, H. G. 1975. Reptiles and Amphibians of Australia. A. H. and A. W. Reed, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia. DOWNES, S. 2001. Trading heat and food for safety: costs of predator avoidance in a lizard. Ecology 82:2870 2881. DOWNES, S., AND R. SHINE. 1998. Sedentary snakes and gullible geckos: predator-prey coevolution in nocturnal rock-dwelling reptiles. Animal Behaviour 55:1373 1385. FRITTS, T. H., AND G. H. RODDA. 1998. The role of introduced species in the degradation of island ecosystems: a case history of Guam. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 29:113 140. FRITTS, T. H., N. J. SCOTT, AND J. A. SAVIDGE. 1987. Activity of the arboreal Brown Treesnake (Boiga irregularis) on Guam as determined by electrical outages. Snake 19:51 58. KOTLER, B. P., J. S. BROWN, AND O. HASSON. 1991. Factors affecting gerbil foraging behavior and rates of owl predation. Ecology 72:2249 2260. LIMA, S. L., AND L. M. DILL. 1990. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 619 640. LINDBERG, A. C., J. A. SHIVIK, AND L. CLARK. 2000. Mechanical mouse lure for Brown Treesnakes. Copiea 2000:886 888. LOCKARD, R. B., AND D. H. OWINGS. 1974. Moon-related surface activity of Bannertail (Dipodomys spectabilis) and Fresno (D. nitroides) Kangaroo Rats. Animal Behaviour 22:262 273. MADSEN, T., AND M. OSTERCAMP. 1982. Notes on the biology of the fish-eating snake (Lycodonomorphus bicolor) in Lake Tanganyika. Journal of Herpetology 16:185 188. MADSEN, T., AND R. SHINE. 1996. Seasonal migration of predators and prey a study of pythons and rats in tropical Australia. Ecology 77:149 156. MULLIN, S. J., AND R. J. COOPER. 1998. Foraging ecology of the Gray Rat Snake (Elaph obsoleta spiloides) visual stimuli facilitate location of arboreal prey. American Midland Naturalist 40:397 401. RODDA, G. H., AND T. H. FRITTS. 1992. The impact of the introduction of the colubrid snake Boiga irregularis on Guam s lizards. Journal of Herpetology 26: 166 174. RODDA, G. H., T. H. FRITTS, AND P. J. CONRY. 1992. Origin and population growth of the brown tree snake, Boiga irregularis, on Guam. Pacific Science 46:46 57. RODDA, G. H., T. H. FRITTS, M.J.MCCOID, AND E. W. CAMPBELL III. 1999. An overview of the biology of the Brown Treesnake (Boiga irregularis), a costly introduced pest on Pacific Islands. In G. H. Rodda, Y. Sawai, D. Chiszar, and H. Tanaka (eds.), Problem Snake Management: The Habu and the Brown Treesnake, pp. 44 80. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY. ROTH, E. D., P. G. MAY, AND T. M. FERRELL. 1999. Pigmy Rattlesnakes use frog-derived chemical cues to select foraging sites. Copeia 1999:772 774. SAVIDGE, J. A. 1987. Extinction of an island forest avifauna by an introduced snake. Ecology 68:660 668. SHAH, B., R. SHINE, S. HUDSON, AND M. KEARNEY. 2004. Experimental analysis of retreat-site selection by thick-tailed geckos Nephrurus milii. Austral Ecology 29:547 552. SHIVIK, J. A., AND L. CLARK. 1997. Carrion seeking in Brown Tree Snakes: importance of olfactory and visual cues. Comparative Physiology and Biochemistry 279:549 553. SHIVIK, J. A., J. BOURASSA, AND S. N. DONNIGAN. 2000. Elicitation of Brown Treesnake predatory behavior using polymodal stimuli. Journal of Wildlife Management 64:969 975. THEODORATUS, D. H., AND D. CHISZAR. 2000. Habitat selection and prey odor in the foraging behavior of Western Rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis). Behaviour 137:119 135, 553. YAMAGISHI, M. 1974. Observation on the nocturnal activity of the habu with special reference to the intensity of moonlight. Snake 6:37 43. WITHERINGTON, B. E. 1997. The problem of photopollution for sea turtles and other nocturnal animals. In J. R. Clemmons and R. Buchholz (eds.), Behavioral Approaches to Conservation in the Wild, pp. 303 328. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Accepted: 1 November 2007.