NATURE RED IN CLAW : HOW AND WHY STARLINGS KILL EACH OTHER

Similar documents
Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

Capture and Marking of Birds: Field Methods for European Starlings

( 162 ) SOME BREEDING-HABITS OF THE LAPWING.

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

AGE AT FIRST BREEDING AND CHANGE IN PLUMAGE OF KELP GULLS LARUS DOMINICANUS IN SOUTH AFRICA. R. J. M. CRAWFORD*, B. M. DYER* and L.

PROTECTING MANLY S PENGUINS

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

By Hans Frey ¹ ² & Alex Llopis ²

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

Slide 1 NO NOTES. Slide 2 NO NOTES. Slide 3 NO NOTES. Slide 4 NO NOTES. Slide 5

(135) OBSERVATIONS IN A ROOKERY DURING THE INCUBATION PERIOD C. M. OGILVIE.

Conservation Management of Seabirds

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

EMBRYO DIAGNOSIS AN IMPORTANT TOOL TO HELP THE HATCHERY MANAGER

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1996) provides a comprehensive account.

BLUEBIRD NEST BOX REPORT

The Life of a Battery Hen Sadia Ahmed

He was a year older than her and experienced in how to bring up a brood and survive.

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL

468 TYRRELL, Nesting of Turkey Vulture

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

The story of Solo the Turnbull National Wildlife Refuge Male Swan

Seven Nests of Rufescent Tiger-Heron (Tigrisoma lineatum)

Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan

From mountain to sea. A Survivor s Guide to Living with Urban Gulls

In collaboration with the NJ Division of Fish & Wildlife s Endangered and Nongame Species Program

Summary of 2017 Field Season

Elwyn s Dream Teacher Notes by Raymond Huber

(261) THE INCUBATION PERIOD OF THE OYSTER-CATCHER

Short Report Key-site monitoring on Hornøya in Rob Barrett & Kjell Einar Erikstad

SOME PHOTOGRAPHIC STUDIES OF THE PINK-FOOTED GOOSE

FOOD HABITS OF NESTING COOPER S HAWKS AND GOSHAWKS IN NEW YORK AND PENNSYLVANIA

SEALANT, WATERPROOFING & RESTORATION INSTITUTE SPRING PEREGRINE FALCONS: DIS RAPTORS OF WORK AT HEIGHT

Level 2 Biology, 2015

Rock Wren Nesting in an Artificial Rock Wall in Folsom, Sacramento County, California

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

ROYAL SWAN UPPING The Queen ueen s Diamond Jubilee Edition

Identification of predators of Royal Albatross chicks at Taiaroa Head in February 1994

Did you know that Snowy Plovers (Charadrius alexandrines char-ad-ree-us alex-an-dreen-us):

(199) THE HATCHING AND FLEDGING OF SOME COOT

(82) FIELD NOTES ON THE LITTLE GREBE.

Best Practice in the Breeder House

the Greek words for Love + Bird = Lovebird.Lovebirds can be classified as aggressive birds to other birds as well as their own species.

Tristan Darwin Project. Monitoring Guide. A Guide to Monitoring Albatross, Penguin and Seal Plots on Tristan and Nightingale

Swallow Monitoring. Cowichan Estuary April 3 rd to July 26th

Record of Predation by Sugar Glider on Breeding Eastern Rosellas 33Km NE of Melbourne in November 2016

The behaviour of a pair of House Sparrows while rearing young

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Hawke s Bay Regional Predator Control Technical Protocol (PN 4970)

LOVE ON THE ROCKS. Beauty of the Beast AMPHIBIAN BEHAVIOR

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

GeesePeace a model program for Communities

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

In the summers of 1977 and 1978, at Gibraltar Point, Lincolnshire, I

MANAGING AVIARY SYSTEMS TO ACHIEVE OPTIMAL RESULTS. TOPICS:

Text: Elly Vogelaar Photos: Aviculture Europa

(340) PHOTOGRAPHIC STUDIES OF SOME LESS FAMILIAR BIRDS. LIX. NIGHT HERON.

By: Rinke Berkenbosch

Chris Knights and Terry Andrewartha have taken some remarkable. 440 [Brit. Birds 73: , October 1980]

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

6 Month Progress Report. Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa. VulPro NPO

THE KOMODO DRAGON. endangered species L ARCHE PHOTOGRAPHIQUE CHARACTERISTICS. Animal Phylum. Kingdom

Red Crowned Parakeet (Cyanoramphus novaezelandiae) health, disease and nesting study on Tiritiri Matangi 2014/2015. Emma Wells on behalf of

Bird Species Fact Sheets

Corn Snake Care Sheet

Yellow-throated and Solitary Vireos in Ontario: 4. Egg Laying, Incubation and Cowbird Parasitism

Endangered Birds. Visit for thousands of books and materials.

Sparrowhawks & Goshawks and the Gymnogene

Parthenogenesis in Varanus ornatus, the Ornate Nile Monitor.

Piping Plover. Below: Note the color of the sand and the plover s back.

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

Great Blue Heron Chick Development. Through the Stages

Unit A: Introduction to Poultry Science. Lesson 1: Exploring the Poultry Industry

CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015

Scholarship 2017 Biology

The Heartfelt Story of our Backyard Bluebirds

RABBITS. Code of practice for keeping rabbits in Western Australia ISBN

Swans & Geese. Order Anseriformes Family Anserinae

A Study to Determine the Preference for Nesting Box Design of Sialia sialis

HEALTH, PRODUCTIVITY AND BEHAVIOUR OF CONVENTIONAL TURKEY BREEDS UNDER ECOLOGICAL OUTDOOR REARING CONDITIONS

Eastern Bluebird Early Egg Viability Outcomes- A Mini- Study. By Penny Brandau and Paula Ziebarth

Wilson Bull., 94(2), 1982, pp

Egg removal and intraspeeifie brood parasitism in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris)

1. Adélie Penguins can mate for life or at least try to find the same mate every year.

By: Dr. Antje Mewes, Andreas Frei, Jan Dams, Gerardus Scheres. Weltvogelpark Walsrode, Germany.

Interim Madge Lake Loon Survey August 2016

UK HOUSE MARTIN SURVEY 2015

SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 757

Short Report Key-site monitoring on Hornøya in Rob Barrett & Kjell Einar Erikstad

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Egg laying Late May to early June Mid-May to mid-july 3 to 10

ISSN Department of Conservation. Reference to material in this report should be cited thus:

Animal behaviour (2016, 2) THE SPOTTED HYENA

NOTES ON THE SPRING TERRITORY OF THE BLACKBIRD

3. Chicks weigh 86 grams when they hatch and gain 100 grams a day until they are about 50 days old when they are ready to take care of itself.

Transcription:

NATURE RED IN CLAW : HOW AND WHY STARLINGS KILL EACH OTHER By J.E.C. FLUX and M.LM. FLUX ABSTRACT In a 20-year nest box study of Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) in New Zealand, intraspecific fighting was the main mortality factor recorded. All fights were between birds of the same sex, 24 male-male and 22 female-female, and most were in the breeding season. Fighting increased as the population rose from about 300 birds in 1969 to over 3000 after 1978. Because of the low annual mortality rate of Starlings in New Zealand (330/0), the chances of ever breeding were small for many birds. Hence conditions were ideal for a high level of fatal fighting, which took the form of grasping the opponent round the head so that the claws penetrated the brain through the eye sockets. INTRODUCTION Fighting, between animals of the same species, that is severe enough to cause serious wounds is normally so rare that individual reports merit publication (e.g. Taylor 1969, Lombardo 1986). Because of the high risk to the contestants, physical combat is usually replaced by conventional threat behaviour (Wynne-Edwards '1962). Smith & Price (1973) demonstrated that a "limited war" strategy could also arise through benefits to the individual and did not depend on group selection. Most discussion has centred on the obvious reluctance with which animals equipped with efficient weapons use them in intraspecific fights, but the list of apparently weakly armed species which occasionally fight to the death continues to grow (Hik & Cooke 1988, Huntingford & Turner 1988). Fatal fighting will evolve "when the value of a contested resource is similar to or greater than the value of the future", that is, "where a major part of a contestant's lifetime reproductive success is at stake" (Enquist & Leimar 1990). In most birds and mammals reproduction extends over several years and it pays to live to fight another day; hence we were surprised to find that intraspecific fighting was the major cause of death observed in a population of Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) under study since 1970. METHODS The 1500 ha study area at Belmont, 15 km northeast of Wellington, New Zealand (41 10' S, 174O54' E) was described by Flux & Flux (1981). It is open hilly pasture grazed by sheep and a few cattle, with no natural nest sites for Starlings within 3 km. Before the study began, about 26 pairs of Starlings nested successfully on ledges in disused army buildings in the centre of the area, and a flock of 300 birds fed in the area. A nest-box colony was set up in the buildings with 100 boxes in 1970, and 100 more were added each year until 500 were available from 1974 to the present. After 1975,400 pairs nested and the population rose to at least 3000 birds by 1978. The nest boxes were 40 cm wide, 20 cm high and 15 cm deep, with an entrance hole 6 cm square in the centre (280 boxes) or towards one side (220 boxes). Starlings preferred boxes with the hole off-centre and this preference was consistent between years (Flux & Flux 1981). We inspected the boxes every second day for the first two weeks of the breeding season, NOTORNIS 39: 293-300 (1992)

294 FLUX & FLUX NOTORNIS 39 and thereafter every 2-3 days until the chicks had flown. For the rest of the year, boxes were examined at 3-month intervals to check for dead birds and to repair broken boxes. From 1980, field work was curtailed to three or four visits during the laying period and two mortality checks, in spring and autumn each year, except for 1984 when the full programme was reinstated. Evidence of mortality was recorded incidentally during brief visits to inspect the nest box contents, and so most corpses (301) were in boxes; another 20 were on the ground nearby. Corpses which had obvious peck marks, or blood in the eyes or bill, were classified as having died fighting. Those which were of normal body weight and in good physical condition with no wounds, but in a disturbed nest, were recorded as "possibly fighting". Birds showing signs of poor health, diarrhoea, or other ailments were recorded as "unknown", and the final category, "predation", included those with large wounds caused by rats, stoats or cats. Adult Starlings are not very palatable, and predators usually ate only the brain and some breast muscle. RESULTS Annual incidence of fighting At the start of the study few Starlings were caught fighting, but this could have been related to the smaller number of nest boxes available (for example, 5 birds fighting in 1970 in 100 boxes would be equivalent to 25 in 1974 when 500 boxes were available). Even so, however, there was a significant increase in the number of fights per 500 boxes, comparing the first five years (55) with the second five years (110) (XZ = 8.44, P< 0.01), as shown in Table 1. TABLE 1 - Starlings found dead in nest boxes by 5-year periods and sex. For definitions of cause of death see text. After 1980, with reduced field work, few bodies could be classified as "fighting" and the number of unknown" increased. Period Cause of Death 1970-74 197579 1980-84 198589 Total Fighting d 2 6 6 14 Probably fighting d 6 25 20 5 56 S 13 37 21 16 87 Unknown d 5 7 24 15 51 S 3 13 37 18 71 Predation d 1 2 1 1 5 Total Fights seen d 8 16 2 1 27

1992 STARLINGS KILL EACH OTHER 295 In the first four years when the number of boxes was increased by 100 each year, occupancy rates were 38010, 47010, 75010, and 78%. Thereafter, with 500 boxes available, occupancy remained steady at an average of 71% (range 67 to 80%). Empty nest boxes were evidently unsuitable or unavailable for other Starlings to nest in, probably because dominant males controlled several boxes. The presence of local birds ready to breed but unable to do so was verified by an experiment on 31 October 1980, when 18 incubating females were killed; by 22 November 1980 all except one of these boxes contained eggs or newly hatched young, and from the age of the chicks at least five of the new females had laid on the seventh day after the box became available. Description and definition of fights Starlings are aggressive birds, both to other species at a bird-table or among themselves when feeding, but the recorded descriptions of fighting, bill stabbing and "Dance-fighting" (Feare 1984) do not include the sort of fighting seen in our nest boxes. In these fights the combatants were silent, motionless except for occasional muscular contractions of the wings or feet, and almost oblivious of their surroundings. Some pairs did not disengage even when picked up and carried to the ground for banding. Attempts by a bird to grasp its opponent's bill often resulted in the symmetrical head-to-tail position shown in Figure 1, and the sharp claws seemed to enter the brain through the eye socket. Normally one bird killed the other and then escaped, but in four boxes both birds died together, all in the head-to-tail position. In the following analyses "fighting" refers to serious encounters only, and not casual squabbling or "dance-fighting". Sex and age of combatants All 46 fights where the sex of both birds was known were male v. male (24) or female v. female (22). No fights involved more than two birds. First-year Starlings can be distinguished from older birds by the length of sheen on the throat feathers (Flux & Flux 1981). In the 22 female fights, 15 were between adults, 3 between an adult and a first-year bird, and 4 between first-year birds. Over the whole study, only 11.5% of the nesting females (n = 2981) were first-year birds, and so the female fights involved more first-year birds than expected ( XZ = 5.85, P - 0.05). Because the percentage of first-year birds nesting varied widely between years (1.3-22.2010) the year with most fights was analysed separately: in 1977, 13 adult females and 9 first-year birds were found fighting, significantly more first-years than expected from the ratio of adult (336) to first-years (74) nesting that year (x2 = 5.63, P < 0.05). In the 24 fights between males, 21 were between adults, 2 between an adult and a first-year bird, and 1 between first-year males. Males were not caught for banding, and so their age ratio in the study area was not known. Quality of nest box and contents Although all the nest boxes were of the same size, material, and colour, in similar buildings, and at the same height (4 m), those with an entrance'hole towards one side were obviously preferred by Starlings (92% occupancy)

296 FLUX & FLUX NOTORNIS 39 FIGURE 1 - Two adult male Starlings found dead in a nest box, 20 October 1986, with claws penetrating each other's eyes

1992 STARLINGS KILL EACH OTHER 297 to boxes with a central hole (65% occupancy, Flux & Flux 1981). Of the 71 fights, 54 were in boxes with a side entrance, compared with 17 in boxes with a central entrance. This was significantly different (X2 = 5.32, P 0.05) from the expected ratio (40: 31) based on the availability of the different boxes (280 side, 220 central), showing a tendency to fight for preferred boxes. However, the trend was not significant (x2 = 1.66) if the comparison was based on the proportion of occupied boxes (46:25). Twenty-four male fights (73%) and 19 female fights (63%) were in boxes containing nests under construction. Two male fights and seven female fights were in nests containing eggs, one fight each for males and females was in a box with halfgrown young (10 and 12 days old), and two male fights were in boxes from which young had recently fledged. Six male fights and three female fights were in empty boxes. Seasonal distribution of fighting Although fighting birds were obviously more likely to be encountered during the breeding season when boxes were examined at least every three days, the seasonal distribution of deaths caused by fighting (Table 2) should be less dependent on searching effort because Starling bodies lasted for several months. Most fights took place in the breeding season (September- November), with only 3 of 25 bodies showing signs of fighting in other months. All three were males, which visit their boxes daily thoughout the year; but one pair of females was found, locked together, in a nest box on 11 April 1986, and so both sexes may fight occasionally outside the breeding season. TABLE 2 - Seasonal distribition of deaths and fights seen in Starling nest boxes J F M A M J J A S 0 NDTotal Males found dead 1 2 1 7 3 14 Females found dead Two males fighting One male and one unidentified fighting Two females fighting 1 One female and one unidentified fighting To examine the distribution of fighting within the breeding season in more detail, we grouped data from all years by 5-day periods, starting from the median first-egg laying date for each year. This is necessary to allow for differences in the start of laying between years, which may be as much as 22 days (Flux 1987). Within years, however, laying is remarkably synchronised, yith almost all first clutches starting within 7 days of the median date. Figure 2 shows that males start fighting over boxes earlier than females do, and they continue longer (goodness of fit test, grouped data, X? = 8.07, P 4 0.05). This agrees with the annual pattern of Table 2, but males were not more aggressive than females overall because the total

FLUX & FLUX Days from peak egg laying (day 0) FIGURE 2 - Dates of 27 fights between males (blank columns) and 27 fights between females (shaded) in relation to the peak first egg laying date, combined data, 1970-1984 numbers of fights between males (35) and between females (38) were similar. The intensity of fights could be assessed by the chance of catching both birds locked together, or only one - again there was no significant difference between sexes (22 pairs, 13 single, males; 22 pairs, 16 single, females). Significance of mortality by fighting Intraspecific fighting appears to be the commonest mortality factor of breeding birds in this Starling population, with 25 definite deaths and 143 probably deaths (Table 1). Although some of the "probably fighting" category may have died of other causes, some of the "unknown" category could have died in fights or from wounds or infection after a fight. The 31 birds that died of predation were mostly killed by stoats (Mustela emzinea), which learned to climb to the boxes in 1976, killing 22 adult Starlings (mostly females on eggs) between 1976 and 1980. Three other Starlings were killed by feral cats (Felis catus). Over the whole study an average of 16 bodies per year was found, while the average nesting population of 296 pairs, at 33% annual mortality, should have produced 195 bodies each year, or at least 49 during the 3-month nesting season. Hence most mortality went undetected, and the importance of fighting in population control could not be assessed. Factors killing birds which were not found are unlikely to be the same as for birds in boxes. DISCUSSION The earliest record of serious fighting among Starlings in New Zealand seems to be by Hornby School (1946); in a nest box "two cock birds were seen to be fighting so fiercely that they were easily caught in one handful. Even then they fought each other if allowed". Also in New Zealand, Coleman (1974) recorded one male-male and three female-female fights in nest boxes, and one participant died. Kessel (1957), at Ithaca, New York, found that "fights with intruding males become quite violent. Several times4ighting pairs have been seen to fall to the ground, gripping each other strongly and

1992 STARLINGS KILL EACH OTHER 299 pecking savagely... even when handled they will not always loosen their grip". Many of our fighters behaved similarly and tried to re-engage when separated instead of flying away. It is also significant that all recorded fights have been between birds of the same sex. Presumably a partner gains if its mate is replaced by a stronger bird, and so it does not pay in evolutionary terms to defend a weak mate. Although casual fighting is normal in Starlings overseas (Feare 1984), serious fights seem commoner in our population than elsewhere. Some serious fighting may have been overlooked, even in a species as widely studied as the Starling, because recording aggression is not the main purpose of nest box studies. Thus, in New Jersey, Romagnano et al. (1986), describing suspected infanticide, incidentally recorded: "similar peck marks have been found around the head and neck of dead adults (n = 8 pairs), suggesting that bill stabbing is not uncommon." On the same study area Stouffer et al. (1988) found that "female starlings fighting in nest boxes (n = 4 fights in 1986)... are not unusual events". In Britain, Evans (1988) recorded that "fights between two females at a nestbox (and between two males) were also observed at a few nests." The number and intensity of fights are likely to vary with the shortage of nest boxes. In Belgium, where "nest boxes were not a limited resource" (Pinxten et al. 1989), "the competition between males for nest boxes was mainly one of primacy of occupation and not of physical struggle." At Belmont our population of over 3000 Starlings was clearly limited by the 500 boxes available, and the chances of getting a box were made worse by the low annual mortality in New Zealand of 33% compared with 50-70% overseas (Flux & Flux 1981). Hence conditions were ideal for the expression of fatal fighting: "When the value of the future is close to zero contestants will never give up after starting to fight; such fights will always be fatal for at least one of the opponents" (Enquist & Leimar 1990). The Starling's ultimate weapon is the head grip (Figure l), which has not been adequately described in any species to our knowledge, although it has been recorded for Bellbirds (Anthmis melanura) by Taylor (1969) and a photograph of a heron applying it illustrates an article by Huntingford & Turner (1988). If, as we believe, the 143 deaths of apparently healthy birds were caused by this grip, in addition to the 25 definite deaths, intraspecific flghting becomes the main mortality factor observed in our study area. It may be found to be more widespread in Starling populations elsewhere and in other birds facing severe competition once the symptoms are recognised. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We are very grateful to P.E. Cowan, B.D. Heather, A.D. Pritchard, M.R. Rudge and R.H. Taylor for helpful comments and suggestions., LITERATURE CITED COLEMAN, J.D. 1974. The use of d cal sites erected for Starlings in Canterbury, New Zealand. NZ J. 2001. 1: 349-354. ENQUIST, M.; LEIMAR, 0. 1990. The evolution of fatal tighthg. him. Behav. 39: 1-9. EVANS, P.G.H. 1988. Intraspecific nest parasitism in the European Starling Sturn Pnrlgaris. him. Behav. 36: 1282-1294. FEARE, C. 1984. The Starling. Oxford University Press. 315 pp.

300 FLUX & FLUX NOTORNIS 39 FLUX J.E.C. 1987. Drift in laying dates of Starlings Sturnus vulgaris. Ornis Sand. 18: 146-148. FLUX: J.E.C.; FLUX, M.M. 1981. Population dynamics and age structure of Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) in New Zealand. NZ J. Ecol. 4: 65-72. HIK, D.S.; COOKE, F. 1988. A possible case of intraspecific killing in the Lesser Snow Goose. Wilson Bull. 100: 665-666. HORNBY SCHOOL. 1946. Breeding habits of Starlings. N.Z. Bid Notes 2: 8-10. HUNTINGFORD, F.; TURNER, A. 1988. Aggression: a biological imperative? New Scientist 4 August 1988: 44-47. KESSEL, B. 1957. A study of the breeding biology of the European Starling (Sturnus wlgaris L.) in North America. Am. Mid. Nat. 58: 257-331. LOMBARDO, M.P. 1986. A possible case of adult intraspecific killing in the Tree Swallow. Condor 88: 112. PINXTEN, R.; EENS, M.; VERHEYEN, R.F. 1989. Polygyny in the European Starling. Behaviow 111: 1-4. ROMAGNANO, L.; LOMBARDO, M.P.; STOUFFER, P.C.; POWER, H.W. 1986. Suspected infanticide in the Starling. Condor 88: 530-531. SMITH, J.M.; PRICE, G.R. 1973. The logic of animal conflict. Nature 246: 15-18. STOUFFER, P.C.; ROMAGNANO, L.C.; LOMBARDO, M.P.; HOFPENBERG, AS.; POWER. H.W. 1988. A case of communal nestlug m the European Starling. Condor 90: 241-245. TAYLOR, R.H. 1969. Male Bellbirds lock+ in combat. Notornis 15: 63. WYNNE-EDWARDS, V.C. 1962. Animal &spersion in relation to social behanow. Oliver and Boyd, Edinburgh and London. 653 pp. J.E.C. FLUX & M.M. FLUX, DSIR Land Resources, Private Bag, Lower Hurt