The Kennel Club/Scottish Kennel Club campaign to prohibit the use of electric shock collars in Scotland

Similar documents
The Kennel Club has long campaigned for a ban on the use and sale of electric shock collars in Scotland.

Potential controls or prohibition of electronic training aids in Scotland

SUBMISSION FROM THE ELECTRONIC COLLAR MANUFACTURERS ASSOCIATION

Behavior Modification Why Punishment Should Be Avoided

The Dog and Cat Management Board. Policy and Procedure for the training of dogs subject to a dangerous dog order

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression Getting Started Safety and Management

Dogs Trust Pawlicy Document

Conflict-Related Aggression

Q1 The effectiveness of the Act in reducing the number of out of control dogs/dog attacks in Scotland.

Aggression Social Aggression to Unfamiliar Dogs

Understanding your dog's behaviour will help you prevent and reduce behaviour problems.

K9K-914 Anti Bark Collar User's Manual. Introduction:

Dog Training Collar Introduction

General Tips If you have any questions, please contact the Customer Care Centre. For a listing of Customer Care Centre telephone numbers, visit our

Insider's Guide To The Cavalier King Charles Spaniel - The Dog Barking Helper HOW TO MANAGE DOGGY PROBLEMS. Dog Barking Help

Kennel Club Response to the Home Affairs Committee s call for evidence on the draft Anti-Social Behaviour Bill.

Calming Signals - The Art of Survival

A1 Control of dangerous and menacing dogs (reviewed 04/01/15)

Behavior Modification Reinforcement and Rewards

Battersea response to the Public Audit and Post-legislative Scrutiny Committee s call for evidence on the Control of Dogs (Scotland) Act 2010

English One Name Reading Test 2 (20 points) Man s Best Friend Just Got Better By Darwin Wigget, The Guardian, March 14, 2016

Deafblind Scotland vision A society in which deafblind people have the permanent support and recognition necessary to be equal citizens

Evaluation of XXXXXXX mixed breed male dog

Article VIII. Potentially Dangerous Dogs and Vicious Dogs

Position statements. Updated May, 2013

Dog and Cat Management Board. Accredited Behavioural Assessments for Greyhounds

Dog Bite Prevention Handout written by Steph Callahan

Basic Commands and Training

Golden Rule Training. Desensitizing Your Dog to Specific Noises, Other Dogs and Situations

Homeward Bound Golden Retriever Rescue

AGGRESSION (CATS) DIAGNOSING AND TREATING

SP6B NUISANCE BARKING IN DOGS AND THE WELFARE IMPLICATIONS OF DIFFERENT TREATMENT MODALITIES

It s a dog s life: vet nursing at Dogs Trust centre, Leeds

Overview LANCTB1. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs. Observe, assess and respond to the behaviour of dogs

RSPCA SA v Ross and Fitzpatrick Get the Facts

Service Animal Procedure, Student and Community Procedure

TRAINING & BEHAVIOR QUESTIONNAIRE

REGIONAL WORKSHOP ON ANIMAL WELFARE IN TRANSPORT AND SLAUGHTER (RWAWTS)

Guide Dogs Puppy Development and Advice Leaflet. No. 3 Relief routines

International Shelter Programme for Applied Canine Studies (ISPACS)

Dogs. Bite Prevention. For People Who in the Course of Their Work, Meet Dogs

To choke or not to choke How positive reinforcement has affected the use of choke collars in dog training

New Student Registration (page 1 of 5)

Our K9 LLC 616 Corporate Way Valley Cottage New York GARNET STATIC SHOCK BARK COLLAR USERS GUIDE

Dangerous Dogs and Safeguarding Children Contents

Durham Kennel Club. Disruptive Dog Policy

What is the average time needed to train a dog using a pet containment system?

Your Dog s Evaluation Result: Separation Anxiety

Is your dog barking too much?

Guide Dogs Puppy Development and Advice Leaflet. No. 4 Identifying and preventing aggressive behaviours inguide dog puppies

Dog Behavior Problems Barking and Training Quiet

PetSpy Premium Dog Training Collar, Models M919-1/M919-2

Biting, Nipping & Jumping Up

2015 No. 108 ANIMALS, ENGLAND. The Microchipping of Dogs (England) Regulations 2015

Behaviour of cats and dogs

Review of dog training methods: welfare, learning ability, and current standards

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression - Sibling Rivalry Treatment

PetSpy Advanced Dog Training System, Model M86N

DOG CONTROL POLICY 2016

THE CORPORATION OF THE VILLAGE OF WARFIELD BYLAW 703

Pediatric Behavior Problems Dogs Basics

JUMPING By Molly Stone, Dip. A.B; CDBC; CC-SF/SPCA Animal Behavior Specialist, SPCA of Wake County

Gemma Stephen, Natures Whisper: Dog Behaviour Grimsby 2015 Page 1

Proceedings of the European Veterinary Conference Voorjaarsdagen

Office of Residence Life Service Animal Procedure

English *P48988A0112* E202/01. Pearson Edexcel Functional Skills. P48988A 2015 Pearson Education Ltd. Level 2 Component 2: Reading

Puppy Development. Part One

Report to ENVIRONMENTAL PLANNING & REGULATIONS Committee for decision

PUBLIC SPACES PROTECTION ORDERS DOG CONTROLS CULTURE AND LEISURE (COUNCILLOR PETER BRADBURY)

Outdoor Bark Control

How to have a well behaved dog

What this guide covers

GARNET STATIC SHOCK BARK COLLAR

Kathy Wilson-Good Dog Manners The Lake Veterinary Hospitals &

Appendix for Mortality resulting from undesirable behaviours in dogs aged under three years. attending primary-care veterinary practices in the UK

GARNET STATIC SHOCK BARK COLLAR

JOINT BVA-BSAVA-SPVS RESPONSE TO THE CONSULTATION ON PROPOSALS TO TACKLE IRRESPONSIBLE DOG OWNERSHIP

GALLATIN COUNTY ORDINANCE NO GALLATIN COUNTY DOG CONTROL ORDINANCE

Other people in your household Name Relationship to you Age

Third Party Sales of Puppies and Kittens

Unit 3 Sustainability and interdependence Sub Topic 3.4: Animal welfare

INVERCARGILL CITY COUNCIL. Bylaw 2018/2 Dog Control

Understanding anxiety & its implications for pet greyhounds. By: Dr Karen Cunnington BSc BVSc Hons

Information Guide. Do you know dog law?

THE KENNEL CLUB WORKING GUNDOG CERTIFICATE HANDBOOK

Play-Aggression in Kittens or Cats

Learning How to Deal with Your Fearful Dog


180 Degree Rescue Canine Adoption Contract

Ultra Min No-Bark Training Collar Ultra Small Ultra Powerful Ultra Control

TINY PAWS DOG RESCUE CANADA Foster Home Application

The welfare impact and efficacy of canine training methods

Table of Contents Operating Guide Training Guide

VILLAGE OF ROSALIND BY-LAW A BYLAW OF THE VILLAGE OF ROSALIND IN THE PROVINCE OF ALBERTA, PROVIDING FOR THE CONTROLLING OF DOGS.

Mark Scheme. November Functional Skills English. Reading Level 2 E202

Guideline to Supplement to Codes of Practice Greyhound Euthanasia

Used correctly, the ecollar is the most humane training tool I have ever seen. This may sound shocking, but nevertheless, it is true.

The Evidence For Positive Reinforcement Training By Pippa Mattinson

Adopting a rescue dog

5. COMPLIANCE. Policy 5.5. Companions Animals Policy. Version 2

Transcription:

The Kennel Club/Scottish Kennel Club campaign to prohibit the use of electric shock collars in Scotland What are electronic training aids? It is important to note electric shock collars are different to other electronic training aids such as vibration collars and sonic collars. The debate is only about shock collars. Other electronic devices include: 1) Vibration collars such collars emit a vibration to a dog around its neck. They are unnecessary in most cases, but in some cases i.e. for deaf dogs, that can be useful training aids. 2) Sonic collars these collars emit a high pitched sound audible only to dogs, which is unpleasant for them. They are aversive training devices and most modern dog trainers would not recommend them. However there is no conclusive research to prove that they cause lasting damage to dogs. 3) Citronella spray collars these collars spray citronella spray on the dogs face. They are aversive training devices and most modern dog trainers would not recommend them. However there is no conclusive research to prove that they cause lasting damage to dogs. What are electric shock collars and why are they different from other electronic training aids? Electric shock collars train dogs through a fear of further punishment, rather than a natural willingness to obey. There are various models (approximately 170) readily available, ranging from 10 to 200 plus. The cheaper collars will normally have one setting, whilst the more expensive collars have a range of settings, some from 1-100. At a very low setting, the collar may emit little more than a vibration, but when used above a 20 setting, they emit more of a shock to the dog which gets greater the higher the setting is. Any negative method employed to prevent a particular behaviour in dogs has to be extremely aversive and painful enough in order to stop the undesired behaviour from reoccurring. Unlike the other electronic devices mentioned above, there is conclusive evidence which proves that they cause physical and psychological harm to dogs. As a result, unlike with the other devices, they are already banned in many other countries. Why would dog owners purchase electric shock collars? Given the manner in which products are marketed, and the wide range of retailers that offer them for sale, we believe that dog owners who purchase electric shock collars are not normally doing so with the intention of causing physical or psychological harm to their pets. Afterall, if a dog owner was intent on harming their animal, they would be able to do so through other means. As an example, this is how an online retailer describes a remote control electronic shock collar: This training system is ideal for times when you take your dog along without a leash. 1

It lets you interrupt your dog's unwanted behavior with a harmless static correction. Dogs quickly learn to associate the static correction with their prohibited behaviour. http://www.drsfostersmith.com/product/prod_display.cfm?c=3307+9+30+24569&pcatid=245 69 Similarly, the few dog trainers in Scotland who encourage the use of training dogs with electric shock collars advertise their training days in such a way that it appears harmless to the average dog owner THIS WORKSHOP IS SUITABLE FOR ANY BREED OF DOG, SIZE & FOR ANY BEHAVIOURAL ISSUES.ALL WELCOME. Are you having issues with your dog? Anything ranging from recall, destruction, aggression, separation issues, in fact anything then this may be the answer. There is a lot of speculation as to what an e-collar is & what it can actually do. The usual response when it's mentioned is that they are cruel & that your dog receives an electric shock the dog will then become "shut down", the list goes on. I will admit I was very sceptical but having seen the results that have not been managed by other methods, they can only be seen as a positive tool. Why not come along and see the proof for yourself. There will be dogs there with issues ranging from no recall, also known as selective hearing!, to aggression towards other dogs. This will be the first time these dogs will have worn an e-collar so you will be seeing how they work right from the beginning. These problems limit the owners as to where they can walk their dog & the amount of exercise the dog can have. I'm sure we would all love to let our dogs off the lead no matter where we take them. These collars are also known as freedom collars & that's exactly what they will allow your dog to do. Please message me if you are interested for costs. Are electric shock collars banned in other countries? How successful have these bans been? Electric shock collars were banned in Wales under the Animal Welfare (Electronic Collars) (Wales) Regulations 2010. The Electronic Collar Manufacturers Association challenged the regulations under Judicial Review on the basis that the regulations breached the European Convention of Human Rights, but the Royal Courts of Justice found in favour of the Welsh Assembly and the regulations remain unchanged following a routine five year review. We are not aware of any dogs being put to sleep in Wales owing to their owners not being able to use an electric shock collar on them (this is the most common justification for their use explained below). The use of shock collars is banned Denmark, Norway, Sweden, Austria, Switzerland, Slovenia, and Germany and in some territories of Australia, including New South Wales and Southern Australia. What support is there for electric shock collars to be completely banned? Within just one month, a Change.org petition calling for the use of shock collars in Scotland to be completely prohibited attracted 15,000 signatures. Prior to this, surveys commissioned by the Kennel Club have shown: 73% of the Scottish public disapprove of the use of electric shock collars on dogs (Kennel Club commissioned survey, 2014) 2

82% agree that positive reinforcement training methods can address behavioural issues in dogs without the need for negative training methods (Kennel Club commissioned survey, 2014) 74% of the Scottish public would support the government to introduce a ban on electric shock collars (Kennel Club commissioned survey, 2014). In addition to this, all the leading welfare, veterinary and training and behaviour organisations have joined forces in calling for a complete ban on shock collars to be brought forward in Scotland including: the Kennel Club, Scottish Kennel Club, Dogs Trust, Battersea Dogs & Cats Home, Edinburgh Dog & Cat Home, the British Veterinary Association, the Animal Behaviour and Training Council, the Association of Pet Dog Trainiers, the Association of Pet Behaviour Counsellors, the COAPE Association of Applied Pet Behaviourists and Trainiers, and Kennel Club Accredited Instructors. The training and behaviour organisations listed represent around 2,000 professional dog trainers, all of whom have signed a code of conduct against using electric shock training devices. Can electric shock collar use be justified as a last resort? At this stage, it has widely been accepted that the general use of electric shock collars is unacceptable, but the Scottish Government believes that there may be a need for them as a last resort, when all other methods have failed, and that the use of a shock collar could save a dog s life. This is wholly untrue. Below are the most commonly cited justifications for using a shock collar: 1) They may be particularly effective for specific types of dogs, including some working dog breeds, which have a very strong instinct to chase other animals and which may not respond to other training cues. However, training methods which are punishment-based and dependent on inflicting pain or creating fear suppress behaviour without addressing its underlying cause or the motivation behind it. Dogs have a natural inbuilt flight or fight response when put in a situation that causes pain and fear, meaning the dog either escapes from the source of pain (flight) or becomes aggressive in response (fight). As a consequence, electric shock collars can cause further behavioural problems in addition to the one(s) being treated. The risk is that the dog can develop superstitious fears to objects in its environment that were heard or seen at the time of receiving the electric shock. This is why cases of dogs attacking other dogs, their owner or another animal close by at the time of the shock are quite common. It is our view that using an electronic collar for this purpose is wholly unnecessary given that most dog walkers will simply keep their dogs on leads when around livestock. This is also the advice of the National Farmers Union. 2) They have helped owners with recall who would otherwise be unable to let their dogs off lead. However, it is the experience of the 2,000-plus members of training and behaviour organisations which ban the use of shock collars, that recall can be effectively achieved through positive training methods and that using electronic devices for recall is wholly unnecessary. 3) Most of those who use electronic training aids use them properly. Anything can be open to misuse, but there is no particular association with electronic training aids if 3

someone is determined to abuse an animal they will find a way to be cruel or neglectful. However, electronic training aids themselves are very difficult to use correctly and create a risk that the animal associates coincidental events with the punishment, especially if that punishment is poorly timed, or for boundary fence systems, if the animal is not able to see the boundary markings. There are much more effective and humane positive reinforcement training methods available. 4) Finally, proponents of electric shock collar training claim that the existing legislation is sufficient to protect animals as it is clear that causing unnecessary suffering to an animal whether with an electronic training aid or by any other means is against the law. However, the existing animal welfare legislation is not sufficient to protect animals, not least because it does not prevent the use of static pulse collars. The unnecessary suffering referenced in the Animal Health and Welfare (Scotland) Act 2006 is a subjective concept which is potentially difficult to prove. It is also difficult to prove psychological damage which cannot always be seen easily. As stated previously, most users of such devices are unlikely to intend to cause their pet to suffer but would have been misled by how the devices are marketed. They may therefore not have deliberately caused suffering, and the suffering may not be obvious to an enforcement officer because even within a single breed, dogs have been shown to have a variable capacity for coping with aversive stimuli (Vincent & Mitchell, 2006). Impact of regulation on businesses and local authorities 3 in 10 respondents to the most recent Scottish Government consultation on electronic training aids thought their business would be affected by a ban or stricter regulations on remote training static pulse collars. However, the most frequently identified possible effect was dealing with fewer animals suffering from the negative effects of having been trained with an electronic training aid. Many of these respondents stressed that they would be delighted to see any reduction in business which results from banning or regulating electronic training aids. Other effects identified included loss of sales or loss of busiess for those trainers who use shock collars, of which there are a very small number based on Scotland. Estimates are between 20 and 100. If they were effective dog trainers, they would continue to be able to train dogs using positive training methods or even less aversive training methods What evidence is there that shock collars are harmful? Scientific evidence Prior to studies paid for by Defra and carried out by the University of Lincoln, there was already a lot of evidence to show the harmful effects of using shock collars on dogs. Studies have focused on the physiological effects, psychological effects and the impacts on learning through the use of electric shock collars. The following studies have all indicated the negative impact that electric shock collars can have on the welfare of the animal. Tsevtkov et al. (2002), Sang et al. (2003) and Lindsay (2005) concluded that electric shock collars can cause high levels of distress and emotional harm to dogs and further contribute 4

to intense anxiety disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder which makes fears instilled in animals resistant to elimination. Schilder et al. (2004) found signs of stress, high-pitched yelps, barks and squeals, avoidance and redirected aggression as well as a continued display of stress when there was no electric shock collar on the dog while in the company of their handler. The researchers concluded that shock collar training is stressful, receiving shocks is a painful experience to dogs and that the shock group of dogs evidently learned that the presence of their owner announced the reception of shocks, even outside of the normal training context. Hilby et al. (2004) studied the effectiveness of positive and negative training methods and concluded that the results showed when using rewards, the incidence of problematic behaviours, including aggression toward people and other dogs, fear, repetitive behaviours, overexcitement, anxiety and separation issues were greatly reduced in comparison to when the electric shock collar was used. Reisner (2003) examined aggression in dogs and the author argued that in order to reduce aggression, all circumstances, provocations, and aversive interactions associated with the dog s aggression need to be avoided. Many aggressive dogs are anxious or fearful, and punishment of any kind should be avoided. The author states that aversive tools such as electric shocks can increase anxiety and therefore increase the risk of biting; in addition, they are likely to lead to treatment failure. The findings of Defra-funded studies published in 2010 and 2011 (AW1402 and AW1402a) are also significant. The first Defra project concluded that there was great variability in how electric shock collars were used on dogs and showed that owners worryingly tended to either not read or follow the advice in the manuals. The main conclusion was that there were significant negative welfare consequences for some of the dogs that were trained with electric shock collars in the study. The second study was designed to use electric shock collars on dogs by trained professionals according to industry standards. For this reason, the Electronic Collar Manufacturers Association (ECMA) were asked to design both the training protocol as well as recommend industry trained professionals to take part in the study. The study concluded that even when the collars were used by professionals following an industry set standard, there were still long term negative impacts on dog welfare. Lastly, the studies also demonstrated that positive reinforcement methods were effective in treating livestock chasing, which is the most commonly cited justification of their use. Even with best practice as advocated by collar manufacturers and trainers, there were differences in the behaviour of dogs that are consistent with more negative emotional states (including anxiety and aversion) in some dogs trained with e-collars.further, the results indicate that there is no statistically significant nor clinically relevant differences in the efficacy of an e-collar training protocol combined with rewards and a reward based programme that does not use an e-collar for the management of dogs presented with comparable levels of livestock chasing, which is one of the most commonly advocated justifications for the necessity of e-collar training. The academics from the University of Lincoln who conducted the Defra study subsequently conducted some re-analysis as a result of drawing on studies not available at the time of the original project. The findings have been published in a peer reviewed, scientific journal but this has not been publicised by Defra and cannot be found on the Defra website. The published article concludes: 5

(Accordingly), it seems that the routine use of e-collars even in accordance with best practice (as suggested by collar manufacturers) presents a risk to the well-being of pet dogs. Dogs showed a number of additional changes in behaviour in the period following electric stimulus presentation, compared with behaviour prior to stimulus presentation. Dogs showed an increase in vocalisation, with none recorded prior to first stimulus compared to a total of 13 yelps and 5 whines after exposure. There was a change in tail carriage from principally an elevated carriage prior to exposure (with only 2% of time was the tail between legs) to the tail being between legs 20% of the time following exposure. Prior to stimulus application the dogs were generally described as being in a neutral (40% of time) or investigatory (20%) state with only 10% of time described as tense; whereas afterwards, dogs were tense for 50% of the time and spent only 5% of their time engaged in investigatory behaviour. A small number of yawns and paw lifts were observed after stimuli, but none seen before exposure. Bouts of lip licking and body shaking were recorded before and after exposure at approximately the same rate. Taken together (with the findings of the preliminary study), these results are consistent with exposure to a significant short term stressor in the form of an aversive and probably painful stimulus during training. (The Welfare Consequences and Efficacy of Training Pet Dogs with Remote Electronic Training Collars in Comparison to Reward Based Training, Jonathan J. Cooper, Nina Cracknell, Jessica Hardiman, Hannah Wright, Daniel Mills, PLOS, September 3, 2014). In the later study by the University of Lincoln, three out of four randomly selected professional dog trainers did not follow manufacturer s best practice. Therefore it is reasonable to expect that a significant proportion of shock collar users do not follow ideal practice. This reinforces our view that shock collars should not only be banned, but that even working with the industry to draw up guidance for dog owners and trainers to advise how to use e- collars properly would be unwise given the inconsistencies in how even professional dog trainers use them, and also that they are not necessary in dog training in the first instance. The ECMA (and to date, the Scottish Government), have effectively, written off the above research as highly subjective and ill-informed opinion and hearsay which does not recognise the technical capabilities and safety of the latest generation of quality modern-day electronic collar products. They concluded that a legislative ban would create a problem for all those involved and would be counterintuitive to the well-being of dogs. However they have published materials outlining when it is appropriate to use electronic training aids and how to use them in a way that is safe, supervised and effective. This includes for instances of dog fouling. Anecdotal evidence There is much anecdotal evidence that has been presented to the Scottish Government from dog owners who claim to have saved their dogs by using an electric shock collar. We do not believe this evidence can be verified. Afterall, those who opted to use a shock collar for their intended purpose will not know the psychological harm they may have caused their dog in doing so, and would not have taken their dog to any of the hundreds of dog trainers or behaviourists based in Scotland who claim to be able to train any dog using positive training techniques (i.e. members of the organisations listed previously). 6

Animal trainers, animal behaviourist and animal welfare organisations were amongst those making these reports. In terms of the impact on the dogs concerned, there were reports of: Anxiety-related behaviour or panic responses to seeing a collar or hearing the noise associated with their use. It was suggested that many users will increase the level of stimulation if they do not achieve immediate results and that this often results in the animal attempting to escape or avoid the stimulus. It was also suggested that animals may fail to show a pain response despite increased levels of electronic stimulation or may become habituated to the pain and endure it. It was noted that the pain and stress caused in such situations has a significant effect on an animal s physiology, increasing cortisol levels and heart rate. Dogs shutting down psychologically, including global suppression of behaviour or learned helplessness. It was suggested that this is frequently mistaken for an animal being trained, as the animal is subdued and tends not to act or react. In extreme cases, it was suggested that animals may refuse to perform any behaviour learned helplessness and will isolate themselves to avoid incurring electronic stimulation. Re-directed aggression towards other dogs, their owner or members of the public. It was suggested that animals may suppress aggression which may resurface at any time, without warning and generally in a more severe form. More specifically, it was reported that using electronic stimulation to reduce behaviours such as barking, lunging and growling may simply suppress the behaviour which could warn of more serious imminent behaviour such as biting. It was further suggested that people and other animals will have no warning before the animal subjected to punishment feels forced to bite. Physical injuries to the animal and to the neck in particular. Further anecdotal evidence was presented to a court in 2001, in a dangerous dogs case after Ostarra Langridge was prosecuted when one of her dogs attacked and killed another dog whilst on a walk. A control order, rather than a destruction order, was imposed as the magistrates accepted the defense that Miss Langridge's dog's aggressive behaviour was attributable to the effects of the shock collar. Miss Langridge sought the help of a behaviourist when her dogs started to run away from her on their walks along the beach. The dogs were given shock collars, which Miss Langridge was told to keep on for three months and activate whenever they misbehaved. But the first time the dogs got a shock was by mistake, after a small dog they were walking past made Miss Langridge jump. From then on her pets associated the shocks with small dogs and became afraid of them. When Miss Langridge described the day in July when her dogs turned on a Shih Tzu she had tears in her eyes. She stated "They connected the pain of the electric shock with little dogs because of the first time I used the collar. The day that machine came in this house I regret." (The Argus, Collars Turned Dogs into Killers). Furthermore, in a written submission to the Scottish Parliament Public Petitions Committee in May 2015 (post-publication of the Defra research), the Electronic Collar Manufacturers Association stated that even in their view, electronic shock training devices should only be used under qualified supervision. Acknowledging that their misuse that has given rise to incidents which have raised concerns for all animal welfare stakeholders including reputable collar manufacturers and retailers. How could regulations permitting restricted use work? It is currently understood that the Scottish Government wants to work with the Electronic Shock Collar Manufacturers Association (NB: not the welfare, veterinary or leading training and behaviour organisations) to regulate the use of electric shock collars. One suggestion for regulating the use of shock collars may be to limit the level of shock that the collars could 7

emit however this has not been discussed (as far as we are aware) because it is the case that electric shock collars must hurt in order to work. Another suggestion has been to develop a Scottish Vocational Qualifitcation for trainers who wish to use shock collars. This would effectively legitimise and normalise a practice that is not only widely discredited, but is in fact banned in other parts of the world even other parts of the UK (explained previously). This is a suggestion that we believe the Scottish Government is considering, even though it has not been consulted on. We do not believe this will help, and may even make the situation worse for the following reasons: ECMA members have already signed up to a strict code of practice under which all products must meet the latest technical requirements and be provided with user guides with consistent instructions yet there is no evidence to suggest that this has improved the way in which the devices are used or that they can be varied to suit the different needs of different dogs. As with most tools, the handler s ability can help determine the effect of the aversive stimulus; however, each dog is an individual and the response to an aversive stimulus will be different for each dog. A low-level electrical stimulus for one dog can be entirely different for another. It may be difficult for a novice, or as proved by the research stated above, even an experienced professional, to determine exactly what effect the collar is having on a dog if they are not aware of the subtleties of canine communication signals. (Electronic Training Devices: A Review of Current Literature Jo Jacques, CPDT, CPCT and Sandy Myers, CDBC 2007). Further, in the later study by the University of Lincoln, three out of four randomly selected professional dog trainers did not follow manufacturer s best practice. Therefore it is reasonable to expect that a significant proportion of shock collar users do not follow ideal practice. This is why we believe that banning these devices to the clearest and most enforceable means of addressing this issue. 8