Brain gene regulation by territorial singing behavior in freely ranging songbirds

Similar documents
Motor-driven gene expression

ZENK Protein Regulation by Song in the Brain of Songbirds

For Whom The Bird Sings: Context-Dependent Gene Expression

Gonads and Singing Play Separate, Additive Roles in New Neuron Recruitment in Adult Canary Brain

state. Results presented here are from birds hatched during the spring of Eggs were marked on the day of laying,

Localized Changes in Immediate-Early Gene Regulation during Sensory and Motor Learning in Zebra Finches

BRIEF REPORT A Brain of Her Own: A Neural Correlate of Song Assessment in a Female Songbird

Accurate Heat Detection with Health Monitoring

Sexual dimorphism in song-induced ZENK expression in the medial striatum of juvenile zebra finches

stable (11). This work also reported that the size of HVC mechanisms behind this growth were not determined. Thus,

DEVELOPMENTAL PLASTICITY IN NEURAL CIRCUITS FOR A LEARNED BEHAVIOR

Conspecific and Heterospecific Song Discrimination in Male Zebra Finches with Lesions in the Anterior Forebrain Pathway

C ellulitis is a superficial spreading infection of the skin.

Neural Constraints on the Complexity of Avian Song

Stephanie M. Dloniak* and Pierre Deviche*,1

The Role of Auditory Experience in the Formation of Neural Circuits Underlying Vocal Learning in Zebra Finches

Arrhythmic Song Exposure Increases ZENK Expression in Auditory Cortical Areas and Nucleus Taeniae of the Adult Zebra Finch

NEURAL RESPONSES TO AUDITORY RHYTHMS IN THE ZEBRA FINCH. Jennifer A. Lampen

Disconnection of a Basal Ganglia Circuit in Juvenile Songbirds Attenuates the Spectral Differentiation of Song Syllables

Genesis and Death of Vocal Control Neurons During Sexual Differentiation in the Zebra Finch

Title. CitationPLoS One, 7(8): e Issue Date Doc URL. Rights(URL) Type.

Estimation of the transmission of foot-and-mouth disease virus from infected sheep to cattle

An Analysis of the Neural Representation of Birdsong Memory

AGRICULTURE, LIVESTOCK AND FISHERIES

Sexually Dimorphic Neurocalcin Expression in the Developing Zebra Finch Telencephalon

Male song quality affects circulating but not yolk steroid concentrations in female canaries (Serinus canaria)

Sex Difference in the Size of the Neural Song Control Regions in a Dueting Songbird with Similar Song Repertoire Size of Males and Females

Context determines the sex appeal of male zebra finch song

Interactions Between Nerve Growth Factor Binding and Estradiol in Early Development of the Zebra Finch Telencephalon

NEUROENDOCRINE CONTROL OF SONG IN THE DARK-EYED JUNCO {JUNCO H YEM A LIS) Stephanie Marie Dloniak

Cultures, genes, and neurons in the development of song and singing in brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater )

A Comparative Study of the Behavioral Deficits following Lesions of Various Parts of the Zebra Finch Song System: Implications for Vocal Learning

INTRODUCTION MATERIALS AND METHODS

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

University of Cape Town

RENGGANIS PRANANDARI 1, SUDIBYO SUPARDI 2, RETNOSARI ANDRAJATI 1 *

Evolution of Mating system: A Game Theory

Testosterone Regulates the Activity and Expression of Aromatase in the Canary Neostriatum

Visual and song nuclei correlate with courtship skills in brown-headed cowbirds

Behavioural Processes

Early Condition, Song Learning, and the Volume of Song Brain Nuclei in the Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata)

Development of hippocampal specialisation in a food-storing bird

I EUNCLASSIFIEDUNLIMITED 0 SAME AS RPT. D OTIC USERS Unclaifi r 22a. NAME OF RIPOSIBLE INDIVIDUAL 22b. TELEPHNE (nclude Area Code) 22c.

1.14 Infanticide by a male lion. Bad fathers in wild life

RESEARCH AND PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE

The effects of Corticosterone on Neuronal Migration in Zebra Finches

Sexual Differentiation of the Zebra Finch Song System Parallels Genetic, Not Gonadal, Sex

Breeding Ecology of the Ferruginous Hawk in Northern Utah and Southern Idaho

Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 Expression in Songbird and Human Brain Predicts Functional Interaction

HELMINTHS OF THE WILD BOAR (SUS SCROFA L.) IN NATURAL AND BREEDING CONDITIONS

A Monoclonal Antibody Specific to a Song System Nuclear Antigen in Estrildine Finches

Rudimentary substrates for vocal learning in a suboscine

A SINGLE VIBRISSAL COLUMN IN THE FIRST SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX OF THE MOUSE DEMONSTRATED WITH 2-DEOXYGLUCOSE

Melanie F. Guigueno, 1,2 David F. Sherry, 1,2,3 Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton 1,2,3 ABSTRACT: INTRODUCTION

Failure to Detect Seasonal Changes in the Song System Nuclei of the Black-Capped Chickadee (Poecile atricapillus)

Original Article. Elham Memary 1, Alireza Mirkheshti 1, Ali Dabbagh 2, Mehrdad Taheri 1, Aida Khadempour 1, and Sadegh Shirian 3,4 *

Efficacy of forearm tourniquet for local intravenous regional anesthesia in bilateral hand surgery

Research Thesis. by Nathaniel J. Sackinger. The Ohio State University June 2013

The S Files Success with Maria: Sunshine: Biting Reported by S.G. Friedman, PhD and L. McGuire

Song Learning, Early Nutrition and Sexual Selection in Songbirds 1

Bedaquiline in multidrug-resistant pulmonary tuberculosis GENESIS-SEFH drug evaluation report*

Neural Pathways for Bilateral Vocal Control in Songbirds

SOAR Research Proposal Summer How do sand boas capture prey they can t see?

A. Pulse-field gel of hummingbird genomic DNA. B. Bioanalyzer plot of hummingbird SMRTbell library

Supplementary Information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALS AND METHODS

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Lateralization of neural control for vocalization by the frog (Rana pipiens)

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Territorial signals. Design rules for territorial signals. Why defend a territory? Bird song and territory defense

Vocal Matching and Intensity of Begging Calls Are Associated with a Forebrain Song Circuit in a Generalist Brood Parasite

ZENK Activation in the Nidopallium of Black-Capped Chickadees in Response to Both Conspecific and Heterospecific Calls

The Efficacy and Safety of Rapid Antigen Detection Test in Adults: A Retrospective Study

Chapter 16: Evolution Lizard Evolution Virtual Lab Honors Biology. Name: Block: Introduction

What causes lizards brains to change size?

Song Function and the Evolution of Female Preferences

The metabolic cost of birdsong production

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

Supporting Online Material for

Behaviour of cats and dogs

HOUSE FINCHES, CARPODACUS MEXICANUS: HORMONES, STRESS, AND SONG CONTROL REGIONS. A Thesis. presented to

Punnett Squares. and Pedigrees. How are patterns of inheritance studied? Lesson ESSENTIAL QUESTION. J S7L3.b Reproduction and genetic variation

March to mid May: Mid May to late June:

DEVELOPMENT OF THE HEAD AND NECK PLACODES

Author's personal copy

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified):

Wire(less)tapping in the Aviary

by John Aaron Howell Oxford May 2017 Approved by Advisor: Dr. Lainy Day Reader: Dr. Chris Leary Reader: Dr. Debra Young

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Electrocommunication. Electrocommunication. Weakly electric fish. Electric organs and electroreceptors

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar

THE AUK A QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF ORNITHOLOGY. VOL. 102 JULY 1985 No. 3 DIFFERENCES IN REPERTOIRE SIZE, SINGING BEHAVIOR, AND ASSOCIATED NEUROANATOMY

Species must be adapted to their habitat.

Dacnis cayana (Blue Dacnis or Turquoise Honeycreeper)

The contralateral impairment of the orienting ocular-following reflex after lesions of the lateral suprasylvian cortex in cats

NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF HYGIENE AND EPIDEMIOLOGY BUI VAN TUAN

Weekly Schedule of Neuroscience (2017/2018) Week 1

Developmental stress affects song learning but not song complexity and vocal amplitude in zebra finches

Texas Quail Index. Result Demonstration Report 2016

Food preference and copying behaviour in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata

Sexual preferences for mate song in female canaries (Serinus canaria)

Transcription:

Neuroethology NeuroReort 8, 2073 2077 (997) TO investigate the ecological relevance of brain gene regulation associated with singing behavior in songbirds, we challenged freely ranging song sarrows with consecific song laybacks within their breeding territories. Males resonded by aroaching the seaker, searching for an intruder and actively singing. In situ hybridization of brain sections revealed significantly higher exression of the transcritional regulator ZENK in challenged birds than in unstimulated controls in several auditory structures and song control nuclei. We conclude that singing behavior in the context of territorial defense is associated with gene regulation in brain centers that control song ercetion and roduction, and that behaviorally regulated gene exression can be used to investigate brain areas involved in the natural behaviors of freely ranging animals. Key words: Avian; Immediate early gene; Learning; Neuroethology; Vocal communication Brain gene regulation by territorial singing behavior in freely ranging songbirds Erich D. Jarvis, Hubert Schwabl, Sidarta Ribeiro and Claudio V. Mello CA The Rockerfeller University, Laboratory of Animal Behavior, 230 York Ave, Box 37, New York, NY 002; Field Research Center, Tyrrel Road, Millbrook, NY 2545 Present Address: Washington State University Deartment of Zoology, Pullman, WA 9964-4236, USA CA Corresonding Author Introduction The question of whether brain regulatory mechanisms studied in the laboratory are of ecological relevance is often neglected by investigators with a reductionistic aroach to animal behavior. To address this issue, one needs to study animals in their natural setting, while they exerience interactions with other individuals in the environment to which they are adated. The henomenon of brain gene regulation associated with ercetion and roduction of song in songbirds has recently generated some intriguing insights into the hysiology of avian vocal communication. 6 When songbirds hear novel consecific songs exression of the immediate early genes ZENK and c-jun is raidly induced in their brains.,2,7 This resonse is most rominent in the caudo-medial neostriatum (NCM) and hyerstriatum ventrale (CMHV), 8 areas of the avian telencehalon believed to corresond to arts of the mammalian auditory cortex. 9,0 In contrast, when songbirds sing, exression of ZENK is induced in nuclei of the motor athway for song roduction, 6 including the high vocal center (HVC) and the robust nucleus of the archistriatum (RA).,2 This singing-induced exression is indeendent of auditory feedback, as it still occurs in deafened males when they sing. 6 The studies described above were erformed under controlled laboratory conditions with domesticated secies bred in cativity. We wondered whether it was technically feasible to study behaviorally regulated changes in brain gene exression in animals in their natural habitat and if so, whether the results in the field differed from those obtained in the laboratory. To address these issues, we studied freely ranging song sarrows, a highly territorial secies in which males resent a robust and rather redictable behavioral resonse when challenged with an intruder s song within their territories during the breeding season. 3,4 We show here that challenged males dislaying this territorial defense have a marked induction of ZENK in several brain regions that control song ercetion and roduction. This induction attern is similar to that obtained with cative secies, but there are also some significant differences which robably reflect the comlexities of the field setting. Materials and Methods Animals: Freely ranging male (n = 7) and female (n = 5) song sarrows (Melosiza melodia) were catured within their natural breeding territories at the Rockefeller University Field Research Center (Millbrook, New York), between the months of Aril and June in 993 and 996. Behavioral aradigm: Breeding territories were initially located by observing various candidate areas where song sarrows were seen flying or singing Raid Science Publishers Vol 8 No 8 27 May 997 2073

within the Field Center. After the territories were determined, the birds were observed for their referred erching sites therein, usually a bush or small tree frequently visited by the male. A seaker and folded mist nets were then strategically ositioned at these sites, and the birds natural behaviors were observed for eriods of one to several hours, without any disturbance or stimulation. After the observation eriod, males were challenged (n = 4) with lay-backs of tae-recorded consecific song for 30 min, to simulate the resence of an intruder within their breeding territories. The tae consisted of a 4 s song bout, reeated every 0 s, obtained from our collection of songs recorded at the Field Center. Immediately after the layback the nets were ulled u with attached strings from a distance of about 20 m by observers hidden behind bushes or small trees. Since challenged birds tended to stay in the vicinity of the seaker (see Results), they were tyically caught within the first 2 3 min after oening the nets. After cature, they were removed from the nets and killed by decaitation. Their brains were quickly dissected out while in the field, laced in a lastic mold, covered with embedding medium (TissueTek), frozen in a dry ice/ethanol bath, transorted to the laboratory and ket at 70ºC. Sex was confirmed by insection of the gonads. Control males were not resented with laybacks. Those that sang less than five song bouts within the last hour of the observation eriod were catured (n = 3) by ulling u the nets, and their brains were rocessed as described above for the challenged birds. As is commonly known for many songbirds, we noticed a high frequency of singing behavior in the early morning hours that decreased with time of day and increasing ambient temerature. The birds tended to be quieter during the hottest hours of the day (between.00 and 6.00 h), resuming active singing in the late afternoon. We therefore concentrated our efforts on erforming the exeriments during this quiet interval, to minimize brain levels of ZENK message due to this unavoidable level of natural activity. In situ hybridization: Gene exression was analyzed by in situ hybridization, using a reviously described rotocol. 8 Serial arasagittal brain sections (0 m thick, u to 3.5 mm from the midline) were cut on a cryostat, mounted onto TESPA-coated (Fluka) glass slides, and stored at 70ºC, 35 S-labeled antisense riborobes were synthesized from the cloned canary homologs of the ZENK 8 and HAT-2 5 genes. The robes were then hybridized to brain sections of the catured song sarrows and of canaries (Serinus canaria) and zebra finches (Taenioygia gutatta) from our laboratory collection after fixation of sections with 3% araformaldehyde. 5 Hybridization and washing stes were carried out at 65ºC. Sections were then exosed to X-ray film for 2 3 weeks, or to a PhoshorImager screen for 2 3 days and then died in autoradiograhic emulsion (NTB-2, Kodak) and exosed for 6 weeks. Died slides were then develoed and counterstained with cresyl violet. As a control for background levels, some birds sections were hybridized with ZENK sense riborobes. Quantification: ZENK exression was quantified by counting silver grains over cells in various brain regions using NIH s Image software, as detailed reviously. 6 For each area, the values obtained in the challenged grou were divided by the mean value obtained in the control grou to generate a normalized scale. The significance of differences between grous was determined by unaired Student s t-test. Results E. D. Jarvis et al. Behavior: Challenged males showed a characteristic resonse 3,4 that consisted of aroaching the seaker and searching actively for the intruder in the grass or behind leaves or on branches in the bushes located near the seaker. During the challenge, the birds engaged in intense singing activity (range: 40 200 bouts within 30 min), most often at two or three referred sites around the area where the seaker was laced. The frequency of countersinging increased with time, articularly 5 0 min after the onset of the layback. Occasionally, a bird would fly away from the immediate vicinity for a few minutes, after which he would return to the area and, in some cases, change his song to later revert back to the original song. Females were more elusive: whenever they tried to aroach the seaker they would be chased back into nearby bushes or out of the immediate vicinity by the defending male. Thus, we were able to cature five unstimulated females, when they haened to fly into the mist nets during control sessions, but no stimulated females. Gene exression: All challenged birds showed increased ZENK exression in several brain regions (Fig. b,d) when comared with unstimulated controls (Fig. a,c). Prominent increases occurred in NCM and CMHV (Fig. b), and less ronounced induction in fields L and L3, the caudo-dorsal aleostriatum (Pc), the HVC shelf and the RA cu (Fig. d). All of the above are art of the central auditory athways in songbirds 9,0 and are known to show exression of ZENK in zebra finches and canaries exosed to novel consecific song. 8 High levels of 2074 Vol 8 No 8 27 May 997

Brain gene regulation in wild song sarrows FIG.. ZENK induction in the brain of male song sarrows: dark-field views of arasagittal sections at the 0.2 mm (a,b) and 2.0 mm (c,d) lanes hybridized with an 35 S-labeled ZENK riborobe and counterstained with cresyl violet. Notice increased ZENK exression (white silver grains) in auditory and song control areas in challenged birds (b,d), comared with controls (a,c). The diagrams on the right reresent the regions shown on the sections. Not indicated are the shelf area immediately ventral to HVC and the cu region adjacent to RA, as described elsewhere. 8 0 Orientation: dorsal is u and rostral to the right. BS, brain stem; Cb, cerebellum; CMHV, caudo-medial hyerstriatum ventrale; H, hiocamus; HA, hyerstriatum accessorium; HV, hyerstriatum ventrale; HVC, high vocal center; L, L2 and L3, subfields of auditory field L; LPO, lobus araolfactorius; LMAN and mman. lateral and medial magnocellular nuclei of the anterior neostriatum; N, neostriatum; NCM, caudo-medial neostriatum; Pc, caudo-dorsal aleostriatum; RA, robust nucleus of the archistriatum. Bar = 2 mm. FIG. 2. Quantification of ZENK and HAT-2 exression in song sarrow brains. Bars reresent mean (± s.e.m.) of silver grain counts over reresentative auditory (NCM) and song control (HVC, area X) regions in challenged birds, normalized to average control values for each area. Significant differences were seen for NCM and HVC (* < 0.008; unaired Student s t-test), but not for area X. Exression of ZENK in LPO (ventral to area X) and exression of HAT-2 were not modulated. HVC, high vocal center; LPO, lobus araolfactorius; NCM, caudomedial neostriatum; X, area X of the aleostriatum. induced ZENK exression were also found in nuclei HVC, RA, and mman (the medial magnocellular nucleus of the anterior neostriatum) of all challenged birds (Fig. b,d). These nuclei are art of the song control system,2 and are known to exress ZENK in resonse to singing. 6 As shown in Fig. 2a, there was a 3- to 6-fold induction in challenged birds, according to the region analyzed The largest induction was seen in HVC. In contrast, ZENK induction in song nuclei of the anterior forebrain was variable: Vol 8 No 8 27 May 997 2075

ZENK exression in area X was very high in one challenged male (Fig. d), low in another male, and undetectable in the other two males (not shown), whereas exression in lman (the lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior neostriatum) was either low (Fig. d) or undetectable in all males. Lower and more variable levels of ZENK exression occurred in the rostral neostriatum and hyerstriatum, and in the cerebellum, but no consistent differences were seen between the two grous. Other brain regions such as LPO (lobus araolfactorius) excluding area X (Fig. 2a), hiocamus and thalamus showed no differences in ZENK exression. In addition, no differences for any brain regions analyzed were seen in exression levels of HAT-2 (Fig. 2b), a forebrain-enriched mrna 5 known to be constitutively exressed. 5 ZENK exression throughout the telencehalon, in articular in the neo- and hyerstriatum, was relatively higher in unstimulated controls (Fig. a,c) than in the laboratory control zebra finches and canaries that were hybridized in arallel (not shown). This higher basal exression in the field resumably reflects the activity levels exerienced by the birds rior to their cature. ZENK exression was very low or absent in the hiocamus and arahiocamal region, telencehalic laminae, most of the brain stem, ventricular zone, meninges and choroid leux. Exression atterns were similar in unstimulated males and females. No signal was detected in sections hybridized to the ZENK sense robe. Discussion The resent study clearly demonstrates ZENK induction in brain areas controlling vocal communication when freely ranging male song sarrows engage in singing behavior in the context of territorial defense. This indicates that the ZENK induction reviously described in laboratory animals in association with vocal communication,6 is of relevance to the natural life of songbirds. A comarison with laboratory exeriments erformed with cative canaries and zebra finches is extremely helful in the interretation of the resent results. In those birds, ZENK induction in areas such as NCM, CMHV and fields L and L3 occurred in both females and males when they heard laybacks of consecific song. 8 This induction occurred in the absence of singing 8 and was abolished in deafened males, whether or not they sang. 6 In contrast, ZENK exression in song control nuclei such as HVC, RA, area X, lman and mman occurred only in laboratory males that sang, irresective of hearing. 6 We conclude that the exression attern obtained in the E. D. Jarvis et al. field is exlained by activation of areas controlling ercetual and motor asects of song, largely in accordance with laboratory studies. ZENK exression in the forebrain of control males catured in the field was generally higher than that in controls from laboratory studies,,6,8 but still significantly lower than in challenged birds. This is an imortant oint to stress, since the birds studied here were not laced in controlled chambers, in searation from other birds. Rather, they were exeriencing interactions with other individuals and the comlexities of an ever-changing environment, and could thus exhibit a full flair of natural behaviors. This fact, however, did not revent us from observing a significant increase in ZENK exression levels in the brain. The resent field results differ from revious studies in another resect: there was considerable variation in exression levels of two nuclei of the anterior forebrain song athway, area X and lman. The occurrence of ZENK exression in these areas in singing adult males 6 is very intriguing, given the current understanding that these nuclei are necessary for song learning in juveniles but not for song roduction in adults. 6 8 The variability observed in the field indicates the existence of other as yet unidentified behavioural and/or contextual variables that, in combination with singing, determine ZENK exression levels in the anterior forebrain athway, as reviously hyothesized. 6 The ZENK gene encodes a transcritional regulator 9 and its activation in other systems has been linked to neuronal lasticity, in articular with induction of hiocamal long-term otentiation, or LTP. 20 22 This raises the ossibility that ZENK induction in the context of song ercetion and roduction is also associated with long-term neuronal modification, and could thus have a rofound influence on the organization of the brain circuitry resonsible for song ercetion and roduction. Interestingly, auditory resonses in NCM show song-secific habituation whose maintenance requires RNA and rotein synthesis at discrete time windows after song resentation. 3,4 ZENK induction kinetics aroximately coincides with the first such window, 6,8 and ZENK is a likely candidate for an early coordinator of long-term habituation. Irresective of the final answer to the question above, it is clear that ZENK can be successfully used to investigate brain areas activated by natural behaviors in field exeriments. This finding should be articularly useful for situations in which the behavior(s) of interest are only dislayed in a natural setting, and the exerimental situation cannot be reduced to a laboratory context. The use of ZENK induction for functional brain maing rovides an adequate tool for erforming such exeriments. 2076 Vol 8 No 8 27 May 997

Brain gene regulation in wild song sarrows Conclusion This study demonstrates that song roduction and ercetion in the context of songbird territorial defense are associated with gene regulation in brain centers that control vocal communication. More generally, our results show that it is ossible, even simle, to study behaviorally driven gene regulation in freely ranging animals. It should thus be ossible to use brain maing with ZENK to identify regions involved in natural behaviors that cannot be studied in the laboratory. References. Mello CV, Vicario D and Clayton DF. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89, 688 6822 (992). 2. Mello CV, Nottenbohn F and Clayton DF. J Neurosci 5, 699 6925 (995). 3. Chew SJ, Mello CV, Vicario D et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92, 3406 340 (995). 4. Chew SJ, Vicario D and Nottebohm F. Science 274, 909 94 (996). 5. Jarvis EJ, Mello CV and Nottebohm F. Learn Mem 2, 62 80 (995). 6. Jarvis EJ and Nottebohm F. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94, 4097 402 (997). 7. Nastiuk KL, Mello CV, George JM et al. Mol Brain Res 27, 299 309 (994). 8. Mello CV and Clayton DF. J Neurosci 4, 6652 6666 (994). 9. Kelley DB and Nottebohm F. J Com Neurol 83, 455 470 (979). 0. Vates EG, Broome B, Mello CV et al. J Com Neurol 366, 63 642 (996).. Nottebohm F, Stokes T and Leonard CM. J Com Neurol 65, 457 486 (976). 2. Nottebohm F, Kelley DB and Paton JA. J Com Neurol 207, 344 357 (982). 3. Searcy WA, McArthur PD, Peters SS et al. Behavior 77, 52 63 (98). 4. Wingfield JC. Horm Behav 9, 74 87 (985). 5. George J and Clayton DF. Mol Brain Res 2, 323 329 (992). 6. Bottjer SW, Miesner EA and Arnold AP. Science 224, 90 903 (984). 7. Sohrabji F, Nordeen EJ and Nordeen KW. Behav Neural Biol 53, 5 63 (990). 8. Scharff C and Nottebohm F. J Neurosci, 2896 293 (99). 9. Christy B and Nathans D. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 86, 8737 87 (989). 20. Cole AJ, Saffen DW, Baraban JM et al. Nature 340, 474 476 (989). 2. Wiseden W, Errington ML, Williams S et al. Neuron 4, 603 64 (990). 22. Bliss TVP and Collingridge GL. Nature 36, 3 39 (993). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS: We thank Fernando Nottebohm for his suort and for making the Field Research Center available for this study. This research was suorted with NIMH s grant #2T32MH525-8 (to E.J.), NIDCD #DC02853-0 (to C.V.M.) and #MH49877 (to H.S.), Kluge fellowshis (to S.R. and E.J.) and by the Mary Cary Flagler Charitable Trust. Received 7 March 997; acceted 9 March 997 General Summary This study demonstrates for the first time that it is ossible to study brain gene regulation in animals behaving freely within their natural habitat. Song sarrows were challenged with laybacks of an intruder s song within their breeding territories in a New York State nature reserve. Challenged males showed their tyical territorial defense resonses, searching for the intruder and actively singing. Exression of ZENK, a gene that regulates other genes and may articiate in long-term memory formation, was increased in brain regions controlling song roduction and ercetion. Exression atterns observed in the field were similar to those found in laboratory animals, even in secies bred in cativity for the ast 500 years, i.e. Waterslager canaries. Differences noted were resumably due to variables that occur naturally in the field. These results show that scientists can address molecular brain mechanisms of behavior in freely ranging animals, exeriments once thought only ossible in the laboratory. Vol 8 No 8 27 May 997 2077