To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Similar documents
The Value of Data Gary Patronek & Stephen Zawistowski Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Distressed Animal Behaviors and Some Recommendations for Improvements at the Kuala Lumpur Zoo, Malaysia Amber Haque Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Hsin-Yi Weng a & Lynette A. Hart b a Department of Pathobiology, College of Veterinary

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Aggression Social Aggression to Unfamiliar Dogs

Canine Aggression SIBLING RIVALRY INDIAN HILLS ANIMAL CLINIC. Indian Hills Animal Clinic

Long-Term Outcome After Treatment of Feline Inappropriate Elimination Amy R. Marder & Joan M. Engel Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Proceedings of the European Veterinary Conference Voorjaarsdagen

Departments, Iowa State University, Ames b Department of Population Medicine, Ontario Veterinary College, University of Guelph,

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Canine Aggression Overview of Diagnosis and Treatment

LIVING IN A MULTI- DOG HOUSEHOLD

To cite this article: Emily Weiss & Sandra Wilson (2003) The Use of Classical and

Dog Behavior and Training - Moving with Your Dog

AGGRESSION (CATS) DIAGNOSING AND TREATING

Behavior Modification Why Punishment Should Be Avoided

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression - Sibling Rivalry Treatment

Dog Behavior and Training - Teaching Calm Settle and Relaxation Training

Aggression in Dogs Overview Basics

for Assistance Elise R. Shore a, Charles Burdsal a & Deanna K. Douglas b a Psychology Department, Wichita State University

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression Diagnosis and Overview

American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior: Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals

Carin Wittnich a b & Michael Belanger b a Canadian Veterinary Medical Association, Ottawa,

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Conflict-Related Aggression

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Dominance aggression in dogs: Part 1

Insider's Guide To The Cavalier King Charles Spaniel - The Dog Barking Helper HOW TO MANAGE DOGGY PROBLEMS. Dog Barking Help

Behavior Modification Reinforcement and Rewards

IDENTIFICATION AND MANAGEMENT OF SEPARATION ANXIETY IN DOGS

Guide Dogs Puppy Development and Advice Leaflet. No. 4 Identifying and preventing aggressive behaviours inguide dog puppies

Nova Scotia Service Dog Assessment

Puppy Socialization and Fear Prevention

Dog Behavior Problems Veterinary Visits/Examinations

The S Files Success with Maria: Sunshine: Biting Reported by S.G. Friedman, PhD and L. McGuire

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression Getting Started Safety and Management

Effects of paedomorphosis on signaling behaviors in dyadic encounters of the domestic dog

FELINE SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR

Published online: 28 Apr 2015.

Nicole Cottam a, Nicholas H. Dodman a, Alice A. Moon-Fanelli a & Gary J. Patronek b a Department of Clinical Science, Tufts Cummings

Behavior News THE NEWSLETTER FROM THE ANIMAL BEHAVIOR PROGRAM. Notes on Puppy Socialization. Behavior Textbook Review/Recommendation

Is dog aggression a problem in Aboriginal communities?

Dog Behavior Problems House Soiling

To choke or not to choke How positive reinforcement has affected the use of choke collars in dog training

b. A description of the animal s signalment (sex, breed, age, size, medical conditions), household composition and current living environment.

For Pet Parents and Pet Professionals

Dog Behavior Problems Barking and Training Quiet

Visual Reward/Correction. Verbal Reward/Correction. Physical Reward/Correction

Demi s Animal Rescue, Inc. Terms of Adoption (Dog) Animal s Name: Breed: Sex: Weight: Age: Microchip ID: Notes:

Desensitization and Counter Conditioning

Puppy Development. Part One

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Proceedings of the World Small Animal Veterinary Association Mexico City, Mexico 2005

Puppy Behavior and Training Handling and Food Bowl Exercises

OBSERVATION AND INFERENCE CRITICAL THINKING ACTIVITY

VOLUNTEER INFORMATION

Management of bold wolves

Proceedings of the Southern European Veterinary Conference - SEVC -

Backgrounder: Dog Behaviour and Social Structure

Appendix for Mortality resulting from undesirable behaviours in dogs aged under three years. attending primary-care veterinary practices in the UK

Puppy Behavior and Training Handling and Food Bowl Exercises

Nervous and aggressive cats

Golden Rule Training

!"#$%&'()*&+,)-,)."#/')!,)0#/') 1/2)3&'45)."#+"/5%&6)7/,-,$,8)9::;:<;<=)>6+#-"?!

Evaluation of XXXXXXX mixed breed male dog

Aggression and social structure

CHILDREN AND PETS How is my pet likely to respond to the new arrival?

ADOPTION POLICIES AND FEES PLEASE READ CAREFULLY BEFORE COMPLETING ADOPTION APPLICATION

Selecting Shelter Dogs for Service Dog Training Emily Weiss Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Housetraining Your Adopted Dog

Guide Dogs Puppy Development and Advice Leaflet. No. 3 Relief routines

Housing Density and Growth in Juvenile Red- Eared Turtles Scott P. McRobert Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Territorial signals. Design rules for territorial signals. Why defend a territory? Bird song and territory defense

Dog Bite Prevention Handout written by Steph Callahan

Dogs. Bite Prevention. For People Who in the Course of Their Work, Meet Dogs

Foster Application. Facebook.com/furrytailendingscaninerescue us at Susan Daniele, President

Amelia J. Cook a & Emily McCobb a a Center for Animals and Public Policy, Cummings

An Argument against Breed Specific Legislation

Member Benefits: One Year Membership: $40.00 Dog Wild Access Pass for unlimited access to the dog park during general-use hours.

Behaviour of cats and dogs

Brian George 1. GENTLE + DAINTY DOGS. Very mutual play Relatively quiet Frequently starts and stops play Easily becomes nervous

Separation Anxiety Syndrome

Demi s Animal Rescue Foster Agreement (Dog)

Ethological perspectives MAN MEETS WOLF. Jane M. Packard, Texas A&M University Canine Science Forum Lorenz (1953)

Dog Surrender Profile

What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of

Re: Proposed Revision To the Nonessential Experimental Population of the Mexican Wolf

Published online: 24 Sep 2008.

German Pinscher Club of America Rescue. (GPCA Rescue)

The communicative relationship between human and dog

Adopting a rescue dog

Skills Assessment Form VTS-Behavior

Be Doggone Smart at Work

Evolution of Dog. Celeste, Dan, Jason, Tyler

Services for Students with Disabilities Interpreting Services. Assistance Animal Policy

Daycare Application Form

Transcription:

This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 08 June 2015, At: 10:22 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/haaw20 The Concept of Dominance and the Treatment of Aggression in Multidog Homes: A Comment on van Kerkhove's Commentary Petra A. Mertens Published online: 04 Jun 2010. To cite this article: Petra A. Mertens (2004) The Concept of Dominance and the Treatment of Aggression in Multidog Homes: A Comment on van Kerkhove's Commentary, Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 7:4, 287-291, DOI: 10.1207/ s15327604jaws0704_8 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0704_8 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL WELFARE SCIENCE, 7(4), 287 291 Copyright 2004, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Concept of Dominance and the Treatment of Aggression in Multidog Homes: A Comment on van Kerkhove s Commentary Petra A. Mertens College of Veterinary Medicine University of Minnesota The comparison of the behavior and the behavioral problems of domestic dogs to the behavior of wolves leads to a variety of misperceptions and problems in clinical behavior practice. Often it is assumed that dogs establish stable, mostly linear, hierarchies with other dogs in the home as well as human family members. In this context, the term dominance is used indiscriminately and synonymously with aggression. The concept of dominance may represent an interesting hypothesis or model that helps in understanding some behaviors, but this approach tends to ignore that the social behavior of the dog is complex and has been altered through domestication. Dogs show a disintegration of ritualized aggression that normally will serve to resolve conflict, diminishing the risk of injury for group members. The provision of food negating the need for group hunting decreases the selective advantage for formation of stable groups in domestic dogs who live in close contact with humans (Frank & Frank, 1982). Groups of free-ranging, domestic dogs show a great variation in social behavior depending on (a) food abundance, (b) location (rural or urban environment), and (c) competition with or predation by foxes, wolves, and humans. Urban groups of stray dogs may live in loosely structured pairs or small groups, whereas dogs in rural areas may form more stable, pack-like groups that resemble the social struc- Requests for reprints should be sent to Petra A. Mertens, Assistant Professor, Behavior Medicine, University of Minnesota, College of Veterinary Medicine, 1352 Boyd Avenue Room C311, St. Paul, MN 55108. Email: merte006@umn.edu

288 MERTENS ture of wolves and other canids (Fox, 1978; Macdonald & Carr, 1992; Pal, Gosh, & Roy, 1998). When Belayaev (1979) selected foxes for tameness, the less fearful animals showed a number of unexpected changes in their appearance (fur, color, and tail position), as well as changes in the concentration of neurotransmitters, such as serotonin, that regulate emotion and behavior. It is possible that domestication changed the selection criteria for dogs in a similar fashion, altering their physical characteristics as well as behavior. Wolves and dogs show great similarities in their genome (Vila et al., 1997); however, the modification of mrna expression in brain areas responsible for emotional responses hypothalamus, amygdala, and frontal cortex account, in part, for differences in behavior (Saetre et al., 2004). The differences in behavior between dogs and wolves and differences between breeds of dogs may also be explained by a change in the rate of behavioral development. This underdevelopment paedomorphism leads to changes in the dog s behavior that correlate positively with the degree of the dog s physical resemblance to the wolf. Dogs who bear a stronger physical resemblance to wolves (e.g., the Siberian husky) show a larger repertoire of agonistic behaviors than do dogs who show little resemblance to wolves (e.g., the Cavalier King Charles spaniel). This effect may alter the behavior from breed to breed and influence the dog s ability to communicate and avoid conflict. Breeds with a very limited repertoire of agonistic behavior continue to display threats but fail to show submissive behaviors during encounters with conspecifics (Goodwin, Bradshaw, & Wickens, 1997). The ability of dogs to communicate and avoid fights through the use of visual signals may be impaired further by breed-specific changes to ear and tail form, posture, and hair coat as well as by skeletal changes that limit facial expressions and body postures. No available clinical study researches the influence of breed-specific differences on communication and its influence on the incidence of fights in multidog households. Typically, I find that dogs who live in the company of conspecifics are able to coexist peacefully within the boundaries of a house. Fights between cohabiting dogs represent between 5% and 18% of the overall caseload of veterinary behaviorists (Flannigan, 2003). Many attempts to explain the etiology of aggression between dogs in the same home are based on the assumption that the problem is due to dominance aggression. The term dominance cannot be used synonymously with the term aggression. Dominance refers solely to the outcome of one encounter within a dyad, requiring to avoid escalation of the conflict the yielding response of the opponent in favor of the dominant individual. Aggression will occur only if the outcome cannot be predicted from resource-guarding potential (Bradshaw & Nott, 1992, Drews, 1993). Based on availability of resources or on the specific situation including alliances such as the presence of a third party the motivation to react aggressively may vary individually.

COMMENT ON VAN KERKHOVE S COMMENTARY 289 That fights between dogs within the same home typically are initiated by the younger dog, once he or she reaches sexual or social maturity, or by the dog who recently was added to the home (Flannigan, 2003; Sherman, Reisner, Laliaferro, & Houpt, 1996), indicates that fights may be due to territoriality or unresolved hierarchical conflicts. The context in which these fights occur supports this: 1. Increased arousal or excitement (51%); 2. Proximity of food or toys (48%); 3. Proximity of the caregiver (43%); 4. Limitation to a confined space (37%); 5. Defense of a preferred resting place (23%); or 6. Response to a postural threat (20%) (Sherman et al., 1996). The majority of fights within the home occur between female female pairs (Flannigan, 2003; Sherman et al., 1996), the behavior resembling that of some groups of free-ranging dogs in which aggressive behavior has been seen more commonly between female dogs. This occurred especially around feeding locations in winter and during lactation (Pal et al., 1998), indicating that aggression is likely to increase if resources are limited and have increased value. A dog s motivation to react aggressively may also be due to fear (lack of socialization and subsequent experience). It is likely that clinicians misdiagnose a number of cases, based on the appearance of the behavior on presentation. A careful analysis of a dog s behavior over time may reveal that fear-motivated behavior will appear more offensive if the dog learned to react in this fashion because it leads to a favorable outcome. The dogs that I see in my clinic belong to a preselected group because they obviously failed to respond to the use of positive punishment for the aggressor and positive reinforcement to the victim of the aggression. In these cases, aggression typically escalated severly over the course of time prior to presentation. This correlation between the onset of the health issue and aggressive behavior may be coincidence, or it may be causative of the aggressive behavior. The sick dog may be the recipient of aggression, presumably due to the perceived weakness of the affected dog. In other cases, a dog s failing health may increase irritability and induce or increase aggressive behavior. Free-ranging dogs can avoid conflict more successfully through avoidance of encounters with other dogs, making fights less common. Space restrictions and the influence of caregivers attempts to control the behavior of their dogs may catalyze problems and prevent resolution using ritualized behavioral patterns. Owners of affected dogs find it hard to refrain from punishing an undesired behavior such as aggression toward another dog in the home. Instead, they tend to scold the aggressor and protect or console the recipient of aggression. The tension within the home significantly affects the owner s relationship with the aggressor

290 MERTENS and alters their interactions, which may escalate the tension between the dogs. Applying positive punishment to the aggressor and positive reinforcement to the victim apparently is ineffective because many cases have been ongoing over time and have escalated in severity. Successful treatment of dog dog aggression in a multidog home requires a careful history. Probably, it is ineffective to support the dog who has the highest resource-guarding potential unless it has been determined that the problem is based on a hierarchical conflict. The owner s support for this dog should be applied whenever the dogs are not fighting (allowing the dog with the higher resource-guarding potential priority access to resources). Many owners find it extremely difficult to follow this advice. A resulting lack of compliance may be part of the reason that this approach leads to marked improvement or resolution in only 56% to 74% of the cases. The prognosis is poor if the problem persists for an extended period of time, if fights lead to injuries, or if fights are not predictable for the owner (Flannigan, 2003; Sherman et al., 1996). Because dogs who display aggression within the a multidog home are commonly found to be anxious, it is helpful to decrease the dog s overall anxiety level by using measures that increase routine and predictability of events and owner reactions in conjunction with avoiding conflict situations and taking safety precautions for the dogs and family members who may witness a fight. The use of negative punishment in response to an undesired behavior (aggression) and the use of positive reinforcement for desired behavior (peaceful coexistence or friendly interactions) are excellent tools that can be used exclusively or in conjunction with the previously described approach. The use of a verbal cue ( too bad, you blew it ), followed by walking the dog to a room before the dog is left in isolation (negative punishment) may pose problems with timing, even if the verbal cue serves as a bridging stimulus. Isolation follows extended interaction with the owner (leashing the dog, walking with owner, and unleashing). If possible, the owners can be encouraged to withdraw attention immediately and reliably by leaving the room. Typically, the owners concerns that their absence may lead to fights is unfounded. Leaving the room actually may lessen the risk of fights because most dogs fight in the owner s presence or over receiving the owner s attention. Successful treatment of behavioral problems between dogs requires a good understanding of canine ethology, the ability to observe and identify interactions between the dogs and their owners, and a flexible approach that allows adjusting the treatment regimen to the individual situation, the dogs motivation to fight, and the owners abilities and expectations. Independent of ancestral history of the species and individual history of the dog, it appears the most significant factor influencing outcome of a case is the owner s compliance.

COMMENT ON VAN KERKHOVE S COMMENTARY 291 REFERENCES Belyaev, D. K. (1979). Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. Journal of Heredity, 70, 301 308. Bradshaw, J. W. S, & Nott, H. M. R. (1992). Social and communication behavior of companion dogs. In J. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people (pp. 199 216). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Drews, C. (1993). The concept and definition of dominance in animal behavior. Behaviour, 125, 284 313. Flannigan, G. (2003, July). Canine sibling rivalry: Characteristics, owner compliance, and factors affecting prognosis for treatment outcome. Paper presented at the first Meeting of the American College of Veterinary Behaviorists, Denver, CO. Fox, M. W. (1978). The dog, its domestication and behavior. New York: Garland STPM. Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1982). On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior. Applied Animal Ethology, 8, 507 525. Goodwin, D., Bradshaw, J. W. S., & Wickens, S. M. (1997). Paedomporphism affects agonistic signals of domestic dogs. Animal Behaviour, 53, 297 304. Macdonald, D. W., & Carr, G. M. (1995) Variation in dog society: Between resource dispersion and social flux. In J. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people (pp. 199 216). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Pal, S. K., Gosh, B., & Roy, S. (1998). Agonistic behavior of free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to season, sex and age. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 59, 331 348. Saetre, P., Lindberg, J., Leonard, J. A., Olsson, K., Petterson, U., Ellegren, H., et al. (2004). From wild wolf to domestic dog: Gene expression changes in the brain. Molecular Brain Research, 126, 198 206. Sherman, C. K., Reisner, I. R., Laliaferro, L. A., & Houpt, K. A. (1996). Characteristics, treatment, and outcome of 99 cases of aggression between dogs. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 47, 91 108. Vila, C., Savolainen, P., Maldonando, J. E., Amorium, I. R., Rice, J. E., Honeycutt, R. L., et al. (1997). Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. Science, 276, 1687 1689.