Zurich Open Repository and Archive. Feeding live prey to zoo animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland

Similar documents
Stereotypic pacing in Siberian tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) and the effect of interspecies presence: A case study

ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER

Comparison of Several Types of Enrichment for Captive Felids

Tigers Stereotypic Pacing and Enrichment

Management of bold wolves

Role of Temperature and Shade Coverage on Behavior and Habitat Use of Captive African Lions, Snow Leopards, and Cougars

Lecture 15. Biology 5865 Conservation Biology. Ex-Situ Conservation

Distressed Animal Behaviors and Some Recommendations for Improvements at the Kuala Lumpur Zoo, Malaysia Amber Haque Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

The Forgotten Taxa : Past, present and future directions in zoo research. Dr Sonya P. Hill

Intact Carcasses as Enrichment for Large Felids: Effects on On- and Off-Exhibit Behaviors

Is It a Cheetah? By Stephanie S. Tolan Stephanie S. Tolan

Abnormal Repetitive Behaviours

lasting compassion and

POSITION DESCRIPTION. Organisational Context: Important Functional Relationships: Page 1. Job Title: Reports To: Direct Reports: Position Purpose:

Beauty, temperament and health as fundamental criteria for a correct selection

Introduction to the Cheetah

Special Educational Needs (SEN) CARING FOR ANIMALS

Effects of construction noise on behaviour of and exhibit use by Snow leopards Uncia uncia at Basel zoo

Anyone interested in serving on this committee please contact Bret Sellers

Meredith J. Bashaw a, Angela S. Kelling b, Mollie A. Bloomsmith b & Terry L. Maple b a TECHlab, Zoo Atlanta, and Center for

Talking about zoos, animal well-being and education with Jon Coe

Guidance: Housing (Scotland) Act 2001

Primate Welfare Meeting

EVOLUTIONARY GENETICS (Genome 453) Midterm Exam Name KEY

International Declaration of Responsibilities to Cats

Dog Off Leash Strategy

Surveys of the Street and Private Dog Population: Kalhaar Bungalows, Gujarat India

May 10, SWBAT analyze and evaluate the scientific evidence provided by the fossil record.

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

BIAZA Animal Transfer Policy (ATP)

International Declaration of Responsibilities to Cats

Knowledge, attitude, and behaviour toward antibiotics among Hong Kong people: local-born versus immigrants

Kathleen Krafte, Lincoln Larson, Robert Powell Clemson University ISSRM: June 14, 2015

Factors Impacting Public Perceptions of Animal Welfare & Animal Rights Candace C. Croney Purdue University

Update on the in-situ and ex-situ conservation of the Lord Howe Island stick insect. Mark Bushell Curator of Invertebrates Bristol Zoological Society

CAREERS INFORMATION. learnwithdogstrust.org.uk. Dogs Trust Registered Charity Nos and SC037843

Animal Movement. Investigating the amazing adaptations different animals have to allow them to move in different ways to survive.

KCAI Scheme Online Assessments: Criteria

Zoo Crew. A SmithSoniAn S national Zoo FAmily EduCAtion GuidE

Interactions between large felids and humans in zoos: animal, keeper and visitor perspectives. Monika Szokalski. Doctor of Philosophy (Psychology)

Chapter 13 First Year Student Recruitment Survey

OIE international standards on Rabies:

Some Foods Used by Coyotes and Bobcats in Cimarron County, Oklahoma 1954 Through

Do you know the helpline number in case of a stray animal in

Wolf Recovery Survey New Mexico. June 2008 Research & Polling, Inc.

FOUR PAWS BIG CAT SANCTUARY LIONSROCK. More Humanity towards Animals

What is the right approach to tackle the illegal consumption and trade of marine turtle products in Cape Verde?

Animal Adaptations. Structure and Function

Call of the Wild. Investigating Predator/Prey Relationships

HUMAN APPENDIX BATS & TROPICAL FLOWERS

What is a tiger? Tigers are felids (members of the cat family). They are in the genus Panthera.

Talks generally last minutes and take place in one of our classrooms.

Aerial view of the Faculty of Veterinary Medicine Utrecht

Public consultation on Proposed Revision of the Australian Code of Practice for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes 2004

Madagascar Spider Tortoise Updated: January 12, 2019

FUNDRAISING GUIDE. Surprising Facts About Big Cats Getting Started Social Media Fundraising Ideas Build a Boma FAQs.

Education. Worksheets Stage One. Designed in conjunction with ACARA curriculum

MANAGING AVIARY SYSTEMS TO ACHIEVE OPTIMAL RESULTS. TOPICS:

Hours of manual cash counting reduced to 12 minutes. John G. Shedd Aquarium, USA

Introduction to the Cheetah

Regulating the scientific use of animals taken from the wild Implementation of Directive 2010/63/EU

Laws and Regulations

Original Draft: 11/4/97 Revised Draft: 6/21/12

The Application of Animal Welfare Ethics Student Activities

Checks and Balances. Dr. Carmen L. Battaglia

Adaptations: Changes Through Time

Explanatory Memorandum to the Mutilations (Permitted Procedures) (Wales) (Amendment) Regulations 2008

Big Cat Rescue Presents. Tigrina or Oncilla

26 August :27 UK. Axolotl verges on wild extinction Matt Walker Editor, Earth News. A captive albino axolotl displays its larval gills

TOGETHER WE ACHIEVE THE BEST IN ANIMAL WELLBEING

Guidelines for the Use of Nonhuman Animals in Behavioral Projects in Schools (K-12)

Frustrated Appetitive Foraging Behavior, Stereotypic Pacing, and Fecal Glucocorticoid Levels in Snow Leopards (Uncia uncia) in the Zurich Zoo

Aquarist. Jobs at an Aquarium

international news RECOMMENDATIONS

BELIEFS AND PRACTICES OF PARENTS ON THE USE OF ANTIBIOTICS FOR THEIR CHILDREN WITH UPPER RESPIRATORY TRACT INFECTION

Kori Bustard Husbandry. Sara Hallager, Biologist, Smithsonian National Zoological Park

About Animals Asia. Who we are. Our work

Tyrone J. Burrows Jr. & William J. Fielding 1 Views of College Students on Pit Bull Ownership : New Providence, The Bahamas

PE1561/J. Ned Sharratt Public Petitions Clerks Room T3.40 The Scottish Parliament Edinburgh EH99 1SP. 11 December 2015.

The Partnership for Preventive Pet Healthcare. March 11, 2012

Third Annual Conference on Animals and the Law

What is an. Amphibian?

More Than Ever, Pets are Members of the Family

Effectiveness of Educational Module on knowledge regarding Dengue and its prevention

Painted Dog (Lycaon pictus)

1. Number of Competencies Evaluated. 2. Number of Competencies Rated 2 or Percent of Competencies Attained (2/1) Grade. Instructor Signature

COMPARING BODY CONDITION ESTIMATES OF ZOO BROTHER S ISLAND TUATARA (SPHENODON GUNTHERI) TO THAT OF THE WILD, A CLINICAL CASE

Dealing with dairy cow lameness applying knowledge on farm

Department of Health and Mental Hygiene. Board of Health

Keywords: Acinonyx jubatus/breeding/captivity/cheetah/management/off-exhibit

RARC: Animal Social Housing & Enrichment Requirements (ASHER)

Care For Us Binturong (Arc,c,s binturong)

Rules and liability insurance applicable for Expo Mitt and Expo Syd

Refinement Issues in Animal Research. Joanne Zurlo, PhD Institute for Laboratory Animal Research National Academy of Sciences

Family Pet Ownership during Childhood: Findings from a UK Birth Cohort and Implications for Public Health Research

Naturalised Goose 2000

The Animals in the Law

Adaptations 4. Adaptations 1 Adaptations 2

DEMOGRAPHIC AND HEALTH SURVEYS ACCIDENT AND INJURY MODULE MODEL HOUSEHOLD QUESTIONNAIRE IDENTIFICATION (1)

Silverback Male Presence and Group Stability in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)

Transcription:

University of Zurich Zurich Open Repository and Archive Winterthurerstr. 190 CH-8057 Zurich http://www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2010 Feeding live prey to zoo animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland Cottle, L; Tamir, D; Hyseni, M; Bühler, D; Lindemann-Matthies, P Cottle, L; Tamir, D; Hyseni, M; Bühler, D; Lindemann-Matthies, P (2010). Feeding live prey to zoo animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland. Zoo Biology, 29(3):344-350. Postprint available at: http://www.zora.uzh.ch Posted at the Zurich Open Repository and Archive, University of Zurich. http://www.zora.uzh.ch Originally published at: Zoo Biology 2010, 29(3):344-350.

Corresponding author: Petra Lindemann-Matthies, Institute of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, CH-8057 Zurich, Switzerland, petral@uwinst.uzh.ch Feeding life-prey to zoo animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland Abstract In summer 2007, with the help of a written questionnaire, the attitudes of more than 400 visitors to the zoological garden of Zurich, Switzerland, towards the idea of feeding live insects to lizards, live fish to otters, and live rabbits to tigers were investigated. The majority of Swiss zoo visitors agreed with the idea of feeding live prey (invertebrates and vertebrates) to zoo animals, both off- and on-exhibit, except in the case of feeding live rabbits to tigers on-exhibit. Women and frequent visitors of the zoo disagreed more often with the on-exhibit feeding of live rabbits to tigers. Study participants with a higher level of education were more likely to agree with the idea of feeding live invertebrates and vertebrates to zoo animals off-exhibit. In comparison to an earlier study undertaken in Scotland, zoo visitors in Switzerland were more often in favor of the live feeding of vertebrates. Feeding live prey can counter the loss of hunting skills of carnivores and improve the animals well-being. However, feeding enrichments have to strike a balance between optimal living conditions of animals and the quality of visitor experience. Our results show that such a balance can be found, especially when live feeding of mammals is carried out off-exhibit. A good interpretation of food enrichment might help zoos to win more support for the issue, and for re-introduction programs and conservation. 1

Introduction Modern zoos see their mission not only in protecting and preserving specimens of endangered species, but also in providing these species with adequate conditions to reproduce and form stable populations, with the goal of reintroducing the animals to the wild (WAZA, 2005; WAZA, 2006). To make reintroductions successful, not only a minimum number of individuals of a certain species should be maintained, but also their social structure and survival skills, foraging and hunting included. Many deaths of reintroduced animals are due to behavioral deficiencies, as generations in captivity make animals lose crucial learned attributes (McPhee, 2004). With carnivores in particular, the supply of ready-made fodder can cause the loss of their hunting skills (Rabin, 2003). As a consequence, carnivores reintroduced to the wild from captivity are four-times more likely to die of starvation than carnivores reintroduced from other locations (Jule et al., 2008). One measure to counter the loss of hunting skills and to improve animal s well-being is the feeding with live prey (Bashaw et al., 2003; Rabin, 2003). For example, in species of felids, the provision of live prey was found to increase activity and enclosure utilisation, and to reduce stereotypic behavior (Shepherdson et al., 1993; Bashaw et al., 2003). Feeding enrichment also increased zoo visitors experience with the animals as they were visible for a longer time (Bashaw et al., 2003). However, if the prey has little or no chance to escape, ethical questions arise (Wickins-Dražilová, 2006). Moreover, education is an essential conservation task of a modern zoo (IUDZG/CBSG (IUCN/SSC), 1993). If feeding live prey is unacceptable for the public, zoos might fail to provide their intended educational message, particularly in terms of conservation (Shepherdson et al., 1993; Mason, 2000). However, zoo visitors are very diverse and include people of all ages and educational levels 2

(Falk et al., 1986; Falk and Adelman, 2003). As a result, different groups of visitors might react differently to the idea of feeding live prey in zoos. In summer 2007, we investigated with the help of a written questionnaire, the attitudes of more than 400 visitors to the zoological garden of Zurich, Switzerland, towards the idea of feeding live insects to lizards, live fish to otters, and live rabbits to tigers. A comparable study had already been carried out in 1995 in Edinburgh zoo, Scotland. It showed that both on- and off-exhibit feeding of live insects to lizards and live fish to penguins was accepted at least by 70% of the study participants, whereas the on-exhibit feeding of live rabbits to cheetahs was only accepted by 32 percent (Ings et al., 1997). Moreover, off-exhibit feeding was more appreciated than on-exhibit feeding. The main objectives of the present study were to investigate: - whether visitors to the zoological garden of Zurich agree to the idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals, - whether they are more concerned about the live feeding of rabbits to tigers than about the feeding of live insects to reptiles or live fish to otters, - whether their attitudes were influenced by the feeding method (on- or off-exhibit) as well as age, gender, level of education, the frequency of annual zoo visits, and pet ownership. Methods Data were collected on Sunday 15 th and Wednesday 18 th July, 2007 at various places in the zoological garden of Zurich, Switzerland. The 10 minute written questionnaire 1 was administered to a total of 409 zoo visitors (207 men and 202 women). Study participants 3

were chosen at random. However, we tried to balance for sex and age by asking an equal proportion of men and women, as well as people from varying age groups to participate in the survey. The main questions used to determine attitudes towards feeding live prey to zoo animals (yes / no answers) were: - Would you agree to live insects being fed to lizards on-exhibit? - Would you agree to live insects being fed to lizards off-exhibit? - Would you agree to live fish being fed to otters on-exhibit? - Would you agree to live fish being fed to otters off-exhibit? - Would you agree to a live rabbit being fed to a tiger on-exhibit? - Would you agree to a live rabbit being fed to a tiger off-exhibit? In addition, all study participants were asked to state their age and gender, and their highest level of education (primary school, secondary school, apprenticeship, high school or equivalent, university). From these data a variable was derived indicating whether a person had a lower (primary or secondary school, apprenticeship) or higher (high school, university) education. Study participants were also asked whether they visited the zoological garden of Zurich and other zoos rarely (less than two times a year) or often (more than once a year). Finally, they were asked whether they owned a pet and, if so, to write down which kind of pet. If any additional comments were made by the study participants, these were also recorded. Study participants were between 12 and 85 years old (mean age = 37 years). About 52% (214 persons) had a higher education. Of the participants, 23% visited the zoological 4

garden of Zurich, and 17% other zoos more than once a year. About 50% (211 persons) were pet owners. Most often they owned cats (114 persons), dogs (51), or rabbits (29). The significance of the proportion of respondents that agreed vs. disagreed to the idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals was tested by simple Chi-square-tests. Differences in the proportion of respondents that agreed to live feeding a certain type of animal on- and offexhibit were analyzed by McNemar-tests. Possible influences of age, gender, frequency of zoo visits, pet ownership, and educational level on the probability that respondents agreed with feeding live prey were analyzed with multiple binary logistic regressions. Minimum adequate models were derived by first fitting all explanatory variables and then removing all non-significant terms (P > 0.1, Mertler and Vannatta, 2005). Results With the exception of feeding rabbits to tigers on-exhibit, most study participants agreed with the feeding of live prey to zoo animals, both on- and off-exhibit (Table 1). In case of feeding live insects to lizards and live fish to otters they did not differ significantly in their agreement to on- and off-exhibit demonstrations, whereas in case of feeding live rabbits to tigers they more often agreed to off-exhibit feeding (see Table 1). [Insert Table 1 about here] In the models, only gender, level of education and the frequency of visits to the zoological garden of Zurich influenced the agreement of zoo visitors to the idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals (Table 2), whereas age, pet ownership and the frequency of visits to other zoos had no effect. Men agreed more often with the on-exhibit feeding of live insects to lizards 5

and live rabbits to tigers, and study participants with a higher education more often with the feeding of live prey off-exhibit (Table 3, see Table 2). [Insert Table 2 about here] [Insert Table 3 about here] Frequent visitors to the zoological garden of Zurich (at least two visits a year) agreed less often than the others with the on-exhibit feeding of live rabbits to tigers (40% and 53%, respectively), whereas they agreed more often to the off-exhibit feeding of live fish to otters (86% and 77%, respectively). Pet ownership in general did not influence visitors opinion about the feeding of live prey to zoo animals (all P > 0.05). However, 72% of the study participants who owned a rabbit disagreed with the on-exhibit feeding of live rabbits to tigers, whereas only 50% of the others disagreed (Chi-square value = 5.42, P = 0.015). Discussion Most study participants agreed with the idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals, both offand on-exhibit, except in the case of the on-exhibit feeding of live rabbits to tigers. Some participants made comments to justify their opinion, such as; it is natural and in nature, it is normal. It has been suggested that there is a hierarchy of concern which is a function of the distance of relationship between the prey animal and primates (Eddy et al., 1993; Ings et al., 1997), and our results are consistent with this idea. The more closely a prey animal was related to primates, the fewer participants agreed that it should be fed alive to zoo animals or, if so, that it should be done off-exhibit. 6

Humans like visually attractive animals with considerable intelligence and the capacity for social bonding, and tend to avoid invertebrates because they are small, and morphologically and behaviorally unlike humans (e.g. Kellert, 1993a; Kellert, 1993b; Lindemann-Matthies, 2005). It is thus not surprising that zoo visitors in both the present and other studies were least concerned about the feeding of live insects, and most concerned about the feeding of live rabbits, especially on-exhibit (Ings et al., 1997; McDole, 2007). Moreover, zoos like other recreational facilities, are social settings that are often visited by families who want to enjoy their leisure time together and watch their favorite animals (Falk et al., 1986). Attractive mammals being killed and eaten by other attractive mammals might not be people s idea of an enjoyable family excursion, especially if the prey resembles their children s beloved pet animals. We had assumed that pet owners would show a greater affection for pet-like prey and, in consequence, disagree more often with the live feeding of rabbits to zoo animals. However, with the exception of rabbit owners themselves, this was actually not the case. Women objected more often than men to the on-exhibit feeding of live animals (see also Ings et al., 1997). This might partly be due to a greater emotional affection of women for large, attractive, primarily domestic pet animals (Lindemann-Matthies, 2005). Moreover, women were found to respond to zoo animals with greater empathy (Reade and Waran, 1996). In comparison to an earlier study undertaken in Edinburgh zoo, Scotland (Ings et al., 1997), zoo visitors in Switzerland were more often in favor of the live feeding of vertebrates. Interestingly, findings of a recent, comparable study from the United States (McDole, 2007) were quite similar to those from our study. The author pointed out that her findings might reflect a general acceptance of live feeding in the United States which is exemplified 7

in the absence of a law against live prey introduction. However, we assume that the difference in attitude between Scottish and Swiss zoo visitors is rather due to an attitude shift over the last twelve years (note: the Scottish survey was carried out in 1995) than to culture-related attitude differences between people in Switzerland and Scotland. Zoo visitors today are probably more knowledgeable about conservation issues, and might thus display more positive attitudes in general towards the on-exhibit feeding of live prey than those more than a decade ago. It could also be that visitors to zoos today are much more broadly exposed to predation events on nature documentaries and television programmes than they were even in 1995, and that this may have increased their habituation to seeing feeding of live prey on-exhibit. Moreover, zoos are increasingly concerned about animal welfare and offer opportunities for animals to hide from the view of the visitors. It is therefore possible that visitors perceive on-exhibit feeding as an attraction and an opportunity to see the animals (see also Bashaw, 2003). The observed influence of the level of education on visitors attitudes also stresses the value of ecological information. This might indicate that with time, a good balance between the well-being of zoo animals, i.e. the preservation of predators behavioral skills and the attraction of zoos as leisure time locations for visitors, might be achieved. However, our data also indicate that at least in Switzerland zoo visitors might still resent the on-exhibit feeding of mammals, although it is allowed in principal by law. A good interpretation of food enrichment and thus animal well-being to the public might help zoos to win more support for the issue, but also support for re-introduction programs and conservation. 8

Conclusions 1. The majority of Swiss zoo visitors agreed with the idea of feeding live prey (invertebrates and vertebrates) to zoo animals, both off- and on-exhibit, except in the case of feeding live rabbits to tigers on-exhibit. 2. Women and frequent visitors to the zoological garden of Zurich disagreed more often with the on-exhibit feeding of live rabbits to tigers. Study participants with a higher level of education more often agreed with the idea of feeding live invertebrates and vertebrates to zoo animals off-exhibit. 3. In comparison to an earlier study undertaken in Scotland, zoo visitors in Switzerland were more often in favor of the live feeding of vertebrates, probably due to an attitude shift over the last twelve years. 4. Feeding live prey can counter the loss of hunting skills of carnivores and improve the animals well-being. However, feeding enrichments have to strike a balance between optimal living conditions of animals and the quality of visitor experience. Our results show that such a balance can be found, especially when live feeding of mammals is carried out off-exhibit. Note The questionnaire (English translation) is available from the authors. Acknowledgements We would like to thank the staff at the zoological garden of Zurich for their support in the present study, and Xuefei Li and Kevin Richards for their help in collecting data. 9

References Bashaw MJ, Bloomsmith MA, Marr MJ, Maple TL. 2003. To hunt or not to hunt? A feeding enrichment experiment with captive large felids. Zoo Biol 22:189-198. Eddy TJ, Gallup GG, Povinelli DJ. 1993. Attribution of cognitive states to animals: anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. J Soc Issues 49:87-101. Falk JH, Adelman LM. 2003. Investigating the impact of prior knowledge and interest on aquarium visitor learning. J Res Sci Teach 40:163-176. Falk JH, Koran JJ Jr, Dierking LD. 1986. The things of science: assessing the learning potential of science museums. Sci Educ 70:503-508. Ings R, Waran NK, Young RJ. 1997. Attitude of zoo visitors to the idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals. Zoo Biol 16:343-347. IUDZG/CBSG (IUCN/SSC) 1993. The World Zoo Conservation Strategy. The role of zoos and aquaria of the world in global conservation. Brookfield, Ill, Chicago Zoological Society. Jule KR, Leaver LA, Lea SEG. 2008. The effects of captive experience on reintroduction survival in carnivores: A review and analysis. Biol Cons 141:355-363. Kellert SR. 1993a. The biological base for human values of nature. In: Kellert SR, Wilson EO, editors. The biophilia hypothesis. Washington DC: Island Press. p 43-69. Kellert SR. 1993b. Attitudes, knowledge, and behaviour toward wildlife among the industrial superpowers: United States, Japan, and Germany. J Soc Issues 49:53-69. Lindemann-Matthies P. 2005. Loveable mammals and lifeless plants: how children s interest in common local organisms can be enhanced through observation of nature. Int J Sci Educ 27:655-677. Mason P. 2000. Zoo tourism: the need for more research. J Sustain Tourism 8:333-339. McDole E. 2007. Fish introduction to jaguars (Panthera onca): response of zoo visitors and jaguars. Unpublished Master thesis. Georgia Institute of Technology. Retrieved April 25, 2008, from http://www.smartech.gatech.edu/dspace/handle/1853/14471 McPhee ME. 2004. Generations in captivity increases behavioral variance: considerations for captive breeding and reintroduction programs. Biol Cons 115:71-77. Mertler CA., Vannatta RA. 2005. Advanced and multivariate statistical methods: practical application and interpretation. 3rd ed. Glendale, CA: Pyrczak Publishing. 348 p. Rabin LA. 2003. Maintaining behavioural diversity in captivity for conservation: natural behaviour management. Anim Welfare 12:85-94. Reade LS, Waran NK. 1996. The modern zoo: how do people perceive zoo animals? Appl Anim Behav Sci 18:31-43. Shepherdson DJ, Carlstead K, Mellen JD, Seidensticker J. 1993. The influence of food presentation on the behavior of small cats in confined environments. Zoo Biol 12:203-216. WAZA 2005. Building a future for wildlife - the world zoo and aquarium conservation strategy. Berne, Switzerland, WAZA. WAZA 2006. Wer Tiere kennt, wird Tiere schützen - die Schweizer Zoos und die Welt-Zoo- Naturschutzstrategie. Berne, Switzerland, Stämpfli Publikationen AG. Wickins-Dražilová D. 2006. Zoo animal welfare. J Agr Environ Ethics 19:27-36. 10

Table 1: Agreement of zoo visitors (N = 409) to the idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals Agreement (%) Agreement vs. disagreement χ 2 On-exhibit vs. off-exhibit (McNemar test) χ 2 Live insects to lizards on-exhibit 87.3 227.45*** Live insects to lizards off-exhibit 82.6 174.30*** Live fish to otters on-exhibit 82.6 174.30*** Live fish to otters off-exhibit 80.0 146.76*** Live rabbits to tigers on-exhibit 48.4 0.41 Live rabbits to tigers off-exhibit 68.2 54.28*** 3.72 1.08 42.38*** ***P < 0.001; All df 1 11

animals: response of zoo visitors in Switzerland. Zoo Biology 28: 1-7. doi: 10.1002/zoo.20261 Table 2: Variables influencing the agreement of zoo visitors to the idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals. The data were analyzed with binary logistic regressions. Given are the Nagelkerke R-squares for the overall models and the regression coefficients (b) and Wald statistics (Wald) for each significant predictor. Predictors Nagelkerke R square Gender Education Visits to Zurich zoo b Wald b Wald b Wald Live insects to lizards (on-exhibit) 0.035-0.85 7.34** - - - - Live fish to otters (on-exhibit) - - - - - - - Live rabbits to tigers (on-exhibit) 0.041-0.54 7.16** - - -0.44 4.25* Live insects to lizards (off-exhibit) 0.028 - - 0.70 6.87** - - Live fish to otters (off-exhibit) 0.044 - - 0.64 6.38* 0.58 4.11* Live rabbits to tigers (off-exhibit) 0.019 - - 0.50 5.45* - - *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; All df 1 12

Table 3: The effect of gender and level of education on the agreement of zoo visitors to the idea of feeding live prey to zoo animals. Pairs of values in bold are significantly different in individual Chi-square-tests (P < 0.05). Agreement (%) Agreement (%) Men Women Lower education Higher education (N = 207) (N = 202) (N = 195) (N = 214) Live insects to lizards (on-exhibit) 91.8 82.7 86.7 87.9 Live fish to otters (on-exhibit) 85.0 80.2 81.0 84.1 Live rabbits to tigers (on-exhibit) 55.6 41.2 44.6 51.9 Live insects to lizards (off-exhibit) 86.0 79.2 77.4 87.4 Live fish to otters (off-exhibit) 82.6 77.2 74.4 85.0 Live rabbits to tigers (off-exhibit) 71.5 64.9 62.6 73.4 16