BEHAVIOR GENETICS OF NEST GLEANING IN HONEY BEES. IV. RESPONSES OF F t AND BACKCROSS GENERATIONS TO DISEASE-KILLED BROOD

Similar documents
Oxford University Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to American Zoologist.

Days and Tasks. Ellen Miller December 2015

Abstract. Introduction

Splits. Aiken Beekeepers Association July 2016 S. Siler

Objectives. Bee Basics. Apis mellifera. Honey bees. Drones. Drones 3/16/2017

13. Cell division is. assortment. telophase. cytokinesis.

Honey Bees. Anatomy and Function 9/26/17. Similar but Different. Honey Bee External Anatomy. Thorax (Human Chest): 4 Wings & 6 Legs

Getting Your Honeybees Through the First Year

Honey Bees Basic Biology

The honey bee colony. by C Roff

VETERINARY MEDICINAL PRODUCTS CONTROLLING VARROA JACOBSONI AND ACARAPIS WOODI PARASITOSIS IN BEES

All You Ever Wanted to Know About Hornets and Yellowjackets

A Beekeeping Diary #5: Early Summer Queen Rearing Begins. Written by KirkWebster

INHERITANCE OF BODY WEIGHT IN DOMESTIC FOWL. Single Comb White Leghorn breeds of fowl and in their hybrids.

Measuring Varroa Sensitive Hygiene

Under One Roof. Beehive Management During the Swarming Season in a single hive. By: - Nick Withers

Temperature Gradient in the Egg-Laying Activities of the Queen Bee

SUPREME QUALITY ITALIAN HONEY THE WORLD OF BEES, AN OPEN STORY

Anatomy of a Swarm. What I Learned from Honeybee Democracy. by Dr. Thomas Seeley. Marja E van den Hende 1

I yellow, a great assortment of shades of red and yellow being known. The

After the treatment, the Gusmer pad was wet and heavy and smelled of formic; not dry.

Southern IL All-A-Buzz

Single-Queen-Founded Nests

Beginners Course COLONY MANAGEMENT MIKE PETT

So let me start by introducing myself. I recently saw a speaker do this by identifying the advantages that he had on his farm to give the audience

TOPIC 8: PUNNETT SQUARES

OLD BEEMAN INVENTIONS SERIES Part II What Bees We Have How to Keep Own Stock Best Grafting House I Know

Black Garden Ant 5A-1

Virtual Lab: Sex-Linked Traits Worksheet. 1. Please make sure you have read through all of the information in the

Genetics Problems. Character Dominant Recessive

Two-queen colony management

RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS.

ical treatments or lots of travel for pollination contracts), well-bred queens should last

OUTBREEDING MITES AND OVERWINTERING HONEYBEE NUCS Successful Beekeeping the Natural Way

I the BUSSEY INSTITUTION of HARVARD UNIVERSITY, it was found that

Diseases and Pests of Honeybees

Today: Mendel s Technique: What Mendel Observes: Mendelian Genetics: Consider this. Mendelian Genetics and Problems (In-Class 6)

9-2 Probability and Punnett. Squares Probability and Punnett Squares. Slide 1 of 21. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Anyone visiting the Bee Department must bring a clean bee suit, wellingtons and Marigold type gloves.

Do I Need a Veterinarian for My Bees?

SUMMARY OF BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON BEES AND WASPS

MANN ALBERT R. LIBRARY. New York State Colleges EVERETT FRANKLIN PHILLIPS BEEKEEPING LIBRARY. Cornell University. OF Agriculture and Home Economics

Yellowjacket Habitat at Home

Yellowjackets. Colorado Insects of Interest

Questions The word species in the first sentence is closest in meaning to A. mates. B. varieties. C. killers. D. enemies.

Understandings, Applications and Skills (This is what you maybe assessed on)

Spring Management of Honeybees HONEY BEE NUTRITIONAL NEEDS NUTRITION MANAGEMENT MITE MANAGEMENT. Spring Issues for Overwintered Colonies

Mendelian Genetics SI

The Year of the Wasp

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Level 2 Biology, 2015

PROJECT: EGGS OF ANT

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

ABSTRACT GLOSSARY OF TERMS. Layman Description

Naked Bunny Evolution

Heredity. What s heredity? An organism s heredity is the set of characteristics it receives from its parents. Today, known as genetics.

Bees and Bee Products Research Unit, Faculty of Agriculture, Chiang Mai University, Muang, Chiang Mai, Thailand

Recent actions by the European Commission concerning bee health

Genetics. Labrador Retrievers as a Model System to Study Inheritance of Hair Color. Contents of this Section

a type of honey. a nest. a type of bee. a storage space.

Breeding Bunnies. Purpose: To model the changes in gene frequency over several generations. 50 orange beads 50 purple beads 1 paper bag 3 cups

1. Describe the series of steps that you would perform to isolate arginine-requiring mutants from a wild-type haploid yeast strain.

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below.

Question 3 (30 points)

Genetics and Probability

Bees and WaIpi. 7ec oteo,igôaj e ''o.,te AUG Extension Circular 565 June W. P. Stephen. Oregon State College

Morphology of a female bee

Biology 201 (Genetics) Exam #1 120 points 22 September 2006

BOURNEMOUTH AND DORSET SOUTH BKA BIBBA DAY.

STINGING ARTHROPODS AROUND HOMES & LANDSCAPES

S7L2_Genetics and S7L5_Theory of Evolution (Thrower)

Genetics Jeopardy. Final Jeopardy. Indepen Assort. Data & Hypoth. Quant. Traits. Pop. Gen. X-linked. Segregati on. Epistasis.

The Barefoot Beekeeper's Guide to Swarming and Swarm Management

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING.

1 - Black 2 Gold (Light) 3 - Gold. 4 - Gold (Rich Red) 5 - Black and Tan (Light gold) 6 - Black and Tan

Mendel s Laws: Their Application to Solving Genetics Problem

Got Mites? Get Apivar! The Club will bee selling Apivar at Friday s meeting. Apivar 10-pack = $35.00

Sexy smells Featured scientist: Danielle Whittaker from Michigan State University

Identifying native honey bees. Gavin Ramsay

Mendelian Genetics Problem Set

7.013 Spring 2005 Problem Set 2

Sex-linked/incomplete dominance/codominance quiz

Genetics for breeders. The genetics of polygenes: selection and inbreeding

Evolution and Gene Frequencies: A Game of Survival and Reproductive Success

Incomplete Dominance and Codominance

Flip through the next few pages for a checklist of five of the more common, sinister summer scoundrels that you ll find throughout Arizona!

BIOL4. General Certificate of Education Advanced Level Examination June Unit 4 Populations and environment. Monday 13 June pm to 3.

Science 10-Biology Activity 17 Worksheet on More Complex Genetics

Sections 2.1. and 2.2. (Single gene inheritance, The chromosomal basis of single-gene inheritance patterns)

Seed color is either. that Studies Heredity. = Any Characteristic that can be passed from parents to offspring

DO NOT WRITE ON THIS TEST Unit 6 Assessment Genetics Objective 3.2.2

THE production of turkey hatching

Meeting the First Monday of Each Month 7 p.m. at Mountain Folk Center (formerly Industrial Park) 65 Folk Center Circle, Murphy, NC 28906

Study Questions. to Wonderful Wasps. naturalists. Young

Two-Factor Crosses. All of the resulting F 1 offsrping had round yellow peas (RrYy).

Level 2 Biology, 2017

ON COMMERCIAL poultry farms during

Biology 164 Laboratory

MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

Transcription:

AM. ZOOLOGIST, 4:111-123(1964). BEHAVIOR GENETICS OF NEST GLEANING IN HONEY BEES. IV. RESPONSES OF F t AND BACKCROSS GENERATIONS TO DISEASE-KILLED BROOD WALTER C. ROTHENBUHLER Department of Zoology and Entomology, The Ohio State University INTRODUCTION The honey bee is in one striking way different from most animals. Much of the animal kingdom lives a more or less solitary existence (except for a minimal amount of social life involved in mating activity and reproduction), whereas the honey bee lives in colonies of up to 6, individuals. Honey-bee colonies are much more than aggregations, flocks, or herds, for the individuals within one are so dependent upon one another that no individual, nor even a group of a few hundred individuals, can survive and perpetuate the species in nature. Some further facts of bee biology are basic to this paper. The nest is the center of colony life, and is composed of a number of wax combs arranged vertically in a suitable cavity such as a hollow tree. In modern apiculture, hives, instead of hollow trees, provide the cavities, and combs are contained inside wood frames which permits their removal from the nest, their examination, and rearrangement. Brood is reared and food is stored in these combs which are used over and over again, year after year. The drone and the queen (male and fe- The data contained in this paper were obtained while the author was a staff member of the Department of Zoology and Entomology, Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa. In addition to support from the Iowa Agricultural Experiment Station, this investigation was supported in part by Research Grant E599(C7) from the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases, Public Health Service, made to Iowa State University, and is presently supported by Research Grant HD368-1 from The National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Public Health Service, made to The Ohio State University. The author's deep appreciation is expressed to Mrs. Christina Barthel (nee Garwood), technician, and Mr. Victor C. Thompson, research associate, for their competent assistance with these experiments. (111) male) are the reproductively important members of the colony. Drones develop from unfertilized eggs, and are consequently haploid in origin and in transmission of genetic factors. Queens arise from fertilized eggs, which makes them diploid in origin and similar to any diploid animal in transmission of genetic factors. The third kind of bee in the colony, the worker bee, is a reproductively undeveloped female which arises from a fertilized egg, the same as the queen. Colony behavior is almost exclusively the behavior of worker bees. It is the worker bees that secrete wax and build comb, nurse the brood, keep the colony warm or cool as the need may be, defend the colony, keep the combs and hive interior clean, and go to the field to gather nectar, pollen, water, and propolis. Such colony labor is further divided among worker bees on the basis of their age. Normally the younger workers engage in duties inside the hive whereas older workers carry out the field activities. By this time, it is perhaps apparent that a social insect such as the honey bee displays a far greater variety of behavior than could be expected of insects living a solitary life. More information on life history, behavior, and social organization of honey bees may be found in Butler, 1954; VonFrisch, 195; Grout, 1963; Lindauer, 1961; Michener and Michener, 1951; Ribbands, 1953; Root, 1962; as well as in a host of other sources. The stock-in-trade of the behavior geneticist, however, is variation in behavior, and the honey bee has it. Different races and strains of bees vary, for instance, in the number of times their colonies swarm, in the number of times they sting, and in the amount of pollen stored. They even engage in different dances in reporting the same food source to the colony (Boch,

112 WALTER C. ROTHENBUHLER FIG. 1. Inoculation of larval food with spores of Bacillus larvae by use of microsyringe. 1957; and others). There is a great deal of behavioral variation in bees but a great lack of careful, quantitative investigation of this variation. More study in this field would be rewarding. Our studies have been concerned primarily with two differences in behavior. The first one is response to immature individuals which have died, in the cells of the brood comb, of the disease American foulbrood; and the second one, studied to a lesser extent, is response to the beekeeper more specifically, stinging behavior. NEST-CLEANING BEHAVIOR Description and nature of investigation The adults of certain colonies of bees remove foulbrood-killed larvae and pupae from the combs very quickly, whereas the adults of other colonies allow these dead individuals to remain in the combs for days, or weeks, or indefinitely. Such larvae and pupae, dead of American foulbrood, allowed to remain in the brood nest are a source of the pathogen, Bacillus larvae, and lead to new infections. This hygienic behavior, by which dead larvae and pupae are quickly removed, is one mechanism of resistance to the disease (Rothenbuhler and Thompson, 1955). Differences in response to American foulbrood-killed brood were discovered more than 2 years ago by O. W. Park (1936) at Iowa State College, and by Alan Woodrow and E. C. Hoist (1942) of the United States Department of Agriculture at Laramie, Wyoming, as they were investigating the mechanisms of resistance to disease. The research reported here was undertaken as part of our investigation of resistance to American foulbrood, but it has grown into a new effort on behavior genetics of bees (Rothenbuhler, 1958, 196, 1964; Jones and Rothenbuhler, 1964; Thompson, 1964). Hygienic behavior may be studied by techniques indicated in Figures 1, 2, and 3. A sample of eggs from a suitable mated queen is obtained in a comb, and is placed for hatching and rearing in the colony whose behavior is to be studied. At the appropriate larval age, the larval food sur- FIG. 2. Comb of brood, reared in a Brown, hygienic colony, about two days before brood emergence, showing that many indhiduals are missing from the spore inoculated rows. All brood remaining in this comb was found to be alive.

BEHAVIOR GENETICS OF NEST CLEANING IN HONEY BEES 113 FIG. 3. Comb of brood, reared in a Van Scoy, non-hygienic colony, showing that most of the brood seems to be present. When uncapped in the laboratory, many individuals in the sporeinoculated rows were dead of American foulbrood. zo 18 16 CHECK SPORES 14 DAILY PER CENT REMOVAL 12 1 8 6 4 2 c=^m c^* 1 II 12 13 14 15 16 AFB DEAD LEFT IN CELLS FIG. 4. Behavior of three Brown colonies resulted in the removal of all American foulbroodkilled (AFB) individuals before the end of the experiment. rounding each larva is inoculated, by means of a microsyringe, with spores of the pathogen dispersed in water. Control (or check) larvae are inoculated with water only, or are uninoculated, as the situation requires. Figure 2 is a picture of a comb photographed at the end of an experiment. It shows spore-inoculated rows of brood from which many individuals have been removed as contrasted with the check rows of brood. This brood was reared in a colony showing hygienic behavior. Figure 3 is a picture of brood reared in a colony which did not show hygienic behavior. Nearly all individuals seem to be present, but when these cells were manually uncapped, large numbers of larvae were found to be dead of American foulbrood in the spore-inoculated rows. Experimental results Figures 4, 5, and 6 show graphs o the data from an experiment involving colonies from our Brown resistant line, our Van Scoy susceptible line, and the F x generation resulting from a cross of the two. All tests were made concurrently. Figure 4 shows the pooled daily percentage removals of check and spore-treated larvae from three Brown-line colonies. On the first day of larval life, not shown in the graph, the larvae were inoculated. On the second day of larval life the larvae were counted and this was the base count for the subsequent calculations. On the horizontal axis it can be seen that, by the third

114 WALTER C. ROTHENBUHLER 2 IB 16 D CHECK SPORES 14 DAILY PER CENT REMOVAL 12 1 e 6 4 2 J 3 IS AFB DEAD LEFT IN CELLS FIG. 5. Behavior o four Van Scoy colonies left most foulbrood-killed individuals in the comb until the end of the experiment on the 16th day. 28 26 24 22 DAILY PERCENT REMOVAL 2 ia 16 14 12 1 8 6 4 2 CHECK SPORES FIG. 6. Behavior o five F, colonies resembled the behavior of the Van Sco)S. AFB DEAD LEFT IN CELLS day, a small percentage of the base count of checks was removed, and also a small percentage of the spore-inoculated base count. It may be pointed out here that capping of the cells occurs between the 4th and 6th days, and the larval stage ends by about the 8th day. There was a substantial amount of removal of spore-inoculated larvae on the 8th, 9th, 1th, and 11th days, and no substantial removal of checks. Very little removal occurred on the last days of the expeiiment. On the 16th da\, at the end of the experiment, all cells were uncapped manually and no individuals dead of American foulbrood were found remaining in the combs. This graph is based upon 622 spore-fed larvae and 322 waterfed larvae. Figure 5, presenting the pooled performance of four Van Scoy susceptible-line colonies, shows a contrast. Practically no removal of brood occurred throughout the experiment, and at the end, when the cells were uncapped, many of the spore-inoculated larvae were found dead of American foulbrood and remaining in the comb.

BEHAVIOR GENETICS OF NEST CLEANING IN HONEY BEES 115 This graph is based upon 753 spore-fed larvae and 383 water-fed checks. The pooled performance of five Fj colonies, presented in Figure 6, was very close to the Van Scoy susceptible-line performance. Here again a large number of larvae were involved 837 spore-fed, and 459 water-fed. These results indicate that the gene or genes for hygienic behavior were recessive. How many loci are involved is now a prominent question. To get an answer, 29 backcrosses of the F x generation were made. For the preceding test, x workers had been reared, but they do not normally reproduce. ~F X queens were necessary for reproduction. From such F x queens, drones were obtained. These drones, having developed from unfertilized eggs were gametes gametes with wings, in fact, and the capability of producing 6 to 1 million more genetically identical gametes in the form of sperms. Such a gamete, from an F x individual, when mated by artificial insemination to an inbred queen of the parental line, will produce a colony of bees of similar genotype. Twenty-nine such drones or gametes from two queens of the 1 generation were mated to 29 queens of the Brown line which were expected to be homozygous for the recessive genes for hygienic behavior. If 1/2 of the 29 colonies developed from the backcrosses were hygienic, one would conclude that one locus only was involved in the difference in hygienic behavior between the two lines. If only \/ A were hygienic, 2 loci would be involved; if i/ s, three loci. It was necessary, of course, to run controls along with the backcrosses, so 7 colonies from the hygienic Brown line were tested, along with 7 colonies of the non-hygienic Van Scoy parental line. Also 8 backcrosses were made to the Van Scoy line. Figure 7 presents graphs of the hygienic behavior of the seven hygienic-line control colonies and a graph of the pooled results. The pooled data in the upper left corner involve a total of 1383 spore-fed larvae and 698 water-fed checks. The same pattern seen previously in the Brown line is obvious. Not a single dead individual remained beyond the 12th day. An unexpectedly high number of check larvae were removed. There is no reason to think that these were dead; in fact it is likely that they were living larvae that for some reason were removed, and consequently they present a problem for the future. The individual responses of the seven colonies are plotted in the seven other graphs which show great uniformity with respect to the main point that Browns remove foulbrood quickly. Figure 8 shows seven cases of Van Scoy non-hygienic behavior, with the pooled results again, in the upper left. This too resembles the non-hygienic behavior already seen. Here it is seen that there was only a slight amount of removal of either spore or check larvae. In the end, about 14% of spore-fed larvae were left dead in the cells of the comb. Less than.5% of the check larvae were dead and left. A glance at the seven individual-colony results shows that all colonies left foulbroodkilled brood in the comb. Some colonies removed more spore-fed larvae than others, but this removal was erratic as to when it occurred. The 29 backcrosses to the Brown line broke up into 4 groups, each of which is presented in a separate figure. Figure 9 presents a group of 6 colonies that showed complete hygienic behavior. Five of the 6 showed behavior nearly identical to the Brown line. M 1423 was about 2 days later in removal than expected, but no dead larvae remained. Six colonies constitute about 14 of 29, which suggests that 2 loci are involved. None of the other 23 colonies showed hygienic behavior, but Figure 1 presents some results that were unexpected. Nine colonies left uncapped dead larvae in the combs. In fact the caps on cells containing dead brood had been removed in every case except for 1 cell in the M 1428 colony and about half such cells in the M 1444 colony. This result was in contrast to the remaining 14 colonies, all of which retained dead brood in capped cells.

116 WALTER C. ROTHENBUHLER There is some variation in the colonies of Figure 1. M 1416, in the upper left, removed to a considerable extent. Perhaps it belongs in the hygienic group, but it did not show complete hygienic behavior. The next seven cases seem to be more surely uncappers and non-removers. M 1444, in the lower right, was not completely uncapped at the end of the experiment but seems to belong in this group, nevertheless. In addition to the indication of two loci from the proportion of hygienic colonies, the 14 that uncapped suggests that one locus pertains to uncapping. If there is one locus for uncapping in a two-locus situation, the other one should be a locus for removing. This idea can be tested by examining the 14 remaining colonies to see whether or not they will remove dead larvae following human uncapping. Figure 11 shows a group of 6 colonies that showed very little hygienic behavior during the course of the experiment and z rspotc 1383 * ; * LCMECK M* 7 BROWNS POOLED DAT OF LARVAL UFC a 11 ENT REMOVI t rwone zoo ""icheck IOS H 142 ill _ Wp m I pki T ^ T T 1 T1 16 M ik i! 14 A OAT OF LARVAL UFE 1 _J"SPO«E 2 LCHECK I2i to 2) a «M M93 Si OAY OF LARVAL LIFE..5-JSPORE 164 ICHECK 75 M I45O BAS tcheck U 1494 7B 'n 1..«/SCORE 26 BAS 1.CHECK 114 o ' 4 2 OAT OF LARVAL UFE L " DAT OF LARVAL UFE FIG. 7. Responses of seven, Broun-linc, control colonies to the indicated numbers of spore-inoculated and check-treated larvae. Data from the seven different matings have been pooled in the upper lett giaph.

BEHAVIOR GENETICS OF NEST CLEANING IN HONEY BEES 117 retained a lot of dead brood. When the combs containing foulbrood-killed individuals were uncapped by us, reassigned to different colonies, and placed in them for testing, the colonies removed a large percentage of the dead brood in 2 days. As can be seen in Table 1, M 1412, M 1415, and M 1431 removed more than 9% in two days. The other three ranged from 53 to 75%. All of these colonies removed more on the second day of the removal test than on the first, indicating perhaps, that worker bees were being increasingly attracted to the job. The last group of 8 colonies (Fig. 12) showed very little hygienic behavior during the course of the experiment, and removed fewer uncapped individuals than the preceding group during the second test (Table 2). Matings 1429, 1432, and 'LCHECK 752 7 VAN ecovfi i SPORE CHECK II a» W b E M 141 JSPORE LCHECK us 64 CENT RlEWOVI C 4 t 1-1 u P 1 it Q IB J * BASE-f SI>OnE ICHECK H 1 4 5 9 184 IOI CENT c ii 1» O I I ^_ 1 z 1 /SPORE 273 'ICHECK 1*5 ; n.. 1 1 4 9 I I. 1 IE * 4 4 5B5 1 ~4» i~ $ BASE-f SP< ' t ' 31 LCHECK IO«SPHEI&3 BASE{; CHECK IOO M 1467 t s '. SPORE 2SZ { CHECK 147 /SPORE 175 LCHECK 6 4^ -f ^ OAY OF LARVAL UFE FIG. 8. Responses of seven, Van Scoy-line, control colonies to the indicated numbers of sporeinoculated and check-treated larvae. Data from the seven matings have been pooled in the upper left graph.

118 WALTER C. ROTHENBUHLER ["SPORE 224 ICHECK IJ5 SPORE n CHECK /SPORE 183 ICHECK 7 'TS -, JOAT OF LARVAL LIFE "Sir Pf'-f M-*-,r A A»»Y OF LARV1L UFE SPORE IBB B»SE{; CHECK 11 DAT OF LARVAL LIFE f w I I 1 2 S" SPORE 173 BASE{, CHECK 83 M 1438 ill Ml urr * 4 3 1 7 1 K) II 12^ 13 M 14 13 AFBKAD *l! - DAT OF LARVAL LIFE FIG. 9. Hygienic responses of 6 of 29 colonies developed in each case from a backcross of one drone from an F t queen to a Brown-line queen. AFIDCAD LEFT at B*T Or utnvu. LIFE IUT Of LARV*L UK ^4- M l Of LARVAl Uff I oat Of LIKVM. urc H" MY Of LAftVU. Ufl P 3 6. MT of u»vw. on l r., i'. f 1 -. Ul W LA*IM. D OUT OF LAMVlt FIG. 1. Responses of 9 of 29 colonies developed in each case from a backcross of one drone from an Fj queen to a Biown-linc queen. These beei aie designated as uncapptrs. UW

BEHAVIOR GENETICS OF NEST CLEANING IN HONEY BEES 119 SPORE n CHECK, -. r-. Of LARVAL LIFE DAT OF LARVAL UFE o it. s 1Ase J LCHECK UT M 143 I).-,,... EL ~~5 i 7 a» «J ii iz is OAT OF LARVAL LIFE -> j ', -. - - OAT OF LARVAL LIFE (SPORE?6 1CHECK i< B«^ICMECK 11 M 441 Z z 3 t P-4- A«OF LARVAL LIFE V~ io~ n ii FIG. II. Responses of 6 of the 29 backcross colonies. This group did not uncap, but did remove following human uncapping. 1434 did not remove a single dead individual, and M 1443 removed 3% in 2 days time. The other colonies ranged from 15 to 47% total removal. Although this group of 14 colonies is not as discretely separated into removers and non-removers as the whole group of 29 was separated into uncappers and nonuncappers, for the time being the 14 are TABLE 1. Percentage of dead individuals removed from uncapped comb on each day based on number present at beginning of day, and percentage of total sample removed. M. No. 1412 1415 143 1431 1436 1441 Dead individuals 47 38 3 38 16 29, Per cent removed * 1st day 2nd day Total 7 63 13 5 25 24 71 79 46 89 67 55 91 92 53 95 75 66 i 14 13 TABLE 2. Percentage of dead individuals removed from uncapped comb on each day based on number present at beginning of day, and percentage of total sample removed. M. No. 1413 1417 1429 1432 1433 1434 144 1443 Dead individuals 33 38 18 21 22 41 32 39, Per cent removed ^ 1st day 2nd day Total 9 29 6 3 33 26 23 1 39 47 23 16 3 interpreted as falling into two groups. This seems to be the simplest possible explanation and justifiable in the light of inadequate knowledge about the environmental factors that affect removal rate. The backcrosses of sons of F x queens to the Van Scoy-line queens present something of a problem. None of these colonies

12 WALTER C. ROTHENBUHLER was expected to show hygienic behavior because Van Scoy queens were expected to be homozygous for dominant genes for non-hygienic behavior. As Figure 13 shows, M 149 showed a typical pattern of hygienic behavior. This result is beyond explanation at present. No F x colony has ever displayed complete hygienic behavior and in this case the backcross of an F x to the Van Scoy line does so. We cannot disregard this result, regardless of how much we would like to, but we are basing the genetic hypothesis on the other data. Hypothesis From the foregoing, the hypothesis presented in Figure 14 has been developed. Hygienic behavior (in the upper left corner) depends upon homozygosity for two recessive genes one a gene (designated u) for uncapping of cells containing dead larvae, the other a gene (designated r) for removal of dead larvae. A cross between JSPORE 2SI CHECK I2O 1. SPORE CHECK s SPORE»»SE-f '" 126 ICHECK 2 11 K B i s «-, -f PiPft A A- O«T OF LARVAL UFE A T L ^. /SPORE 283 ""HcHECK l«rspone 244 LCHECK irr m i: Pi OAY OF LARVAL UFE M 19.7 W II OK E-T 5 ""' I CHECK l * CENT a: " Tn t P L rr t r t OAT OF LARVAL UFE T^ rspore 11 ICHECK 35 MSC LCHECK 63, ; I-. P, t -» s-v-a-v-t- A A A A OAY OF LARVAL UFE FIG. 12. Responses of the 8 baclaross colonies uhich neither uncapped nor irmo\c<!.

BEHAVIOR GENETICS OF NEST CLEANING IN HONEY BEES 121 the two opposite homozygous types would result in an F t which is non-hygienic. An Fj queen would produce four kinds of drones or gametes which, backcrossed to the parental type showing hygienic behavior, would result in four kinds of backcross colonies in a 1:1:1:1 ratio. In this case a 6:9:6:8 ratio was obtained. One of the most attractive features of this hypothesis is the ease with which it can be tested. One can predict how each of these classes should behave in further breeding. Queens and drones from the hygienic group should breed true for hygienic behavior. The uncapping group should breed true for uncapping, and the removing group for removing. For the time being, however, most effort is going into studies of the environmental factors that may conceivably affect the colony's response to dead brood. For these investigations, the parental lines have been and are being used. It is equally important to test the x generation for its stability and variability under various environmental conditions. 3 It In I t- ' C I. PERCE BASE T CHECK 73 M I4OJ SPORE CHECK S,. B J-SPORE NO T.CHECK 53 1 1 ft! - 1 1 1 n.. T to II ij i s M is DAT OF LARVAL UFE 4 1 1 A T. T* OAT OF LARVAL UFE TfW -r i BAS /SPRE243 B ** LCHECK 144 M 145 {SS'S iuj a o a. t '^g? t 4 J1 DAT OF LARVAL UFE LCHECK I I4O APSDCAO " " FIG. 13. Responses of 8 colonies developed in each case by backcrossing one drone from an queen to a queen from the Van Scoy line.

122 WALTER C. ROTHENBUHLER TABLE 3. Frequency distribution of four groups of colonies according to the total number of limes the beekeeper was stung while engaged in the Mine operations with each under similar conditions. Fourteen visits were made to each colony. Total number oc slings Type of colony 12 3 4 5 6 7 11 15 19 2 21 23 26 31 Seven Van Scoy colonies None 6 1 hygienic Seven Brown colonies All hygienic Twenty-nine colonies from back- 9 9 2 3 12 crosses of F t to Brown line Six hygienic The six hygienic colonies from 2 3 above backcrosses STINGING BEHAVIOR In the hygienic behavior experiment, observations on several other characteristics were made. One of the most obvious was stinging behavior. Table 3 presents the data. In the course of 98 visits to the 7 Van Scoy colonies, we were stung only once. In the course of 98 similar visits on the same days to the 7 Brown colonies, we were stung 143 times. The fact that this was an obvious characteristic needs no emphasis, and it may be said that sophisticated quantitative measurements were not really necessary to alert us that we had a real, line difference. It has been thought by most people that disease-resistant bees are always cross, and logically explained further that keeping the brood nest free of dead larvae and defending the colony against the beekeeper is a manifestation of the same general characteristic a high level of vigor in the bees. If hygienic behavior and stinging behavior are due to the same underlying characteristic, namely vigor, the hygienic colonies among the backcrosses ought to PARENTAL TYPES BACKCROSS TYPES XL Jm HYGIENIC UNCAP ONLY 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 be stingers. This was not so. There were 2 hygienic colonies that never stung, 3 stung once each, and one stung 7 times. The colony that stung equally as much as the parental, hygienic line was non-hygienic. The stinging behavior of all 29 of the backcross colonies indicates that more than one or two loci are involved in this behavior difference. In these experiments, colony behavior characteristics which appeared together in the parental lines have separated in the backcross colonies. We are accustomed to this result among individuals, but here it is happening among colonies made up of many individuals. This can occur only when the individuals making up the colony are genetically similar, and they can be genetically similar only when the mother of the colony is highly inbred and has been mated to a single haploid drone. With the exception of the sex locus, the mother makes identical (or nearly identical) genetic contributions to every worker bee, and the father's contribution is expected to be genetically identical in every case. This inbred queen - single drone technique promises to be useful in the analysis of colony behavior characteristics (Rothenbuhler, 196). NON-HYGJENIC: ^*" \ "*-- Jt«i. REMOVEONLY \ NON-HYGIENIC \ BEHAVIOR:» * ' N ^ ~ NON-HYGIENIC FIG. 14. Genetic hypothesis offered in explanation of different responses to AFB-killed brood observed in 63 colonies of bees. SUMMARY Two lines of honey bees differ greatly in the response of their colonies to brood killed by the disease American foulbrood and in the number of times they sting the

BEHAVIOR GENETICS OF NEST CLEANING IN HONEY BEES 123 beekeeper during a colony inspection. The Brown line removed dead larvae quickly (hygienic behavior), and in one experiment in which counts were made, stung the beekeeper about 1.5 times per colony visit. The Van Scoy line left dead larvae in the brood nest throughout the course of the experiment and stung the beekeeper about.1 times per colony visit. The F 1 generation allowed dead larvae to remain in the brood nest as did the Van Scoy line. No observations were recorded on the stinging behavior of the F x. A technique of mating single drones from 1 queens to inbred queens of the original lines was employed to secure colonies with genetically similar worker bees in each for colony tests. Colonies resulting from such backcrosses to the Brown line were of four types: (1) uncappers of cells and removers of dead brood contained therein, (2) uncappers only, (3) removers only after human uncapping, (4) neither uncappers nor removers. From these results a two-locus hypothesis has been developed. It states that uncapping of a cell containing dead brood is dependent upon homozygosity for a single recessive gene (designated u), and removing is dependent upon homozygosity for a single recessive gene (designated r). The hypothesis can be tested by breeding and investigating further generations within each phenotypic class, but before this is done, information is needed on the effect of certain environmental factors. Such information is being collected presently. The same backcrosses that were tested for hygienic behavior indicated that stinging behavior is dependent upon more than one or two loci. Stinging behavior and hygienic behavior assorted independently in the backcross colonies. This result shows that the two behaviors depend upon different genetic bases. REFERENCES Boch, R. 1957. Rassenmassige Unterschiede in den Tanzen der Honigbiene (Apis mellifica L.). Z. vergleich. Physiol. 39:289-32. Butler, Colin G. 1954. The world of the honeybee. Collins, St. James Place, London. Frisch, K. von. 195. Bees, their vision, chemical senses, and language. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, New York. Grout, Roy A. (ed.). 1963. The hive and the honey bee. Dadant & Sons, Hamilton, Illinois. Jones, Ronald L., and Walter C. Rothenbuhler. 1964. Behavior genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees. II. Responses of two inbred lines to various amounts of cyanide-killed brood. Animal Behaviour (in press). Lindauer, Martin. 1961. Communication among social bees. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Michener, Charles D., and Mary H. Michener. 1951. American social insects. D. Van Nostrand Company, Inc., New York, N. Y. Park, O. W. 1936. Disease resistance and American foulbrood. Am. Bee J. 74:12-14. Ribbands, C. R. 1953. The behaviour and social life of honeybees. Bee Research Association Ltd., London. Root, J. A. (ed.). 1962. The ABC and XYZ of bee culture. The A. I. Root Company, Medina, Ohio. Rothenbuhler, Walter C. 1958. Genetics of a behavior difference in honey bees. Proc. 1th Inter Cong, of Genetics, Vol. 11:242.. 196. A technique for studying genetics of colony behavior in honey bees. Am. Bee J. 1: 176-198.. 1964. Behavior genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees. I. Responses of four inbred lines to disease-killed brood. Animal Behaviour (in press). Rothenbuhler, Walter C, and Victor C. Thompson. 1955. Resistance to American foulbrood in honey bees. I. Differential survival of larvae of different genetic lines. J. Econ. Entomol. 49:47-475. Thompson, Victor C. 1964. Behavior genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees. III. Effect of age of adult bees on their response to disease-killed brood. J. Apic. Res. (in press). Woodrow, A. W., and E. C. Hoist. 1942. The mechanism of colony resistance to American foulbrood. J. of Econ. Entomol. 35:327-33.