Escape Behaviors and Flight Initiation Distance in the Common Water Snake Nerodia sipedon

Similar documents
Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Objectives: Outline: Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles. Characteristics of Amphibians. Types and Numbers of Amphibians

Uncertainty about future predation risk modulates monitoring behavior from refuges in lizards

8/19/2013. Who eats herps? Topic 20: Predators. Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps? Who eats herps?

Reptiles Notes. Compiled by the Davidson College Herpetology Laboratory

Departamento de Biologia Animal, Universidad de Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain. INTRODUCTION

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour

Ecological Archives E A2

Prey or predator? Body size of an approaching animal affects decisions to attack or escape

reproductive life History and the effects of sex and season on morphology in CRoTALus oreganus (northern PaCifiC RATTLESNAKES)

Animal Adaptations. Structure and Function

Squamates of Connecticut

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

Influence of predation pressure on the escape behaviour of Podarcis muralis lizards

Plestiodon (=Eumeces) fasciatus Family Scincidae

STATE-DEPENDENT AND RISK-SENSITIVE ESCAPE DECISIONS IN A FOSSORIAL REPTILE, THE AMPHISBAENIAN BLANUS CINEREUS

Juvenile Collared Lizards Adjust Tail Display Frequency in Response to Variable Predatory Threat

Bio4009 : Projet de recherche/research project

Field Herpetology Final Guide

HIGLEY UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT INSTRUCTIONAL ALIGNMENT. Zoology Quarter 3. Animal Behavior (Duration 2 Weeks)

Taseko Prosperity Gold-Copper Project. Appendix 5-6-D

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

Behaviour and spatial ecology of Gilbert s dragon Lophognathus gilberti (Agamidae: Reptilia)

NAME: DATE: SECTION:

Flexibility in antipredatory behavior allows wall lizards to cope with multiple types of predators

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

Alligators. very long tail, and a head with very powerful jaws.

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis)

by Andrew L. Shiels, Leader, Nongame and Endangered Species Unit

Management of bold wolves

REPORT OF ACTIVITIES TURTLE ECOLOGY RESEARCH REPORT Crescent Lake National Wildlife Refuge 31 May to 4 July 2017

Maritime Shipping on the Great Lakes and the Lake Erie Water Snake

T HE recent and interesting paper by Alexander F. Skutch (1962) stimulated

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

Marine Reptiles. Four types of marine reptiles exist today: 1. Sea Turtles 2. Sea Snakes 3. Marine Iguana 4. Saltwater Crocodile

Biota of the Lehigh Gap Wildlife Refuge Reptiles and Amphibians

Evolution. Evolution is change in organisms over time. Evolution does not have a goal; it is often shaped by natural selection (see below).

Fact Sheet: Oustalet s Chameleon Furcifer oustaleti

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

SECTION 3 IDENTIFYING ONTARIO S EASTERN MASSASAUGA RATTLESNAKE AND ITS LOOK-ALIKES

Writing: Lesson 31. Today the students will be learning how to write more advanced middle paragraphs using a variety of elaborative techniques.

4 Many species of mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians and fish 940L. Source 1 Habitats

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

Faculty Mentor, Department of Integrative Biology, Oklahoma State University

10/11/2010. Kevin Enge

Nest-site selection in Eastern hognose snakes (Heterodon platirhinos) Casey Peet-Paré

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

ethology Ethology RESEARCH PAPER

8/19/2013. What is a community? Topic 21: Communities. What is a community? What are some examples of a herp species assemblage? What is a community?

Activity for Biology. Background Information on Lake Erie water snake and round goby:

Seasonal Shifts in Reproductive Investment of Female Northern Grass Lizards ( Takydromus septentrionalis

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

Rubber Boas in Radium Hot Springs: Habitat, Inventory, and Management Strategies

Signalling displays during predator prey interactions in a Puerto Rican anole, Anolis cristatellus

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

THE concept that reptiles have preferred

DO URBAN ENVIRONMENTS INFLUENCE ANTIPREDATOR AND FORAGING BEHAVIOR OF THE LIZARD ANOLIS CRISTATELLUS?

Grade Level: 1-2. Next Generation Sunshine State Standards SC.1.L.14.1; SC.1.L.17.1; SC.1.N.1.1 SC.2.L.17.1; SC.2.L.17.2; SC.2.N.1.

Reptilian Physiology

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Iguana Technical Assistance Workshop. Presented by: Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission

Coloring Book. Southern Piedmont Wildlife.

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Coloring Book. Southern Piedmont Wildlife.

Status of the Nile Monitor in South Florida. Todd Campbell, Ph.D., Assistant Professor Department of Biology, University of Tampa

Squamates of Connecticut. May 11th 2017

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

Species Results From Database Search

Effects of Natural Selection

MATERNAL NEST-SITE CHOICE AND OFFSPRING FITNESS IN A TROPICAL SNAKE (TROPIDONOPHIS MAIRII, COLUBRIDAE)

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF TWO SPECIES OF EGERNIA (SCINCIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

Grade Level: 3-5. Next Generation Sunshine State Standards SC.3.L.15.1 SC.4.L.16.2; SC.4.L.17.4 SC.5.L.15.1; SC.5.L.17.1

I LOVE MY DRAGONS! Dragons of Sydney Harbour Factsheet Kids Version

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF CTENOPHORUS CAUDICINCTUS (AGAMIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

Like mother, like daughter: inheritance of nest-site

*Using the 2018 List. Use the image below to answer question 6.

There are 35 phyla of animals These phyla can be classified into two groups (vertebrates or invertebrates) based on external and internal physical

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

ADAPTATION IN ANIMALS. 1. Which body feature of a frog MAINLY helps it to capture a flying insect? Ans

JoJoKeKe s Herpetology Exam

The Origin of Species: Lizards in an Evolutionary Tree

Reptile Round Up. An Educator s Guide to the Program

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

6-3.4 Physical Responses

Diversity of Animals

Identifying Bird and Reptile Vulnerabilities to Climate Change

Consequences of Extended Egg Retention in the Eastern Fence Lizard (Sceloporus undulatus)

Announcements. Results: due today at 5pm for weekend feedback, otherwise due at Monday at 9am

Effect of Tail Loss on Sprint Speed and Growth in Newborn Skinks, Niveoscincus metallicus

A.13 BLAINVILLE S HORNED LIZARD (PHRYNOSOMA BLAINVILLII)

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

Northern Copperhead Updated: April 8, 2018

Biodiversity and Extinction. Lecture 9

Escaping predators on vertical surfaces: Lacerta perspicillata in limestone quarries of Lithaca

ACTIVITY #6: TODAY S PICNIC SPECIALS ARE

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

Reptiles and amphibian behaviour

Animal Biodiversity. Teacher Resources - High School (Cycle 1) Biology Redpath Museum

Transcription:

Journal of Herpetology, Vol. 42, No. 3, pp. 493 500, 2008 Copyright 2008 Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles Escape Behaviors and Flight Initiation Distance in the Common Water Snake Nerodia sipedon WILLIAM E. COOPER JR., 1 OMAR ATTUM, AND BRUCE KINGSBURY Department of Biology, Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne, Fort Wayne, Indiana 46805, USA ABSTRACT. Escape behavior of the natricine snake Nerodia sipedon when approached by a human was studied in populations in wetlands in Ohio and Michigan. Snakes responded by dropping from vegetation, but not immediately fleeing (44%), by diving beneath the water (37%), or by swimming away with the head above the surface (19%). Flight initiation distance (distance between predator and prey when escape begins) was significantly greater for individuals that dropped than those that dove or swam on the surface. This novel finding suggests that dropping may position snakes well to escape should approach continue and perhaps might serve as a pursuit-deterrent signal. Adult females were warier (had greater flight initiation distance) than adult males or juveniles. Several hypotheses to account for this difference are discussed. Flight initiation distance increased with starting distance (distance between predator and prey when the predator begins to approach). This effect was much stronger than in the few lizard species studied. We propose that degree of risk assessed by snakes may change during approaches because a predator that moves directly toward a snake for a longer distance is more likely to have detected the snake and be attacking than a predator that approaches over a shorter distance. Increase in perceived risk would account for greater flight initiation distance associated with greater starting distance. Flight initiation distance for basking snakes did not vary with air temperature or perch height, contrary to previous findings for body temperature and perch height. Reasons for these findings are discussed. Diverse aspects of snake antipredatory behavior have been studied (e.g., Greene, 1988), arguably most notable among them envenomation (Greene, 1988), rattling and other warning displays (Klauber, 1956; Bellairs, 1970), aposematism and mimicry (Smith, 1975, 1977; Greene and McDiarmid, 1981; Pough, 1988; Brodie, 1993), death-feigning (Myers and Arata, 1961; Platt, 1969), and crypsis (Cott, 1940; Sweet, 1985). Some aspects of escape behavior, including ontogeny of escape destinations (e.g., Sweet, cited in Greene, 1988), effects of time of day, proximity to refuge, and portion of body attacked (Langkilde et al., 2004) on tendency to attempt escape (Duvall et al., 1985; Khan and Tasnim, 1986), and others have been studied. Less is known about factors affecting flight initiation distance, the distance between an approaching predator and prey when the prey begins to flee. According to escape theory (Ydenberg and Dill, 1986; Cooper and Frederick, 2007a), flight initiation distance increases as expected fitness cost of not fleeing increases and decreases as expected fitness cost of escaping increases. Rather than fleeing immediately upon detecting a predator, prey are expected by these models to monitor the predator s approach, fleeing when 1 Corresponding Author. E-mail: cooperw@ipfw. edu cost of remaining equals cost of escaping (Ydenberg and Dill, 1986) or when the prey s expected fitness after the encounter is maximized (Cooper and Frederick, 2007a). Studies of numerous predation risk factors and opportunity cost factors (e.g., loss of foraging and courting opportunities) have confirmed these predictions of escape theory. Predation risk factors for which the predictions have been confirmed include predator approach speed and directness (Burger and Gochfeld, 1990; Cooper, 1997a; Cooper et al., 2003), distance from refuge (Dill, 1990; Cooper, 1997b), turn direction (Cooper, 1998a), conspicuousness (Heatwole, 1968; Cooper, 1998b,c), and others. The prediction that flight initiation decreases as cost of escaping increases has been confirmed when fleeing entails loss of opportunity to feed (Cooper, 2000; Cooper et al., 2003, 2006; Cooper and Pérez-Mellado, 2004) or engage in fitnessenhancing social activities (Díaz-Uriarte, 1999; Cooper, 1999; Cooper and Wilson, 2007). Much less is known about factors that affect flight initiation distance in snakes than other lizards. Flight initiation distance increased with body temperature in Regina septemvittata (Layne and Ford, 1984). Because the range of body temperatures was about 15 32uC, snakes at the lowest temperatures were slowed and may have relied more on crypsis to avoid detection than did warmer, faster snakes. Flight initiation distance did not vary with water temperature

494 W. E. COOPER JR. ET AL. in another natricine, Nerodia sipedon (Weatherhead and Robertson, 1992). Flight initiation distance of basking N. sipedon increased as perch height decreased, perhaps because snakes are less vulnerable to predators when higher above water. Reproductive state affects flight initiation distance in Tropidonophis mairii (Natricinae, Colubridae; Brown and Shine, 2004). Flight initiation distance is greater in gravid than nongravid females because gravid females are slower while escaping because of the added mass of the clutch. The increase in flight initiation distance presumably compensates for increased predation risk (Brown and Shine, 2004). Interestingly, male T. mairii permit closer approach during the breeding period than nonreproductive males, which may be attributable to decreased vigilance (Brown and Shine, 2004). In another natricine, Thamnophis sirtalis, flight initiation distance of males is shorter during courtship than afterwards (Shine et al., 2000, 2003). This relationship also might be the result of decreased vigilance (Shine et al., 2003). Other variables that may affect flight initiation distance in N. sipedon are number of approaching people, directness of approach by people, and habituation to people (Burger, 2001). However, in the latter study, distance to respond, which was not clearly defined, was measured rather than flight initiation distance, and the approach protocol differed from that in other studies of escape in that it included both indirect and direct approach during different portions of some trials. Possible relationships between flight initiation distance and types of escape behaviors used are largely unknown in snakes. Another factor that affects flight initiation distance in some birds (Blumstein, 2003) and lizards (Cooper, 2005) is starting distance (distance between predator and prey when the predator begins to approach). It is important to measure starting distance in circumstances and taxa for which it is known to affect flight initiation distance if only for use as a covariate during assessment of effects of risk and costs on flight initiation distance. Effects of starting distance should also be studied in taxa for which they are unknown because they seem to vary both among taxa and with predator approach speed (Blumstein, 2003; Cooper, 2005, 2007). We studied escape behaviors, starting distance, and flight initiation distance in the Common Water Snake, N. sipedon. Our goals were to examine several types of escape behaviors and factors that might affect flight initiation distance. We studied the following: (1) relationships between escape behaviors and factors that might affect the types of escape behaviors used, as well as their relationships to flight initiation distance; (2) the relationship between starting distance and flight initiation distance; (3) effects of air temperature, perch height, and initial distance from water on flight initiation distance; and (4) possible differences among age and sex groups in frequencies of escape behaviors and flight initiation distance. MATERIALS AND METHODS Study Sites. We studied escape reactions of the semiaquatic N. sipedon in northwestern Ohio and southern Michigan on public and private land (Attum et al., 2007). The study site contains a variety of wetland sizes and types. The upland landscape consists of forested, shrub-scrub, old field, roads, and agricultural and residential habitats. We surveyed 111 wetlands three times each between 28 April and 16 June 2005, typically in early morning and late afternoon, when snakes would be more likely to be observed basking. We believe the probability of pseudoreplication by observing the same snake twice was low based on pilot mark and recapture studies of a similar species, Nerodia erythrogaster neglecta, in the same area. Data Collection. To study escape responses, an investigator simulated threat from a predator by approaching snakes. Although this technique might fail to reveal defensive responses specific to particular natural terrestrial predators (Stuart-Fox et al., 2006), such as birds and large mammals, it has several advantages. First, it permits rapid data collection using standardized methods of approach. Second, it avoids ethical issues that arise when staging encounters using real predators. Third, it allows data to be collected efficiently in habitats where mechanical models of predators are difficult or impossible to operate. It seems reasonable for snakes to respond to human beings as if they were predators because of long-term slaughter of snakes by human beings that presumably subjected snakes to natural selection to avoid being killed by people. Even in many taxa not subject to such frequent killing by human beings, predictions for features of antipredatory behavior included in escape and refuge use theories (escape, Ydenberg and Dill, 1986; Cooper and Frederick, 2007a; refuge use, Martín and López, 1999; Cooper and Frederick, 2007b) have been confirmed repeatedly. The most extensive studies have be done in lizards (e.g., Rand, 1964; Bulova, 1994; Martín and López, 1996; Martín et al., 2003), but predictions of escape and refuge use theories have also been confirmed using human simulated predators in some studies of other reptiles (turtles, (López et al.,

ESCAPE BEHAVIOR OF NERODIA SIPEDON 495 2005a; Martín et al., 2005b) and amphibians (frogs, Martín et al., 2005a) and in a wide range of vertebrate (fish, Dill, 1990; Metcalfe and Steele, 2001; birds, Burger and Gochfeld, 1981; Martín et al., 2006; but for a possible exception, see Blumstein et al., 2004; mammals, Dill and Houtman, 1989; Blumstein and Pelletier, 2005), and invertebrate taxa (crabs, Hemmi, 2005a,b; insects, Cooper, 2006; spiders, Nelson and Formanowicz, 2005; serpulid worms, Dill and Fraser, 1997). Focal surveys were made by walking along the wetland s perimeter and wading through the water. When it was not possible to walk around a portion of a wetland, we used binoculars to survey that area. Once detected, snakes were approached directly. The point where the observer first detected the snake and the observer s location when the snake fled was marked. Starting distance and flight initiation distance was measured in 0.1-m intervals. Approach speed was standardized by placing the heel of the shoe on the moving foot in contact with the tip of the shoe on the planted foot, then moving the other foot forward in the same way, alternating feet at the fastest pace allowing the investigator to maintain stability and a straight path. The same investigator made all approaches. Air temperature and perch height also were recorded. The observer stopped approaching as soon as a snake began to flee. Given the sexual dimorphism (females are conspicuously larger than males), it was not necessary to capture extremely large individuals to sex them. We captured smaller individuals to determine age and sex categories. No sex was recorded for smaller individuals that escaped (34% of all snakes). We classified individuals with snout vent length less than 30 cm as juveniles. For each snake, we recorded initial type of escape behavior: dropping from elevated perch (but not moving away); diving underwater; swimming away along the shoreline; and swimming away across the wetland. Because we detected no differences associated with swimming away along the bank or across the wetland, we combined these two behaviors. Analysis. To ascertain whether age and sex categories differed in types of escape behaviors, we used a single-factor Chi-square test. Simple and multiple regressions were conducted to assess relationships between flight initiation distance and starting distance, air temperature, and perch height. Possible differences in flight initiation distance among age-sex groups and among types of escape behaviors were examined using single-factor analyses of variance and analyses of covariance with starting distance as covariate. A factorial analysis could not FIG. 1. Proportions of individuals of Nerodia sipedon for which flight initiation data were collected that performed various types of escape behaviors. DROP dropped from vegetation into water, DIVE swam completely under water, SWIM swam on surface. be conducted because observations were missing for some cells. Levene s tests were conducted to examine the assumption of homogeneity of variance. When significant heterogeneity of variance was found, data were logarithmically transformed prior to analysis. Significance tests are two-tailed with a 5 0.05. RESULTS The snakes for which types of escape behaviors and flight initiation distances were recorded employed three distinct escape behaviors more than once (Fig. 1): diving (N 5 15); swimming away with head out of water (N 5 29); and dropping from vegetation without further escape (N 5 34). Frequencies were large enough to test for sex differences in adults: diving (six males, eight females); swimming away with head above water (10 males, 13 females); and dropping from vegetation (11 males, 10 females). Frequencies of these three escape behaviors did not differ significantly between the sexes of adults (x 2 2 5 0.45, P. 0.10). Numbers of juveniles were too low for detailed statistical comparison of behavioral frequencies with adults, but seven of 10 juveniles swam away with head above water, two dropped from vegetation, and one dove. The frequency of swimming away was somewhat higher for juveniles than for adults, but a comparison of frequency of swimming away versus pooled other types of escape behaviors detected no significant difference (Fisher exact P, 0.094). Variances of flight initiation distance were heterogeneous among age-sex groups using raw

496 W. E. COOPER JR. ET AL. FIG. 2. Flight initiation distance was greater in female than male Nerodia sipedon; the differences between juveniles and adults of each sex were not significant. M male, F female, J juvenile. data (Levene s F 2,79 5 3.99; P, 0.03) but were homogeneous using logarithmically transformed data (Levene s F 2,79 5 0.52; P. 0.10). Flight initiation distance for transformed data differed significantly among age-sex groups (F 2,79 5 6.47, P, 0.0025; Fig. 2). Females (N 5 41) exhibited significantly greater flight initiation distance than males (N 5 31; P, 0.0021). Flight initiation distance of juveniles (N 5 10) did not differ significantly from that of either sex of adults (P. 0.10). Results were the same with starting distance as a covariate, and starting distance did not vary among the agesex groups (F 2,80 5 0.69, P. 0.10; variances were homogeneous: Levene s F 2,80 5 1.42). Variances of flight initiation distance among types of escape behaviors were significantly heterogeneous (Levene s F 2,75 5 6.64, P, 0.003), but variances were homogeneous for logarithmically transformed data (Levene s F 2,75 5 1.10, P. 0.10). The types of escape behavior performed varied significantly with flight initiation distance (F 2,75 5 6.04; P, 0.004; Fig. 3). Flight initiation distance was significantly greater for snakes that dropped from vegetation than for those that swam along the bank (P, 0.01) or dove under water (P, 0.02). The difference in flight initiation distance between snakes that dove and swam along the bank was not significant. The same pattern of findings was obtained using starting distance as a covariate. Flight initiation distance for basking snakes was strongly affected by starting distance (F 1,93 5 45.44, P, 1 3 10 26 ; r 5 0.57). A similar relationship was found for snakes initially in water (F 1,16 5 5.39, P, 0.034; r 5 0.50). The relationship for all snakes is given by FID 5 0.48 SD 2 0.22, where FID is flight initiation distance and SD is starting distance (Fig. 4). For basking FIG. 3. Flight initiation distance was significantly greater for Nerodia sipedon that escaped by diving than for those that swam along the bank or dropped from vegetation into water. DROP dropped from vegetation into water, DIVE swam completely under water, SWIM swam on surface. snakes, flight initiation distance was not significantly related to air temperature (F 1,91 5 1.45, P. 0.10) or perch height (F 1,72 5 0.00, P. 0.10). DISCUSSION Types of escape behavior and their frequencies did not differ among age and sex groups. It remains possible that other aspects of defense such as location in the habitat and distance from refuges differ among ages, sexes, and reproductive states. Options for escape behaviors may have been determined largely by the locations of snakes when approached and characteristics of the surrounding habitat or by individual differences among snakes in preferred escape behaviors. Many reasons for the greater flight initiation distance by females than males are possible. FIG. 4. Flight initiation distance in Nerodia sipedon increased as starting distance increased.

ESCAPE BEHAVIOR OF NERODIA SIPEDON 497 Females, which are substantially larger than males, may assess greater risk of being detected by a predator at a fixed distance than do males. If gravid females have greater flight initiation distance than nongravid females because litter mass causes escape speed to decrease as in T. mairii (Brown and Shine, 2004), inclusion of gravid females in our study might have raised mean flight initiation distance for all females. We did not record reproductive state, but any gravid females would have been in early stages when embryonic mass is small. Another hypothesis is that flight initiation distance decreases in reproductively active males. This hypothesis for the seasonally breeding N. sipedon predicts findings analogous to those for male T. mairii, in which most males are reproductively active throughout the year (Brown and Shine, 2004). Because our data were collected during early seasonal activity and the beginning of the breeding season, some males were presumably reproductively active and others not. Brown and Shine s (2004) interpretation of greater flight initiation distance by gravid than nongravid female T. mairii is consistent with escape theory. Ydenberg and Dill s (1986) graphical model and optimal escape theory (Cooper and Frederick, 2007) predict that, if other aspects of escape strategy are not altered to compensate for increased risk, flight initiation distance increases as cost of remaining (not fleeing) increases. At a given distance, the cost of remaining is greater for a slower, gravid female than for a nongravid female. In natricines, decreased flight initiation distance in reproductive versus nonreproductive males and in courting than noncourting males have been attributed to decreased vigilance (Shine et al., 2003b; Brown and Shine, 2004). This is certainly a plausible hypothesis, especially during courtship. Another possibility is that opportunity costs are greater for reproductive than nonreproductive and courting than noncourting males. Cost of losing a fertilization opportunity or a combination of that cost with decreased vigilance might account for the differences we observed in males. For the escape cost hypothesis, shorter flight initiation distance by reproductive than nonreproductive male T. mairii may be a consequence of a cost of giving up searching for potential mates or remaining near females not observed by the researchers. Flight initiation distance of male Broad- Headed Skinks (Plestidon laticeps) is shortened during mate-guarding, courting, and aggressive behavior despite awareness of a nearby predator (Cooper, 1999). Opportunity costs of abandoning a female during mate-guarding or courtship or abandoning a rival male that might fertilize nearby females explain the shorter flight initiation distance. In the present study, difference in vigilance between sexes and reproductive states were unlikely because all snakes had clear views of the investigator. Further research is needed to establish the roles of vigilance and escape costs in flight initiation distance of male natricines. Variation in types of escape behavior with flight initiation distance is a novel finding. Although snakes that dropped exhibited greater flight initiation distance than those that swam away with head above water or dove, they did not move away after dropping. Presumably, they would have fled using one of the other behaviors had the investigator continued to approach. Snakes elevated in vegetation may be at greater risk because they are easier to detect there, accounting for greater flight initiation distance. Dropping may position them for effective escape when they are in exposed, elevated locations. Although we did not collect relevant data, snakes basking in vegetation appeared to be more likely to drop during cool periods when swimming or diving would reduce body temperature. In this way they would avoid high thermal escape costs unless forced to swim. An alternative hypothesis not mutually exclusive with the others is that dropping is a pursuit-deterrent signal. Why do some snakes dive when others swim on the surface? Diving may occur when assessed risk is greater because it renders the escaping snake more difficult to detect or invisible. In the lizard Lacerta monticola, pronounced variation in wariness exists because of individual differenceces, even under similar predation threat (López et al., 2005b). If similar variation occurs, warier snakes dive when bolder ones escape on the surface. Or snakes that dove may have been in locations exposing them to greater risk. The effect of starting distance on flight initiation distance is far greater in N. sipedon than in other lizards studied to date. At approach speeds comparable to that in our study of N. sipedon, starting distance had no effect on flight initiation distance in Leiocephalus carinatus (Cooper, 2007), Sceloporus virgatus, or Urosaurus ornatus (Cooper, 2005). The magnitude of the effect in N. sipedon is similar to that of some bird species: The proportion of variance in flight initiation distance explained by starting distance was higher in 16 of 58 avian species from Tasmania and eastern Australia (Blumstein, 2003) than the R 2 5 0.48 for N. sipedon. For birds that take flight frequently, increase in flight initiation distance with starting distance

498 W. E. COOPER JR. ET AL. could be an artifact of the longer time needed to approach from greater starting distance. Blumstein (2003) proposed that cost of monitoring the approach increases with starting distance because prey must cease or devote less attention to current activity longer during longer approaches. Thus, cost of not fleeing increases as starting distance increases, causing flight initiation distance to increase. Although reasonable for active birds, this explanation seems unlikely for a sedentary snake such as N. sipedon. Snakes in our study did not forage or engage in social behaviors, and basking need not be interrupted to monitor an approaching predator. To account for the effect of starting distance, cost of not fleeing must increase or cost of fleeing decrease as starting distance increases. We favor the hypothesis that the strong effect of starting distance in N. sipedon is a consequence of a change in perceived risk. When a predator moves on a straight path toward a prey, flight initiation distance is greater than during approach on an indirect path that bypasses the prey (Cooper, 1997a, 2003a,b; Cooper et al., 2003). During direct approach, prey assess that risk is greater than during indirect approach because the predator is more likely to have detected or to detect the prey and attack if it continues moving in its current direction. Similarly, a predator that continues to approach a prey directly for a greater distance is more likely to have detected and be currently attacking or to continue on its path, leading it to detect and attack the prey. For prey incorporating these factors into risk assessment, flight initiation distance would increase as starting distance increases. Study of effects of starting distance on flight initiation distance has barely begun, but starting distance clearly is an important factor in diverse reptiles. Causes of the relationship are as yet uncertain, and both its causes and importance are likely to vary among prey taxa. Studies of effects of starting distance in additional prey taxa, experiments designed to distinguish between effects caused by increased monitoring costs, assessment of greater risk associated with long starting distances, and studies of effects of movement rates of undisturbed prey in relation to time required for approaches offer fertile ground for future research. Body temperature strongly affected flight initiation distance in the natricine R. septemvittata (Layne and Ford, 1984), but air temperature did not affect it in N. sipedon despite a wide range of air temperatures (9.4 30.6uC). This finding is consistent with lack of effects of air and water temperatures on flight initiation distance in this species (Weatherhead and Robertson, 1992). Nevertheless, an effect of body temperature on flight initiation distance could have been obscured in two ways. Behavioral thermoregulation may have restricted variation in body temperature. Second, a large majority of observations were made between 16 and 25uC, reducing the likelihood of detecting a relationship. The lack of relationship between perch height and flight initiation distance in our study differs from the negative linear relationship observed in N. sipedon by Weatherhead and Robertson (1992). In their study, snakes were perched in willow trees at heights up to 4 m, whereas perch height varied much less in our study (0 2 m). Some snakes were at ground level and others perched on various objects rather than trees directly over water. Thus, there is no contradiction between the findings. The relationship may pertain only to snakes in elevated basking sites over water. Acknowledgments. This work was a byproduct of funding by the U. S. Fish and Wildlife Services Section 6 Grant Program, Michigan Department of Natural Resources, and the Center for Reptile and Amphibian Conservation and Management, Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne. We are indebted to N. Herbert and B. Putman, who provided field assistance for this project. We are also thankful for the logistical support of D. Soards (Lake LaSuAn Wildlife Area) and to the Toledo Area Council of Boy Scouts/Pioneer BSC for permitting us to survey their property. The study was conducted according to research protocol 00-037-06 of the Purdue Animal Care and Use Committee. LITERATURE CITED ATTUM, O., Y. M. LEE, J. H. ROE, AND B. A. KINGSBURY. 2007. Upland-wetland linkages: relationship of upland and wetland characteristics with watersnake abundance. Journal of Zoology 271:134 139. BELLAIRS, A. 1970. The Life of Reptiles. Universe Books, New York. BLUMSTEIN, D. T. 2003. Flight-initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. Journal of Wildlife Management 67:852 857. BLUMSTEIN, D. T., AND D. PELLETIER. 2005. Yellow-Bellied Marmot hiding time is sensitive to variation in costs. Canadian Journal of Zoology 83:363 367. BLUMSTEIN, D. T., E. FERNANDEZ-JURICIC, O. LEDEE, E. LARSEN, I. RODRIGUEZ-PRIETO, AND C. ZUGMEYER. 2004. Avian risk assessment: effects of perching height and detectability. Ethology 110:273 285. BRODIE, E. D., I. 1993. Differential avoidance of coral snake banded patterns by free-ranging avian predators in Costa Rica. Evolution 47:227 235. BROWN, G. P., AND R. SHINE. 2004. Effects of reproduction on the antipredator tactics of snakes (Tropidonophis mairii, Colubridae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56:257 262.

ESCAPE BEHAVIOR OF NERODIA SIPEDON 499 BULOVA, S. J. 1994. Ecological correlates of population and individual variation in antipredator behavior of two species of desert lizards. Copeia 1994:980 992. BURGER, J. 2001. The behavioral response of basking Northern Water (Nerodia sipedon) and Eastern Garter (Thamnophis sirtalis) Snakes to pedestrians in a New Jersey park. Urban Ecosystems 5:119 121. BURGER, J., AND M. GOCHFELD. 1981. Discrimination of the threat of direct versus tangential approach to the nest by incubating herring and great blackbacked gulls. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 95:676 684.. 1990. Risk discrimination of direct versus tangential approach by basking black iguana (Ctenosaura similis): variation as a function of human exposure. Journal of Comparative Psychology 104:388 394. COOPER, W. E., JR. 1997a. Factors affecting risk and cost of escape by the Broad-Headed Skink (Eumeces laticeps): predator speed, directness of approach, and female presence. Herpetologica 53:464 474.. 1997b. Escape by a refuging prey, the Broad- Headed Skink (Eumeces laticeps). Canadian Journal of Zoology 75:943 947.. 1998a. Direction of predator turning, a neglected cue to predation risk. Behaviour 135:55 64.. 1998b. Effects of refuge and conspicuousness on escape behavior by the Broad-Headed Skink (Eumeces laticeps). Amphibia-Reptilia 19:103 108.. 1998c. Risk factors and emergence from refuge in the lizard Eumeces laticeps. Behaviour 135:1065 1076.. 1999. Tradeoffs between courtship, fighting, and antipredatory behavior by a lizard, Eumeces laticeps. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 47:54 59.. 2000. Tradeoffs between predation risk and feeding in a lizard, the Broad-Headed Skink (Eumeces laticeps). Behaviour 137:1175 1189.. 2003a. Effect of risk on aspects of escape behavior by a lizard, Holbrookia propinqua, in relation to optimal escape theory. Ethology 109:617 626.. 2003b. Risk factors affecting escape behaviour in the Desert Iguana, Dipsosaurus dorsalis: speed and directness of predator approach, degree of cover, direction of turning by a predator, and temperature. Canadian Journal of Zoology 81:979 984.. 2005. When and how does starting distance affect flight initiation distance. Canadian Journal of Zoology 83:1045 1050.. 2006. Risk factors and escape strategy in the grasshopper Dissoteira carolina. Behaviour 143: 1201 1218.. 2007. Escape and its relationship to pursuitdeterrent signalling in the Cuban Curly-Tailed Lizard Leiocephalus carinatus. Herpetologica 62: 144 150. COOPER, W. E., JR., AND W. G. FREDERICK. 2007a. Optimal flight initiation distance. Journal of Theoretical Biology 244:59 67.. 2007b. Optimal time to emerge from refuge. Biological Journal of the Linnaean Society 91:375 382. COOPER, W. E., JR., AND V. PÉREZ-MELLADO. 2004. Tradeoffs between escape behavior and foraging opportunity by the Balearic lizard (Podarcis lilfordi). Herpetologica 60:321 324. COOPER, W. E., JR., AND D. S. WILSON. 2007. Sex and social costs of escaping in the Striped Plateau Lizard Sceloporus virgatus. Behavioral Ecology 18:764 768. COOPER, W. E., JR., V. PÉREZ-MELLADO, T. A. BAIRD, T. D. BAIRD, J. P. CALDWELL, AND L. J. VITT. 2003. Effects of risk, cost, and their interaction on optimal escape by nonrefuging Bonaire whiptail lizards, Cnemidophorus murinus. Behavioral Ecology 14:288 293. COOPER, W. E., JR., V. PÉREZ-MELLADO, AND D. HAWLENA. 2006. Magnitude of food reward affects escape behavior and acceptable risk in Balearic lizards, Podarcis lilfordi. Behavioral Ecology 17:554 559. COTT, H. B. 1940. Adaptive Coloration in Animals. Methuen, London. DÍAZ-URIARTE, R. 1999. Anti-predator behaviour changes following an aggressive encounter in the lizard Tropidurus hispidus. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences 266:2457 2464. DILL, L. M. 1990. Distance-to-cover and the escape decisions of an African cichlid fish, Melanochromis chipokae. Environmental Biology of Fishes 27:147 152. DILL, L. M., AND A. H. G. FRASER. 1997. The worm returns: hiding behavior of a tube-dwelling marine polychaete, Serpula vermicularis. Behavioral Ecology 8:186 193. DILL, L. M., AND R. HOUTMAN. 1989. The influence of distance to refuge on flight initiation distance in the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis). Canadian Journal of Zoology 67:233 235. DUVALL, D., M. B. KING, AND K. J. GUTZWILLER. 1985. Behavioral ecology and ethology of the Prairie Rattlesnake. National Geographic Research 1985:80 111. GREENE, H. W. 1988. Antipredator mechanisms in reptiles. In C. Gans and R. B. Huey (eds.), Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 16. Ecology B, Defense and Life History, pp. 1 152. Alan R. Liss, New York. GREENE, H. W., AND R. W. MCDIARMID. 1981. Coral snake mimicry: does it occur? Science 213:1207 1212. HEATWOLE, H. 1968. Relationship of escape behavior and camouflage in anoline lizards. Copeia 1968: 109 113. HEMMI, J. M. 2005a. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs. 1. Escape decisions in relation to risk of predation. Animal Behaviour 69:603 614.. 2005b. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs. 2. The visual cues. Animal Behaviour 69:615 625. KHAN, M. S., AND R. TASNIM. 1986. A note on tail injury in Eryx johnii. Snake 18:57 58. KLAUBER, L. M. 1956. Rattlesnakes, Their Habits, Life Histories, and Influence on Mankind. University of California Press, Los Angeles. LANGKILDE, T., R. SHINE, AND R. T. MASON. 2004. Predatory attacks to the head vs. body modify behavioral responses of garter snakes. Ethology 110:937 947.

500 W. E. COOPER JR. ET AL. LAYNE, J. R., JR., AND N. B. FORD. 1984. Flight distance of the Queen Snake, Regina septemvittata. Journal of Herpetology 18:496 498. LÓPEZ, P., I. MARCOS, AND J. MARTíN. 2005a. Effects of habitat-related visibility on escape decisions of the Spanish terrapin Mauremys leprosa. Amphibia- Reptilia 26:557 561. LÓPEZ, P., D. HAWLENA, V. POLO, AND J. MARTÍN. 2005b. Sources of individual shy-bold variations in antipredator behaviour of male Iberian rock lizards. Animal Behaviour 69:1 9. MARTÍN, J., AND P. LÓPEZ. 1996. The escape response of juvenile Psammodromus lizards. Journal of Comparative Psychology 110:187 192.. 1999. When to come out from a refuge: risksensitive and state-dependent decisions in an alpine lizard. Behavioral Ecology 10:487 492. MARTÍN, J., P. LÓPEZ, AND W. E. COOPER JR. 2003. Loss of mating opportunities influences refuge use in the Iberian Rock Lizard, Lacerta monticola. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 54:505 510. MARTÍN, J., I. MARCOS, AND P. LÓPEZ. 2005a. When to come out of your own shell: risk-sensitive hiding decisions in terrapins. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57:405 411. MARTÍN, J., J. LUQUE-LARENA, AND P. LÓPEZ. 2005b. Factors affecting escape behavior of Iberian Green Frogs (Rana perezi). Canadian Journal of Zoology 83:1189 1194. MARTÍN, J., L. DE NEVE, V. POLO, AND J. A. FARGALLO. 2006. Health-dependent vulnerability to predation affects escape responses of unguarded chinstrap penguin chicks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60:778 784. METCALFE, N. B., AND G. I. STEELE. 2001. Changing nutritional status causes a shift in the balance of nocturnal to diurnal activity in European minnows. Functional Ecology 15:304 309. MYERS, C. W., AND A. A. ARATA. 1961. Remarks on defensive behavior in the Hognose Snake Heterodon simus (Linnaeus). Quarterly Journal of the Florida Academy of Sciences 24:108 110. NELSON, M. K., AND D. R. FORMANOWICZ JR. 2005. Relationship between escape speed and flight distance in a wolf spider, Hogna carolinensis (Walckenaer 1805). Journal of Arachnology 33:153 158. PLATT, D. R. 1969. Natural history of the hognose snakes Heterodon platyrhinos and Heterodon nasicus. University of Kansas Publications of the Museum of Natural History 18:253 420. POUGH, F. H. 1988. Mimicry and related phenomena. In C. Gans and R. B. Huey (eds.), Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 16. Ecology B, Defense and Life History, pp. 153 234. Alan R. Liss, New York. RAND, A. S. 1964. Inverse relationship between temperature and shyness in the lizard Anolis lineatopus. Ecology 45:863 864. SHINE, R., D. O CONNOR, AND R. T. MASON. 2000. Female mimicry in Garter Snakes: behavioural tactics of she-males and the males that court them. Canadian Journal of Zoology 78:1391 1396. SHINE, R., B. PHILLIPS, H. WAYE, AND R. T. MASON. 2003. Behavioral shifts associated with reproduction in Garter Snakes. Behavioral Ecology 14:251 256. SMITH, S. M. 1975. Innate recognition of Coral Snake pattern by a possible avian predator. Science 187:759 760.. 1977. Coral-Snake pattern recognition and stimulus generalisation by naive great kiskadees (Aves: Tyrannidae). Nature 265:535 536. STUART-FOX, D., M. J. WHITING, AND A. MOUSALLI. 2006. Camouflage and colour change: antipredator responses to bird and snake predators across multiple populations in a dwarf chameleon. Biological Journal of the Linnaean Society 88:437 446. SWEET, S. S. 1985. Geographic variation, convergent crypsis and mimicry in Gopher Snakes (Pituophis melanoleucus) and Western Rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridus). Journal of Herpetology 19:55 67. WEATHERHEAD, P. J., AND I. C. ROBERTSON. 1992. Thermal constraints on swimming performance and escape response of Northern Water Snakes (Nerodia sipedon). Canadian Journal of Zoology 70:94 98. YDENBERG, R. C., AND L. M. DILL. 1986. The economics of fleeing from predators. Advances in the Study of Behavior 16:229 249. Accepted: 19 January 2008.