Mating and Male Combat in Australian Blacksnakes, Pseudechis porphyriacus

Similar documents
Communication and Displays of Snakes

Combat Rituals in Snakes

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Reptiles and amphibian behaviour

Squamates of Connecticut

Northern Copperhead Updated: April 8, 2018

Dipsas trinitatis (Trinidad Snail-eating Snake)

Territoriality in a snake

Snakes on the Plain. Copperhead. By Brooke Cain

Ontogenetic changes in tail-length and the possible relation to caudal luring in northeast Kansas Copperheads, Agkistrodon contortrix

AGGRESSIVE DISPLAY OF THE CORN-CRAKE.

BODY SIZE AND SEXUAL SIZE DIMORPHISM OF BULLSNAKES (PITUOPHIS CATENIFER SAYI) IN WISCONSIN, USA

reproductive life History and the effects of sex and season on morphology in CRoTALus oreganus (northern PaCifiC RATTLESNAKES)

Cub Scout Den Meeting Outline

Three snakes from coastal habitats at Pulau Sugi, Riau Islands, Indonesia

Superior Snakes. By: Jake Elliott Richards

Squamates of Connecticut. May 11th 2017

Notes on the diets of seven sympatric snakes in the genera Agkistrodon, Nerodia, Sistrurus, and Thamnophis

Dangerous Snakes Of Australia By Peter Mirtschin;Richard Davis

by Andrew L. Shiels, Leader, Nongame and Endangered Species Unit

Copperhead (Agkistrodon contortrix)

Wandering Garter Snake

Carphophis amoenus Family Colubridae Subfamily Xenodontidae

(135) OBSERVATIONS IN A ROOKERY DURING THE INCUBATION PERIOD C. M. OGILVIE.

Recognizing Dangerous Snakes in the United States and Canada: A Novel 3-Step Identification Method

(170) COURTSHIP AND DISPLAY OF THE SLAVONIAN GREBE.

Ecology of the Australian Elapid Snake Tropidechis carinatus1

SECTION 3 IDENTIFYING ONTARIO S EASTERN MASSASAUGA RATTLESNAKE AND ITS LOOK-ALIKES

Notes on Road-Killed Snakes and Their Implications on Habitat Modification Due to Summer Flooding on the Mississippi River in West Central Illinois

Midwestern Snakes Facts & Folklore

The captive maintenance and breeding of. Diporiphora winneckei (Cane grass dragon) at the Alice Springs Desert Park.

Snake Attack. A snake overview. Australian Venomous Snakes

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

ARIZONA GAME AND FISH DEPARTMENT HERITAGE DATA MANAGEMENT SYSTEM CLASSIFICATION, NOMENCLATURE, DESCRIPTION, RANGE

Doug Scull s Science and Nature

Reproduction, Feeding and Growth in the Australian Burrowing Snake Vermicella annulata

Title Madagascan Snake, Leioheterodon mad. Author(s) Mori, Akira; Randriamboavonjy, Tahi. Citation Current Herpetology (2010), 29(2):

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Behaviour and spatial ecology of Gilbert s dragon Lophognathus gilberti (Agamidae: Reptilia)

Breeding behavior of the boreal toad, Bufo boreas boreas (Baird and Girard), in western Montana

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

The puff adder is a large, sluggish, thick-bodied snake that rarely exceeds a meter in length.

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

(82) FIELD NOTES ON THE LITTLE GREBE.

Summary. Introduction

THE concept that reptiles have preferred

Typical Snakes Part # 1

Crotalus durissus vegrandis in captivity / 81

The Modern Myth or Are You A Man Or A Mouse?

Reptile Identification Guide

AUSTRALIAN MUSEUM SCIENTIFIC PUBLICATIONS

ENVENOMATION BY THE MALAGASY COLUBRID SNAKE Langaha madagascariensis D CRUZE NC (1)

A NEW SNAKE FROM QUEENSLAND, AUSTRALIA (SERPENTES: ELAPIDAE).

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

Species Results From Database Search

Writing: Lesson 31. Today the students will be learning how to write more advanced middle paragraphs using a variety of elaborative techniques.

A SURVEY FOR THREATENED AND ENDANGERED HERPETOFAUNA IN THE LOWER MARAIS DES CYGNES RIVER VALLEY

SNAKES. CITES Identification manual. Tentative tool for Thai CITES officers TANYA CHAN-ARD. Compiled by

Seasonal Activity of the Florida Kingsnake Lampropeltis getula floridana (Serpentes: Colubridae) in Southern Florida

Investigations of Giant Garter Snakes in The Natomas Basin: 2002 Field Season

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

WildlifeCampus Advanced Snakes & Reptiles 1. Vipers and Adders

The Bushmaster Silent Fate of the American Tropics The natural history of the largest, most dangerous viper in the world

Rattlers. Rattlers. Visit for thousands of books and materials.

Venomous Snakebite Protocols from Joe Pittman

All about snakes. What are snakes? Are snakes just lizards without legs? If you want to know more

Snakes. Written and Illustrated by Yow Ming

Culverts and Fencing to Reduce Wildlife-Vehicle Collisions and Maintain Permeability

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

MICROHABITAT AND PREY ODOR SELECTION IN THE FORAGING PIGMY RATTLESNAKE

John Thompson June 09, 2016 Thompson Holdings, LLC P.O. Box 775 Springhouse, Pa

Surveys for Giant Garter Snakes in Solano County: 2005 Report

Alberta Conservation Association 2016/17 Project Summary Report

ON THE NEW GUINEA TAIi'AN.

Chris Petersen, Robert E. Lovich, Steve Sekscienski

WHAT ARE HERPTILES? WHICH IS WHICH? 1. Vertebrates are animals that have 2. Complete the following chart of vertebrate groups: EGGS LAID WHERE?

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

Rubber Boas in Radium Hot Springs: Habitat, Inventory, and Management Strategies

Objectives: Outline: Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles. Characteristics of Amphibians. Types and Numbers of Amphibians

A.13 BLAINVILLE S HORNED LIZARD (PHRYNOSOMA BLAINVILLII)

Reproductive strategies in snakes

A NEW GENUS AND A NEW SPECIES OF SKINK FROM VICTORIA.

Snakes of the Everglades Agricultural Area 1

4 Many species of mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians and fish 940L. Source 1 Habitats

Ethology of Crayfish. Name. Lab partners: Page 1

Different animals move in different ways. Cut and sort the animals into the correct groups. Walk Fly Swim Slide

REPTILE BEHAVIOR BASICS FOR THE VETERINARY CLINICIAN

Grade Level: 3-5. Next Generation Sunshine State Standards SC.3.L.15.1 SC.4.L.16.2; SC.4.L.17.4 SC.5.L.15.1; SC.5.L.17.1

8/19/2013. Topic 14: Body support & locomotion. What structures are used for locomotion? What structures are used for locomotion?

On the immunity of snakes to their own venom and to the venom of conspecifics across ontogeny

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

Tails of enticement: caudal luring by an ambushforaging

Draw a line from the names below to the animals they match. Red Fox. Wild Turkey. Wood Duck. White-tailed Deer. Black Bear

People hunt reptiles for their skin. It is used to make leather products like belts, shoes or handbags. A reptile s body

Reptiles of Tennessee

Maturity and Other Reproductive Traits of the Kanahebi Lizard Takydromus tachydromoides (Sauria, Lacertidae) in Mito

The effects of translocation on the spatial ecology of tiger snakes (Notechis scutatus) in a suburban landscape

Snakes of Wisconsin by Wisconsin DNR

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

CONSPECIFIC SCENT TRAILING BY GARTER SNAKES ( Thamnophis sirtalis ) DURING AUTUMN. Further Evidence for Use of Pheromones in Den Location

Transcription:

. 1981 JOURNAL OF HERPETOLOGY 15(1):101-107 Mating and Male Combat in Australian Blacksnakes, Pseudechis porphyriacus Richard Shine, Gordon C. Grigg, Terri G. Shine and Peter Harlow School of Biological Sciences, A08, University of Sydney, N.S.W. 2006, Australia ABSTRACT-We observed ritual combat and other agonistic behavior between male blacksnakes (Pseudechis porphyriacus) in a field population in central New South Wales. Combat bouts vary greatly in duration (2 to 30 minutes), and apparently function as male "strategies" to displace rival courting males. Direct attack and biting may serve to displace rival males in copulo. We briefly review published literature on male combat in snakes. INTRODUCTION Ritualized "combat" behavior between rival males has been recorded in many snake species, including several Australian elapids (Fleay, 1937, 1951; Baker, 1968; Shine, 1977a; review in Shine, 1978a). However, few detailed observations on male combat have been made under field conditions. This dearth of information has led to differing interpretations of the adaptive significance of male combat in snakes (reviewed by Bogert and Roth, 1966). The present paper (i) describes field observations of mating and male combat in wild blacksnakes (Pseudechis porphyriacus), (ii) compares these observations to previous descriptions of male combat in snakes, and (iii) interprets the adaptive significance of Pseudechis male combat. LOCATION, SPECIES AND METHODS Our observations were made in the Macquarie Marshes, an extensive swamp in central northern New South Wales. The exact location was on a bypass channel running through "Sandy Camp" property, 20 km west of the town of Coonamble (147 30'E, 30 30'S). The channel runs north-south, is about 6 meters in width with fairly steep banks, and was full of slowly-flowing water at the time of our observations on 6 October 1979. The day was warm (maximum temperature about 25 C) with sunny periods, and followed several days of cooler weather. The common or red-bellied blacksnake (Pseudechis porphyriacus) is a large, venomous elapid snake abundant in the Macquarie Marshes. The species is distributed widely in eastern Australia, usually in association with water (Cogger, 1978). Macquarie Marshes specimens typically attain large body sizes (adult males average 126 cm snout-vent length-shine, 1978a). Mating occurs mainly in October and November (Shine, 1977a,b) and male combat in this species has been described by Fleay (1937, 1951) and Baker (1968). The description below is based on notes taken at the time, plus analysis of 26 photographs taken during the observation period. We watched the snakes with binoculars from across the channel at a distance of 10 meters. Sexes of the snakes were determined by body size (males average much larger than females-shine, 1978b) and by tail shape.

102 RICHARD SHINE, GORDON C. GRIGG, TERRI G. SHINE AND PETER HARLOW OBSERVATIONS At 1230 hours, we noticed three blacksnakes coiled together on the eastern bank of the bypass channel. A male and a female were copulating, and another male was lying across the body of the female. The copulating snakes were in contact at their vents only-the female was lying loosely coiled in grass on the channel's bank, the male lying along the water's edge. The snakes were attached by a single hemipenis, which was stretched sufficiently to make it obvious in observations and photographs. Neither of the copulating snakes showed much movement. However, the "extra" male was constantly moving with abrupt, jerky motions. His mental area ("chin") was often adpressed to the female's dorsal surface, and his body was aligned with (and draped over) the female's body. This male's movements were so active that at one point the copulating male was "pushed" into the water, although copulation was not interrupted (see Fig. 1a). An additional male blacksnake approached the above trio at 1235 hours. This snake was first seen 20 meters south of the trio, on the same (eastern) bank of the channel. He moved directly to the trio (Fig. 1b) but immediately upon contacting it, he took to the water rapidly and swam to the opposite (western) bank of the channel. This brought him into close proximity to the observers: he was then frightened away by our movements. At the time of contact between this male and the trio, the female blacksnake moved = 1 meter further up the bank, the in copula male being dragged passively out of the water by his hemipenis. A few minutes later, a fourth male was noticed approaching from the north along the eastern bank. He rapidly covered the 30 meters separating him from the trio, showing typical "searching" behavior similar to that seen in foraging blacksnakes (high frequency of tongue-flicks to substrate; side-to-side waving of head). This snake was identifiable by his very short tail, probably the result of some previous injury. As soon as "Short-tail" reached the trio of blacksnakes, his movements became more agitated. He bit the non-copulating male on the neck, and these two males then reared up, facing each other, with hoods spread (Fig. 1c). This posture was held only for a few seconds, and the two males then began writhing about and "plaiting" the foreparts of their bodies together (Fig. 1d). The form of combat in this species has been described and illustrated by Fleay (1951) and Baker (1968) and will be described here only briefly (see Fig. 1). The two snakes intertwined their bodies and engaged in "head-pushing" contests (c.f. Akester, 1979), where it appeared that each of the combatants attempted to push the other's head downwards with his own head. The combat behavior was vigorous, with the snakes moving about together rapidly. The combatants tumbled down the bank into the water soon after the commencement of the bout. Combat continued in the water (Fig. 1e), and as the snakes moved back onto the bank. The combat bout ended suddenly, with the original male (not Short-tail) taking to the water and swimming across to the eastern bank. That entire combat bout took only three minutes (1242-1245 hours). The copulating pair of blacksnakes played no part in the bout, and appeared not to react to the fighting males in any way. At the conclusion of the bout, the short-tailed male returned to the copulating snakes and began behaving in the same way as the male he had dispossessed (head on female's mid-dorsal surface, moving along female's body with abrupt jerky motions; body aligned with female's body, draped over female; tail pushed under female's tail in apparent attempt to insert hemipenis). Unlike the previous male, which had shown no interest in the copulating male, Short-tail bit the copulating male vigorously at mid-body on one occasion (1255 hours). The copulating male showed no reaction to this bite, and Short-tail continued to concentrate his attentions on the female. At 1304 hours, yet another male blacksnake appeared, and moved towards the trio. This snake came from the north also, but along the western rather than the eastern bank; when it encountered the observers, it took to the water and we did not sight it again. The short-tailed male persisted in his attentions to the copulating pair, with only brief periods of inactivity. His movements were often vigorous, with hood-spreading. At 1319 hours he again bit the copulating male at mid-body (Fig. 1f), and all three snakes fell from the bank into the water. The

BLACKSNAKE MATING AND MALE COMBAT 103 FIGURE 1. Mating and male combat in blacksnakes. as described in the text. Shaded snake is the female. Drawn from 35 mm colour slides. (a) Position of snakes when first sighted. Male lying in water is copulating with female (shaded). (b) Arrival of additional male snake to previous trio. (c) Commencement of ritual combat between male blacksnakes. Copulating pair lie beneath rival males. (d) Male blacksnakes in combat, with copulating pair beneath. (e) Male blacksnakes in combat, after having fallen into the bypass channel. (I) Disruption of copulating pair of blacksnakes by extra ("short-tailed") male. Photograph taken as all three snakes fell from the bank into the water.

104 RICHARD SHINE, GORDON C. GRIGG, TERRI G. SHINE AND PETER HARLOW short-tailed male immediately re-emerged and began "searching" behavior (tongue-flicking, etc.) around the area where the copulating pair had been. Meanwhile, the two copulating snakes, still attached by a hemipenis, floated downstream (north) along the channel. Both snakes attempted to swim, but often in different directions. The female finally swam across to the western bank, dragging the male tail first against the current. The two snakes reached the western bank about 30 meters downstream and across from their original position, and were still attached at that time. However, they had separated by the time they crawled 2 meters to the top of the western bank (1325 hrs). The male still had a hemipenis everted, and crawled about actively with "searching" behavior. The hemipenis was retracted within two minutes. The female coiled on the bank and remained still. The short-tailed male remained close to the original site of copulation, with much tongueflicking. About 30 minutes later, a male snake (possibly one of those observed earlier) swam up the bypass channel past this area. The short-tailed male immediately moved into the water, swam straightacrossto the other male,anda brief(2 minute)bout of combat occurred in midstream. This bout took the same form as the previous one, except that biting was not observed. At the conclusion of the bout, the "intruding" male swam away rapidly. Another bout of male combat in P. porphyriacus had been observed and photographed by S. Cohn and party the preceding day, on a nearby part of the same property. Dr. Cohn kindly provided photographs and the following description: 'The snakes were observed in the Southern Marshes along the Monkeygar Creek. We observed the snakes for at least 30 minutes between 1230 and 1300 hrs. The snakes were 'caught in the act' (Le. already in combat) so they may have been carrying on for some time. No other snake was observed. They did not appear to bite each other; on the contrary, they seemed to the layman to rub each other almost affectionately... They really did not seem to cease their routine but only move out of our range. As the slides imply, they did this in and out of the water." His photographs clearly show the snakes intertwined; this posture, and the large size of the snakes, leave no doubt that these were males in combat. DISCUSSION In general, our observations are in good agreement with previous descriptions of reproductive behavior in P. porphyriacus and in other snakes. Firstly, the postures adopted by the fighting male blacksnakes were very similar to those figured by Fleay (1937, 1951) and Baker (1968). The "head-pushing" behavior of the snakes during combat is only slightly different from some of the manoeuvers seen in colubrid and viperid combat (e.g. Bogert and Roth, 1966; Akester, 1979). Also consistent with published records of combat are (i) the Iow frequency of biting, and (ii) the lack of response of the male in copula to other males (Shaw, 1951). The spasmodic movements of the courting males, and the adpression of the male's mental, area to the female's mid-dorsum, seemed to one of us (R. Shine) to be almost identical to courtship' sequences he has watched in the North American gartersnake Thamnophis sirtalis. These behaviors are known to be common in courtship of many colubrid snakes (e.g. Shaw, 1951). However, our observations differ from previous reports in several respects. Most interesting is the duration of combat bouts: both bouts we watched were very brief (2-3 minutes). A review of published data on the duration of snake combat bouts suggests that most bouts are more prolonged (Table 1; X duration= 34.5 mins, S.E. = 6.9, n = 24). This figure itself is an underestimate, since many bouts were already in progress when observations commenced (references in Table 1). Nevertheless, it is clear that P. porphyriacus combat bouts sometimes are protracted (e.g. Baker, 1968; observations by S. Cohn reported in present paper). Fleay (1937) described combat in captive blacksnakes as a series of minute-long "rounds," after each of which the snakes would disengage completely, separate, and immediately line up together again and repeat the "wrestling" behavior.

BLACKSNAKE MATING AND MALE COMBAT 105 TABLE 1. Duration of combat "bouts" in snakes. Where data are available, mean and standard error of bout durations are calculated. Family Boidae Colubridae Elapidae Viperidae Duration of Species Conditions bout (min) Authority Morelia spilotes Field > 60 Covacevich, 1975 Sanzinia madagascariensis Captive 7.5 (2.1) Carpenter et al., 1978 Drymarchon corals Captive 20 Tinkle, 1951 EIaphe longissima Field > 60 Stemmler-Morath, 1935 E. obso/eta Field > 18 Rigley, 1971 Lampropeltis calligaster Field > 30 Moehn, 1967 L. mexicana Captive 5 Murphy et al., 1978 Masticophis taeniatus Field 8 Bennion and Parker, 1976 Pituophis melanoleucus Field > 15 Woodbury, 1941 P. melanoleucus Field > 60 Bogert and Roth, 1966 Senora episcopa Captive 5.7 (2.3) Kroll,1971 Austrelaps superbus Field > 30 Shine and Alien, 1980 Oxyuranus scutellatus Field > 8 Hosmer, 1953 Pseudechmporphynacus Field > 5 Fleay, 1937 P. porphyriacus Field > 10 Baker, 1968 P. porphyriacus Field 2, 3, > 30 Present study Agkistrodon contortrix Field > 20 Gloyd, 1947 A. plscivorus Field > 30 Perry, 1978 Bothrops mooieni Captive > 120 Leloup, 1975 Crotalus sp. Field > 30 Klauber, 1956 C. cerastes Captive > 90 Lowe and Norris, 1950 C. horridus Field > 30 Klauber, 1956 C. ruber Captive > 15 Klauber, 1956 C. viridis Field > 30 Gloyd, 1947 Sistrurus miliarius Captive > 120 Palmer and Williamson, 1971 The long duration of combat bouts reported in the literature could be interpreted as an artefact of captivity, since under these conditions the snakes are forced to remain in close proximity to each other. At first sight, this hypothesis is supported by data in Table 1: mean combat duration is much greater in captive bouts (X = 47.9 min., S.E. = 18.6, n = 8) than in field bouts (X = 27.8 min., SE = 4.6, n = 16). However, these two means are not significantly different from each other (using Wilcoxon's rank-sum test, p> >.1); the difference is entirely due to three prolonged captive bouts in vipers (Table 1). More importantly, some of the longest bouts have been observed in the field. Hence, protracted combat cannot be a laboratory artefact. Instead, the data in Table 1 indicate the great range of combat durations observed both in the field and in the laboratory. Our observations on Pseudechis porphyriacus include the briefest combats recorded (2-3 minutes), but it seems likely that many such brief combats could escape attention. The longer a bout persists, the greater the probability of its being observed. We believe that our observations on blacksnake behavior shed some light on the adaptive significance of male combat in this species. Combat in snakes has been interpreted as a male "strategy" for (i) forcing rival males to flee from the vicinity of a receptive female (e.g. Prior, 1933), (ii) physically "carrying" rival males away from a female (Leloup, 1964), (iii) defending against homosexual courtship (Shaw, 1951), and (iv) gaining access to food items (Shaw, 1951). Our observations support the first of these hypotheses: in both of the combat bouts we observed, the short-tailed male (initiator of both bouts) displaced other males. Of equal interest is the behavior of male P. porphyriacus in relation to the copulating pair. Two different males, at different times, engaged in active courtship of the female while she was in copula. In both cases, the actions of the courting males were so vigorous that the copulating snakes were pushed about. In the second episode, where the "extra" male bit the copulating male

106 RICHARD SHINE, GORDON C. GRIGG, TERRI G. SHINE AND PETER HARLOW as well as jostling the pair, the "extra" male's movements were sufficient to throw all three snakes into the bypass channel. This ultimately separated the mating pair, an event which should tend to increase the mating opportunities of the displacing male. We interpret our observations to mean that the adaptive significance of agonistic behavior in male blacksnakes may be (i) to displace copulating or courting males through direct attack and biting, and (ii) to displace rival courting males through ritual combat. Tests of these hypotheses will come only from detailed field studies on male reproductive behavior in relation to mating success. The lack of quantitative data on this topic for any snake species is a serious gap in our understanding of snake ecology. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank Andrew Grigg for field assistance, S. Cohn for observations and photographs of P. porphyriacus combat, and June Jeffery for drawing the Figure in this paper. S. Robertson kindly allowed us to camp, collect and observe on "Sandy Camp," and directed us to the blacksnake population where our observations were made. LITERATURE CITED Akester, J. 1979. Male combat in captive gaboon vipers. Herpetologica 35:124-128. Baker, A. B. 1969. Snakes in combat. Proc. Roy. Zool. Soc. New South Wales 1966-B7:29-31. Bennion, R. S. and W. S. Parker. 1976. Field observations on courtship and aggressive behavior in desert striped whipsnakes, Masticophis t. taeniatus. Herpetologica 32:30-35. Bogert, C. M. and V. D. Roth. 1966. Ritualistic combat of male gopher snakes, Pituophis melanoleucus affinis (Reptilia, Colubridae). Ann. Mus. Novit. 2245:1-27. Carpenter, C. C., J. B. Murphy and L. A. Mitchell. 1978. Combat bouts with spur use in the Madagascan boa (Sanzinia madagascariensis). Herpetologica 34:207-212. Cogger, H. G. 1978. Reptiles and amphibians of Australia. A. H. & A. W. Reed, Sydney. 608 pp. Covacevich, J. 1975. Snakes in combat. Vict. Natural. 92:252-253. Fleay, D. 1937. Black snakes in combat. Proc. Roy. Zool. Soc. New South Wales, August: 40-42.. 1951. Savage battles between snakes. Walkabout 17:10-13. Gloyd, H. K. 1947. Notes on the courtship and mating behavior of certain snakes. Nat. Hist. Miscell. 12:1-4. Hosmer, W. 1953. An account of supposed mating of the taipan, Oxyuranus scutellatus. North Queensland Natural. 22:1-3. Klauber, L. M. 1956. Rattlesnakes, their habits, life histories, and influence on mankind. Univ. California Press, Berkeley. 1476 pp. Kroll, J. C. 1971. Combat behavior in male Great Plains ground snakes (Sonora episcopa episcopal. Texas J. ScL 23:300. Leloup, P. 1964. Observations sur la reproduction du Dendroaspis jamesoni kaimosae (Loveridge). Bull. Soc. Roy. Zool. Anvers 33: 13-27.. 1975. Observations sur la reproduction de Bothrops moojeni Hoge en captivite. Acta Zool. Pathol. Antverp. 62:173-201. Lowe, C. H. Jr. and K. S. Norris. 1950. Aggressive behavior in male sidewinders, Crotalus cerastes with a discussion of aggressive behavior and territoriality in snakes. Nat. Hist. Miscell. 66:1-13. Moehn, L. D. 1967. A combat dance between two prairie kingsnakes. Copeia 1967:480-481. Murphy, J. B., B. W. Tryon and B. J. Brecke. 1978. An inventory of reproduction and social behavior in captive gray-banded kingsnakes, Lampropeltis mexicana alterna (Brown). Herpetologica 34:84-93. Palmer, W. M. and G. M. Williamson. 1971. Observations on the natural history of the Carolina pigmy rattlesnake, Sistrurus miliarius miliarius Linnaeus. J. Elisha Mitchell Sci. Soc. 87:20-25. Perry, J. 1978. An observation of "dance" behavior in the western cottonmouth, Agkistrodon piscivorus leucostoma (Reptilia, Serpentes, Viperidae). J. Herpetol. 12:429-431. Prior, H. 1. J. 1933. The dance of the adders. Countryside 9:492-493.. Rigley, L. 1971. "Combat dance" of the black snake, EIaphe o. obso/eta. J. Herpetol. 5:65-66. Shaw, C. E. 1951. Male combat in American colubrid snakes with remarks on combat in other colubrid and elapid snakes. Herpetologica 7:149-168. Shine, R. 1977a. Reproduction in Australian elapid snakes. I. Testicular cycles and mating seasons. Aust. J. Zool. 25:647--653.. 1977b. Reproduction in Australian elapid snakes. 11.Female reproductive cycles. Aust. J. Zool. 25:655-666.. 1978a. Sexual size dimorphism and male combat in snakes. Oecologia 33:269-278.

BLACKSNAKEMATING AND MALE COMBAT 107. 1978b. Growth rates and sexual maturation in six species 01Australian elapid snakes. Herpetologica 34:73-79. and S. Alien. 1980. Ritual combat in the Australian copperhead, Austre/aps superbus (Serpentes, Elapidae). Vic!. Natural. 97:188-190. Stemmler-Morath, C. 1935. Beitrag zur lortpllanzungsbiologie europaischer Colubridae. Zool. Garten 8:38-41. Tinkle, D. W. 1951.Peculiarbehavior01indigosnakesin captivity.copeia1951:77-78. Woodbury, A. M. 1941. Copulation in gopher snakes. Copeia 1941:54. Accepted 30 Jul 1980 Copyright 1981 Society lor the Study 01Amphibians and Reptiles