Long-term follow-up of the efficacy of a behavioural treatment programme for dogs with firework fears

Similar documents
Noise sensitivities in dogs: a new licenced treatment option

ANXIETY IN DOGS: THE SILENT EPIDEMIC Debra F. Horwitz, DVM, DACVB

Prevalence and impact of sound sensitivity in dogs

Minimising pet stress at veterinary practice visits

Separation Anxiety Syndrome

Best behavior starts here. Feliway and Adaptil the #1 behavior solution for cats and dogs

Critical appraisal Randomised controlled trial questions

BEHAVIOURAL OR MEDICAL? ANXIETY DISORDERS IN OLDER ANIMALS. Dr Kersti Seksel BVSc (Hons), MRCVS, MA (Hons), FACVSc, DACVB, CMAVA, DECVBM-CA

J.D. Warford, DVM

Managing Separation Anxiety: An Evidence-Based Approach

Annual Review of Cases 2003

How stress affects health and behaviour; strategies for prevention and treatment

Incorporating Behavioral Medicine Into General Practice

Adaptil is clinically proven to help dogs deal with stressful situations.

Journal Club. Focus on Canine Noise-Induced Fear. Overview of Selected Literature

The Kennel Club has long campaigned for a ban on the use and sale of electric shock collars in Scotland.

Dog Behavior and Training - Moving with Your Dog

Understanding Fear and Anxiety in Pets

IDENTIFICATION AND MANAGEMENT OF SEPARATION ANXIETY IN DOGS

Aggression Social Aggression to Unfamiliar Dogs

Dog Behavior and Training - Teaching Calm Settle and Relaxation Training

Comprehensive References

Proceeding of the LAVC Latin American Veterinary Conference Oct , 2010 Lima, Peru

Senior Dogs: Common Behavior Changes Veterinary & Aquatic Services Department, Drs. Foster & Smith

Enrichment for Indoor Cats Ilona Rodan, DVM, ABVP Dipl., Feline Milwaukee Veterinary Medical Association November 2014

Managing stressed canine patients: practical techniques

Dog Behavior Problems Veterinary Visits/Examinations

Waiting for a Forever Home

Basic Commands and Training

Dog Behavior Problems House Soiling

How do we assess for welfare of individuals? Can we have a generic welfare assessment? Will it work for all shelters?

Separation Anxiety. Techniques to calm your dog

Shelter Guidelines Project. Shelter Guidelines - Content

AGGRESSION (CATS) DIAGNOSING AND TREATING

Proceedings of the European Veterinary Conference Voorjaarsdagen

BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT TOOLS FOR DOGS IN A SHELTER SETTING. Sara L. Bennett, DVM, MS, DACVB

Is your pet scared of fireworks?

General Information. Owner s Name. Cat s Name

Outline. What is a Puppy Mill? What is a Puppy Mill? What is a Puppy Mill? Misguided Love 11/26/2018

Managing separation anxiety in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) Discusses the efficacy of behavioural modification, auditory and olfactory

AUSTRALIAN AND NEW ZEALAND COLLEGE OF VETERINARY SCIENTISTS FELLOWSHIP GUIDELINES

Drs. S.K.J. Bosma Student nr Sept 23 Dec 2011

Use of the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid to assess the life time experience of animals and cumulative severity of procedures

Polishing up your low stress skills. Dr. Sally J. Foote DVM,CABC-IAABC AVSAB member Okaw Veterinary Clinic Tuscola Il

Life Expectancy and Environmental Factors: Maintaining Behavioral Health

Are Pressure Vests Beneficial at Reducing Stress in Anxious and Fearful Dogs?

Can ativan kill a dog

Dog Behavior Problems Barking and Training Quiet

SP6B NUISANCE BARKING IN DOGS AND THE WELFARE IMPLICATIONS OF DIFFERENT TREATMENT MODALITIES

Feline behavior consultation questionnaire

Total Running Time 51:17

CONSIDERING BEHAVIOURAL FACTORS WHEN DIAGNOSING FELINE MEDICINE CASES SARAH HEATH

Puppy Behavior and Training Handling and Food Bowl Exercises

Behavior Modification Why Punishment Should Be Avoided

BEHAVIORAL IMPACT OF PUPPY MILL AND HOARDING ENVIRONMENTS. Sara L. Bennett, DVM, MS, DACVB

Desensitization and Counter Conditioning

Understanding anxiety & its implications for pet greyhounds. By: Dr Karen Cunnington BSc BVSc Hons

b. A description of the animal s signalment (sex, breed, age, size, medical conditions), household composition and current living environment.

Housetraining Your Adopted Dog

Canine Aggression SIBLING RIVALRY INDIAN HILLS ANIMAL CLINIC. Indian Hills Animal Clinic

To choke or not to choke How positive reinforcement has affected the use of choke collars in dog training

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression Diagnosis and Overview

Puppy Socialization and Fear Prevention

Puppy Behavior and Training Handling and Food Bowl Exercises

1. Establish a predictable routine Since your dog is anxious you need to begin by making his day calmer and more predictable whether you are home or

Puppy behaviours when left home alone: A pilot study

How To Housebreak Your New Puppy

A happy pet has a forever home.

KCAI Scheme Online Assessments: Criteria

The Dog and Cat Management Board. Policy and Procedure for the training of dogs subject to a dangerous dog order

Outline CANINE ENRICHMENT: STRESS REDUCTION FOR HAPPY AND HEALTHY SHELTER DOGS. What is Environmental Enrichment?

Golden Rule Training. Desensitizing Your Dog to Specific Noises, Other Dogs and Situations

General Information: Date and time of consultation (if scheduled): Clinic address: City/Town: Clinic phone: ( ) Who referred you to our service?

Dog Impulsivity Assessment Scale (DIAS) (Wright, Mills & Pollux, 2011)

Housesoiling Dogs Basics

General Canine Behavior History

Proceedings of the Southern European Veterinary Conference - SEVC -

Conflict-Related Aggression

INTRODUCTION. 1 VICTORIA STILWELL Positively

Antianxiety medications and treatments

World Organisation for Animal Health

Pilot study to identify risk factors for coprophagic behaviour in dogs

Introduction. Fireworks & Your Dog Simple, practical tips on helping your dog deal with noise phobia in firework season. 1 of 8

EXOTIC PETS The landscape has changed

Alternative Approaches: Canines Helping Persons with PTSD Resources Wednesday, August 16, :00 PM 3:15 PM

FEAR FREE VETERINARY VISITS

FELINE BEHAVIOUR CONSULTATION QUESTIONNAIRE

Best Paw Forward: Bringing Out the Best in Shy Dogs

Effect of Multidog Play Groups on Cortisol Levels and Behavior of Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) Housed in a Humane Society

Nicole Cottam a, Nicholas H. Dodman a, Alice A. Moon-Fanelli a & Gary J. Patronek b a Department of Clinical Science, Tufts Cummings

Behavior News THE NEWSLETTER FROM THE ANIMAL BEHAVIOR PROGRAM. Notes on Puppy Socialization. Behavior Textbook Review/Recommendation

Skills Assessment Form VTS-Behavior

HOW TO PROTECT YOUR PET OVER SUMMER

1 FELINE BEHAVIOR CONSULTATION QUESTIONNAIRE GENERAL INFORMATION PET INFORMATION ENVIRONMENT / LIFESTYLE

The 5 animal welfare needs. Puppy socialisation. Things to think about

Refinement Issues in Animal Research. Joanne Zurlo, PhD Institute for Laboratory Animal Research National Academy of Sciences

Evaluation of XXXXXXX mixed breed male dog

Canine Aggression Overview of Diagnosis and Treatment

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression Getting Started Safety and Management

Referral Form. Client s Name. Pet s Name. Client s Contact Details. Referring Veterinary Surgeon. Practice Address.

Transcription:

C4438 Set by: Mark Date: 25.01.2008 Proofed: 31.01.08 THIS PDF IS SUPPLIED FOR PROOFING PURPOSES ONLY. PLEASE PRINT YOUR ARTICLE AND MARK ESSENTIAL CORRECTIONS AND CHANGES ON THE PRINTOUT. RETURN THE CORRECTED PRINTOUT TO: THE VETERINARY RECORD, 7 MANSFIELD STREET, LONDON W1G 9NQ, UNITED KINGDOM Long-term follow-up of the efficacy of a behavioural treatment programme for dogs with firework fears E. D. Levine, D. S. Mills NOISE fears are a common behavioural problem in dogs (Overall 1997, 2002, Beaver 1999, Landsberg and others 2003) that can significantly reduce the animal s welfare (Beerda and others 1997, Ladewig 2000, Hydbring-Sandberg and others 2004, Dreschel 2004, Dreschel and Granger 2005). Treatment plans for noise fears, such as behavioural modification programmes, psychoactive medication and pheromone therapy, are often recommended (Askew 1996, Overall 1997, Beaver 1999, Horwitz and others 2002, Landsberg and others 2003, Houpt 2005). While these are commonly advocated treatment plans, few studies have examined the efficacy of such treatments. Studies that have investigated noise fear or phobia treatments (Crowell-Davis and others 2003, Mills and others 2003, Levine and others 2007) have been limited to short-term outcome (up to four months). This short communication describes the one-year follow-up of 38 dogs that were involved in an eight-week treatment trial investigating the efficacy of using noise recordings, in conjunction with dog-appeasing-pheromone (DAP; Ceva Santé Animale), for desensitisation and counter-conditioning of dogs with firework fears (Levine and others 2007). The goal of using the noise recording and the DAP was to help teach the dogs not to be fearful of noises by using a medium (a sound recording on a CD) over which the owners could control the level of exposure, according to the dog s ability to cope, in order to steadily build up its tolerance. DAP has been reported to reduce anxiety in dogs in a variety of contexts, especially in relation to noise stimuli (Sheppard and Mills 2003). In the original study (Levine and others 2007), owners were given one of two self-contained noise reduction programmes and instructed to read and follow the instructions that accompanied their respective CD. The owners were not given any further clarification of the directions that accompanied the CD at this time. Half of the participants received a programme called Fear of Fireworks (FOF) (fearoffireworks. com) and the other half a programme called Sounds Scary (SS) (Sounds Scary Ltd). The distribution of the CDs to participants was matched based on initial owner perceived global fear scores (scale 0 to 10). Both programmes recommended the use of DAP. In order to help ensure even compliance with the use of pheromone therapy, DAP was distributed to all participants free of charge. In order to monitor the level of fear exhibited by these dogs throughout the study and respective follow-ups, the same questionnaires were used throughout the study (baseline and two follow-ups in the original study, as well as the Veterinary Record (2008) 162, XXX-XXX E. D. Levine, BSc, DVM, DiplACVB, MRCVS, D. S. Mills, BVSc, PhD, ILTM, CBiol, MIBiol, DipECVBM-CA, MRCVS, Animal Behaviour, Cognition and Welfare Group, University of Lincoln, Riseholme Park, Riseholme LN2 2LG Correspondence to Professor Mills The Veterinary Record, DATE

C4438 2 present follow-up study). In the questionnaires, owners identified both the frequency and the intensity of individual fear-related behaviours in which their dogs engaged during fireworks while inside the home. Answer options for frequencies of behaviours were never (0), rarely (1), frequently (2), and every time (3). Answer options about intensity of a behaviour were numerical ratings from 1 to 5 with 1 being a small amount and 5 being an extensive amount. The severity of individual behaviours was calculated by multiplying the frequency by the intensity of that behaviour. The total severity fear score for each dog was calculated by summing the severities of each behaviour. The total severity fear scores were then converted into percentages by dividing each individual total severity fear score by the total possible severity score. The owners were also asked to assign a global fear score on a scale from 0 to 10 relating to their perception of their dogs firework fear. Zero related to a mild fear response whereas 10 was the most severe fear response imaginable to them. An option to say there was no fear present was also provided at follow-up. For all follow-up interviews owners were asked to give a global fear score. The results of the initial investigation revealed that the majority of dogs showed behavioural improvement when exposed to real fireworks up to three months after the initial eight-week treatment programme. The current study (12 months after the initial eight-week programme) collected follow-up information in November 2005. Of the original 38 dogs that completed the training programme, 30 were exposed to fireworks on Bonfire Night, November 5, 2005, while the dogs remained in their homes. The majority of owners (89 per cent) reported that the severity of firework exposures were either similar to or worse than the previous year. The median total severity fear score for the 30 dogs was significantly lower in November 2005 compared to August 2004 (baseline) (Wilcoxon matched pairs test, P<0 001) (Fig 1). The global fear scores provided by the owners also significantly decreased from August 2004 to November 2005 (global fear scores of 8 and 5 respectively, P<0 001) and there was a positive correlation between the global fear scores and the total severity fear scores (Pearson correlation=0 816, P<0 001). With regards to individual fearrelated behaviours, only 23 dogs met the criteria to be included in the analysis (that is, information from all three exit interviews). Of these 23 dogs, there was significant improvement from baseline (August 2004) to each exit interview in eight behaviours (Fig 2). Pacing showed significant improvement in November 2004 and January 2005, but not in November 2005. The behaviours seeking the owner and scanning the environment did not show any significant improvement at any exit interview in this population. Vocalisation was only reported to significantly improve in November 2005. For this 12-month follow-up study, only 30 per cent of the owners reported using the CD before Bonfire Night 2005 and only 23 per cent reported using DAP before the final follow-up. Sixty-six per cent of these owners reported either a moderate or great improvement with respect to their dog s fear of fireworks with one owner reporting that their dog was worse. Seventy-four per cent of owners reported that they were either moderately or very satisfied with the results they had seen. Both owner-reported improvement and owner satisfaction results were similar to those reported in November 2004 and January 2005 (Levine and others 2007). Eighty- 2 The Veterinary Record, DATE

C4438 3 three per cent of owners stated that they would use this programme again if they were to acquire another dog that was fearful of fireworks. The positive correlation between the reported global fear scores and total severity fear scores in the current study and in the previous study (Levine and others 2007) are encouraging, as it appears, for this population, the owners overall impression of their dog s level of fear is correlated with the reported severity of individual fear-related behaviours and reflected behavioural improvement. However, it should not be assumed in other instances that global fear scores will reflect improvement in signs, since it is possible that the global fear assessment scores may be identifying components of the fear behaviour (for example, the overall emotional state of the animal) that are not revealed by simply scoring individual behaviours, or may be biased towards certain signs of particular concern to the owner (Crowell-Davis and others 2003). Thus Mills and others (2006) have recently argued that reliable global measures of emotional state may be more useful measures of treatment response than specific behaviours in studies such as this, not least because they might also require fewer subjects for a reliable result. Further research is needed to validate the use of owner-reported global fear scores. The sustained improvement for up to one year after the training period with very few individuals repeating the training or continuing the use of DAP before the last exit interview is of interest and provides, for the first time, solid evidence of the longer-term outcome of such programmes, assuming the original date were reliable. Owners are commonly advised to continue with desensitisation and counter-conditioning even if they believe their animal has improved; however, if, as in this report, animals appear to display sustained improvement one year following treatment with no or little retraining periods in between, such continuation therapy may be unnecessary. Clients may be more compliant if they are told at the outset that it is unlikely that they will need to repeat training on a permanent basis following initial resolution. However, it is worth noting that the majority of the individual fearrelated behaviours scores did start to show an increase from the second exit interview (January 2005); therefore, retraining may be necessary at a later date, if the problem continues to re-emerge over time. For the 23 dogs for which individual behaviors were statistically analysed, neither owner-seeking nor scanning the environment improved throughout the study. Owner-seeking behaviour may not have improved as owners frequently tend to reinforce their dog seeking them out by rewarding them with vocal, eye or physical contact. Scanning the environment, or vigilance, may not have improved as this behaviour may be related to information gathering. Vigilance may be related to the orienting response. Identifying the source of a potentially threatening sound would be evolutionarily advantageous, as locating the source of the sound would give the animal information to help execute the most appropriate behaviours; for example, running in a direction away from the source. Therefore, it may be expected that this particular behaviour will take longer to dissipate than behaviours such as panting and trembling, which do not have the potential to provide information to the animal, but are an expression of a more intense emotional response. Interestingly, pacing improved for the first two exit interviews (November 2004, The Veterinary Record, DATE 3

C4438 4 January 2005), but was not improved significantly by the third exit interview one year later. It may be that certain behaviours are more likely to need retraining more frequently than others in order to sustain improvement or that certain behaviours emerge at different times. An ethological analysis of the emergence of this problem might be a useful future study. The majority of owners were satisfied with their dog s improvement and stated they would be willing to use this protocol again if they were to acquire another dog that was scared of fireworks. Despite their reporting willingness to use it for a new dog, it is interesting to note how few of them reinstated training for their own dogs in this study. In summary, it appears that an eight-week training period of desensitisation and counter-conditioning with a noise recording and the use of DAP for firework fears can result in significant reported improvement in the long term, and so should be used as a routine part of a treatment plan for firework fears in dogs. References ASKEW, H. R. (1996) Fear problems. In Treatment of Behavioural Problems in Dogs and Cats. 1st ed. Oxford, Blackwell Science. pp 204-215 BEAVER, B. (1999) Canine behaviour of sensory and neural origin. In Canine Behavior: A Guide for Veterinarians. Ed B. Beaver. Philadelphia, W. B. Saunders. pp 43-105 BEERDA AND OTHERS (1997) CROWELL-DAVIS, S., SEIBERT, L. M., SUNG, W., PARTHASARATHY, V. & CURTIS, T. M. (2003) Use of clomipramine, alprazolam, and behaviour modification for treatment of storm phobia in dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 222, 744-749 DRESCHEL, N. A. (2004) Salivary cortisol measurements in dogs. In Proceedings of the Annual Symposium of Animal Behaviour Research, American Veterinary Society of Animal Behaviour. Philadelphia, July 26, 2004. p 8 DRESCHEL, N. & GRANGER, D. (2005) Physiological and behavioural reactivity to stress in thunderstorm-phobic dogs and their caregivers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 95, 135-168 HORWITZ AND OTHERS (2002) HOUPT, K. A. (2005) Miscellaneous behavioral disorders. In Domestic Animal Behaviour. 4th edn. Ed K. Houpt. Ames, Blackwell Publishing. pp 373-374 HYDBRING-SANDBERG, E., VON WALTER, L. W., HOGLAND, K., SVARTBERG, K., SWENSON, L. & FORKMAN, B. (2004) Physiological reactions to fear provocation in dogs. Journal of Endocrinology 180, 439-448 LADEWIG, J. (2000) Chronic intermittent stress: A model for the study of long-term stressors. In The Biology of Animal Stress. Eds G. Moberg, J. Mench. Wallingford, CABI Publishing. pp 159-169 LANDSBERG, G., HUNTHAUSEN, W. & ACKERMAN, L. (2003) Fears and phobias. In Handbook of Behaviour Problems of the Dog and Cat. 2nd edn. Toronto, Saunders. pp 227-268 LEVINE, E., RAMOS, D. & MILLS, D. (2007) A prospective study of two selfhelp CD-based desensitization and counter-conditioning programmes with the use of Dog Appearing Pheromone for the treatment of firework fears in dogs (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science (In press) MILLS, D., GANDIA ESTELLES, M., COLESHAW, P. H. & SHORTHOUSE, C. (2003) Retrospective analysis of the treatment of firework fears in dogs. Veterinary Record 153, 561-562 MILLS, D., RAMOS, D., GANDIA ESTELLES, M. & HARGRAVE, C. (2006) A triple blind placebo-controlled investigation into the assessment of the effect of Dog Appearing Pheromone (DAP) on anxiety related behaviour of problem dogs in the veterinary clinic. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 98, 114-126 4 The Veterinary Record, DATE

C4438 5 OVERALL, K. (1997) Fears, anxieties, and stereotypies. In Clinical Behavioural Medicinwwe for Small Animals. 1st edn. St Louis, Mosby. pp 209-250 OVERALL, K. (2002) Noise phobias in dogs. In BSAVA Manual of Canine and Feline Behavioural Medicine. Eds D. Horwitz, D. Mills, S. Heath. Gloucester, British Small Animal Veterinary Association. pp 164-172 SHEPPARD, G. & MILLS, D. S. (2003) Evaluation of dog appeasing pheromone as a potential treatment for dogs fearful of fireworks. Veterinary Record 152, 432-436 Median proportionate total severity score 0 6 0 5 0 4 0 3 0 2 0 1 0 n Both CDs n Fear of Fireworks CD n Sounds Scary CD Aug 04 Nov 04 Jan 05 Nov 05 Month of interview FIG 1: Standardised total severity scores reflecting response to noises by dogs at baseline (August 2004) and at each follow-up interview following treatment with Dog Appeasing Syndrome and CD-based sound desensitisation therapy. All values in November 2004, January 2005 and November 2005 are significantly different (P 0 05) from baseline 12 10 n Aug 04 n Nov 04 n Jan 05 n Nov 05 Mean severity score 8 6 4 * * * * ** ** * ** * ** * * * * 2 ** * * * * ** * 0 Run Drool Hide Cower Pace Freeze Vocalise Individual behaviours Pant Seek-owner Scan Bolt Startle Shake FIG 2: Mean severity scores for individual responses of 23 dogs to noise at baseline (August 2004) and at each follow-up interview following treatment with Dog Appeasing Pheromone and CD-based sound desensitisation therapy. *Significant difference compared to baseline value, P 0 05 The Veterinary Record, DATE 5