--- By --- Joshua Frazier Hanover. March 21 st, 2017

Similar documents
DETECTION OF MAGNETIC INCLINATION ANGLE BY SEA TURTLES: A POSSIBLE MECHANISM FOR DETERMINING LATITUDE

Representation, Visualization and Querying of Sea Turtle Migrations Using the MLPQ Constraint Database System

MAGNETIC ORIENTATION AND NAVIGATION BEHAVIOR OF LOGGERHEAD SEA TURTLE HATCHLINGS (Caretta caretta) DURING THEIR TRANSOCEANIC MIGRATION

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

PERCEPTION OF OCEAN WAVE DIRECTION BY SEA TURTLES

RESEARCH ARTICLE Orientation of hatchling loggerhead sea turtles to regional magnetic fields along a transoceanic migratory pathway

A Sea Turtle's. by Laurence Pringle illustrated by Diane Blasius

D irections. The Sea Turtle s Built-In Compass. by Sudipta Bardhan

Your web browser (Safari 7) is out of date. For more security, comfort and the best experience on this site: Update your browser Ignore

Title Temperature among Juvenile Green Se.

Navigation and seasonal migratory orientation in juvenile sea turtles

Bald Head Island Conservancy 2018 Sea Turtle Report Emily Goetz, Coastal Scientist

Trapped in a Sea Turtle Nest

North Carolina Aquariums Education Section. Prepare to Hatch. Created by the NC Aquarium at Fort Fisher Education Section

ORIENTATION TO OCEANIC WAVES BY GREEN TURTLE HATCHLINGS

Title. Grade level. Time. Student Target. PART 3 Lesson: Populations. PART 3 Activity: Turtles, Turtle Everywhere! minutes

SEA TU RTL ES AND THE GU L F O F MEXICO O IL SPIL L

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International Advanced Subsidiary and Advanced Level

Human Impact on Sea Turtle Nesting Patterns

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

Response to SERO sea turtle density analysis from 2007 aerial surveys of the eastern Gulf of Mexico: June 9, 2009

Field report to Belize Marine Program, Wildlife Conservation Society

Biology Of Sea Turtles, Vol. 1

Teaching Assessment Lessons

Who Really Owns the Beach? The Competition Between Sea Turtles and the Coast Renee C. Cohen

Bycatch records of sea turtles obtained through Japanese Observer Program in the IOTC Convention Area

Teacher Workbooks. Language Arts Series Internet Reading Comprehension Oceans Theme, Vol. 1

PARTIAL REPORT. Juvenile hybrid turtles along the Brazilian coast RIO GRANDE FEDERAL UNIVERSITY

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

A SPATIAL ANALYSIS OF SEA TURTLE AND HUMAN INTERACTION IN KAHALU U BAY, HI. By Nathan D. Stewart

Title. Grade level. Time. Student Target. Materials. PART 2 Lesson: Nesting. PART 2 Activity: Are you my Mother? minutes

When a species can t stand the heat

North Carolina Aquariums Education Section. You Make the Crawl. Created by the NC Aquarium at Fort Fisher Education Section

D irections. The Sea Turtle s Built-In Compass. by Sudipta Bardhan

SEA TURTLE CHARACTERISTICS

Is it better to be bigger? Featured scientists: Aaron Reedy and Robert Cox from the University of Virginia Co-written by Matt Kustra

Define evidence based practices for selection and duration of antibiotics to treat suspected or confirmed neonatal sepsis

Pikas. Pikas, who live in rocky mountaintops, are not known to move across non-rocky areas or to

from an experimental bag net SHIODE, DAISUKE; TAKAHASHI, MUTSUKI Proceedings of the 6th Internationa SEASTAR2000 workshop) (2011): 31-34

SOAR Research Proposal Summer How do sand boas capture prey they can t see?

TURTLE PATROL VOLUNTEER REFERENCE GUIDE

Final Report for Research Work Order 167 entitled:

Tour de Turtles: It s a Race for Survival! Developed by Gayle N Evans, Science Master Teacher, UFTeach, University of Florida

D. Burke \ Oceans First, Issue 3, 2016, pgs

COCA-LOCA : Connectivity of Loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta) in Western Indian Ocean, implementation of local and regional management measures

Accepted 6 February 2012

A brief report on the 2016/17 monitoring of marine turtles on the São Sebastião peninsula, Mozambique

INDIVIDUAL IDENTIFICATION OF GREEN TURTLE (CHELONIA MYDAS) HATCHLINGS

IN-WATER SEA TURTLE DISTRIBUTION AND ABUNDANCE MONITORING ON PALM BEACH COUNTY NEARSHORE REEFS FOR:

Tristan Darwin Project. Monitoring Guide. A Guide to Monitoring Albatross, Penguin and Seal Plots on Tristan and Nightingale

Treasured Turtles GO ON

When a species can t stand the heat

Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1

Oil Spill Impacts on Sea Turtles

DARK SKIES & SEA TURTLE NESTING

I. Introduction. Orientation and Navigation 3/8/2012. Most difficult problem Must know. How birds find their way. Two terms often misused

Types of Data. Bar Chart or Histogram?

BBRG-5. SCTB15 Working Paper. Jeffrey J. Polovina 1, Evan Howell 2, Denise M. Parker 2, and George H. Balazs 2

Dr Kathy Slater, Operation Wallacea

Loggerhead Turtles: Creature Feature

Return to the sea: Marine birds, reptiles and pinnipeds

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt

Legal Supplement Part B Vol. 53, No th March, NOTICE THE ENVIRONMENTALLY SENSITIVE SPECIES (GREEN TURTLE) NOTICE, 2014

Greece Turtle Conservation

1995 Activities Summary

AN APPLIED CASE STUDY of the complexity of ecological systems and process: Why has Lyme disease become an epidemic in the northeastern U.S.

LOGGERHEADLINES FALL 2017

Marine Reptiles. Four types of marine reptiles exist today: 1. Sea Turtles 2. Sea Snakes 3. Marine Iguana 4. Saltwater Crocodile

Voyage of the Turtle

Structured PVA Historical essay: for example history of protection of Everglades

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

FACT FUN! *Loggerheads are the most common species of sea turtle in the ocean off of South Carolina.

The Effect of Localized Oil Spills on the Atlantic Loggerhead Turtle Population Dynamics

TURTLE TIMES. Turtle Foundation SEPTEMBER 2016 Protecting sea turtles and their habitats TURTLE TIMES SEPTEMBER 2016

Great Science Adventures Lesson 12

Teacher Guide Teacher Answer Key and Kentucky Core Academic Standards for RPA 1 Grade 3

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Sea Turtles and Longline Fisheries: Impacts and Mitigation Experiments

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Growth analysis of juvenile green sea turtles (Chelonia mydas) by gender.

METEROLOGICAL AND OCEANOGRAPHIC FACTORS IMPACTING SEA TURTLE NESTING

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

People around the world should be striving to preserve a healthy environment for both humans and

Zander Srodes. Turtle Talks Activity Book

BIODIVERSITY CONSERVATION AND HABITAT MANAGEMENT Vol. II Initiatives For The Conservation Of Marine Turtles - Paolo Luschi

The Development of Behavior

DOWNLOAD OR READ : SEA TURTLES ANIMALS THAT LIVE IN THE OCEAN PDF EBOOK EPUB MOBI

2011 Winner: Yamazaki Double-Weight Branchline

Sea Turtle Strandings. Introduction

This article is downloaded from.

5 State of the Turtles

Dive-depth distribution of. coriacea), loggerhead (Carretta carretta), olive ridley (Lepidochelys olivacea), and

It Is Raining Cats. Margaret Kwok St #: Biology 438

The Effect of Aerial Exposure Temperature on Balanus balanoides Feeding Behavior

Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission Fish and Wildlife Research Institute Guidelines for Marine Turtle Permit Holders

Navigation by green turtles: which strategy do displaced adults use to find Ascension Island?

Relationship Between Eye Color and Success in Anatomy. Sam Holladay IB Math Studies Mr. Saputo 4/3/15

AN APPLIED CASE STUDY of the complexity of ecological systems and process: Why has Lyme disease become an epidemic in the northeastern U.S.

Animal Behavior: Biology 3401 Laboratory 4: Social behaviour of young domestic chickens

RESEARCH AND CONSERVATION AT GEORGIA AQUARIUM, INC.

Transcription:

Magnetoreception Abilities in Juvenile Loggerhead Sea Turtles --- By --- Joshua Frazier Hanover Senior Honors Thesis Biology University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill March 21 st, 2017 Approved:. Dr. Catherine Lohmann, Thesis Advisor Dr. Charles Mitchell Dr. Amy Maddox

Abstract: Loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta) complete extensive, open-ocean migrations around the North Atlantic subtropical gyre current system over the course of many years. Evidence suggests that magnetoreception, or the ability to detect the Earth s magnetic field, plays a role in their ability to navigate large, open areas lacking obvious cues. One possibility is that sea turtles use geographically predictable differences in the Earth s magnetic field in order to guide themselves. In order to exploit these geographical differences, turtles must be able to monitor changes in the ambient magnetic field. Two experimental methods were used to assess how juvenile sea turtles react to a changing magnetic field. The first, an orientation experiment, was designed to test whether juvenile loggerhead sea turtles can detect and respond to changes in the inclination (angle at which the field intersects the Earth s surface) and intensity (strength of the field) in a simulated environment. The second, an activity experiment, was performed to develop a laboratory-based behavioral assay to quantify changes in the activity level of juvenile sea turtles when exposed to a rapidly-changing magnetic field. This information could be valuable to future studies addressing the mechanisms underlying magnetoreception in migratory animals. In both experiments, a similar approach was taken in which juvenile turtles were placed into an aquatic arena with a changing magnetic field. For the orientation investigation, the field changed in accordance with the turtle s own movement and an orientation vector was calculated. For the activity investigation, the field changed in a predetermined manner and activity was quantified by comparing the number of flipper strokes in the absence or presence of the change. Orientation trial results were inconclusive in that animals did not respond to the applied magnetic fields, unlike the animals in previous experiments. Activity trials failed to demonstrate that turtles have an activity response to a changing magnetic field. Further research into how juvenile loggerhead sea turtles respond to changes in a magnetic field are needed, including studies that examine how responses vary with age, nest location, and how their behavior compares with that of other species. Results of this and similar studies could help inform future conservation efforts for similar vulnerable, migratory species. 2

Introduction: Loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta) that hatch on the eastern coast of the United States complete extraordinary migrations around the massive ocean current system known as the North Atlantic subtropical gyre (Fig. 1) (Godley et al., 2008; Kenneth J. Lohmann, Putman, & Lohmann, 2012). The Gyre current flows from the eastern United States coast, across to the western European coast, down the coast of Africa, across to the Caribbean, and back up the United States coast. Individuals spend several years within the gyre system before returning to the eastern coastal waters of North America as juveniles (Avens et al., 2003). As adults, the turtles migrate between open-ocean feeding sites and nesting beaches, which are often the same, natal beaches from which they themselves hatched (Avens et al., 2003; Carr, 1987; Godley et al., 2008; Kenneth J. Lohmann & Lohmann, 1996). 3

Figure 1. This image shows the North Atlantic Subtropical Gyre current predicted magnetic field isolines where the magnetic field inclination angle is constant. The North Atlantic subtropical gyre current flows from the eastern United States coast, across to the western European coast, down the coast of Africa, across to the Caribbean, and back up the United States coast system and, as represented by the blue arrows. The magnetic field variations are represented by the red, dashed lines. Taken from the Lohmann Lab website at (http://www.unc.edu/depts/oceanweb/turtles/). Out in the open ocean, there is little guidance in terms of environmental cues (visual, olfactory, or auditory) to help turtles orient themselves. Yet, an ability to navigate in the open ocean is vital. If turtles veer off course too far, they could enter colder water currents and perish (Godley et al., 2008; Mansfield, Wyneken, Porter, & Luo, 2014). Considerable evidence suggests that loggerhead sea turtles use Earth s magnetic field, which varies geographically, to navigate (Fuxjager, Eastwood, & Lohmann, 2011; Johnsen & Lohmann, 2005; K. Lohmann & 4

Lohmann, 1996; K J Lohmann & Lohmann, 1998; Kenneth J. Lohmann & Lohmann, 1994, 1996; Kenneth J Lohmann, 1991). The field varies in a predictable way (Fig. 1). At each location on Earth, the magnetic field has two specific properties: inclination and intensity. The inclination angle is the angle at which magnetic field lines intersect the Earth s surface and the intensity is the strength of the field (Kenneth J. Lohmann & Lohmann, 1994, 1996). Organisms capable of distinguishing both the inclination and intensity of the field can potentially determine their approximate global position using this information, and magnetoreception, or an animal s ability to detect and respond to magnetic fields, is a well-supported phenomenon (Avens et al., 2003; Johnsen & Lohmann, 2005; K. Lohmann & Lohmann, 1996; K J Lohmann, Cain, Dodge, & Lohmann, 2001; K J Lohmann & Lohmann, 1998; Kenneth J. Lohmann & Lohmann, 1994, 1996; Kenneth J Lohmann, 1991). However, the precise mechanism by which organisms detect the geomagnetic field is still a topic of much debate (Godley et al., 2008). This research used a computer-controlled electromagnetic coil system to generate the magnetic fields that turtles naturally encounter within the gyre. As the turtle swims, the magnetic field changes to simulate the fields the turtle would encounter if it were swimming the same direction in the wild. Thus, the turtle swims in a virtual reality environment designed to mimic its migratory environment. One focus of this research was to determine if young turtles will display their migratory route in this type of simulated, virtual environment. Turtles were exposed to magnetic fields from three different locations within the Gyre and their orientation was measured to determine what direction they attempted to travel from those various locations. This could shed light on exactly what juvenile sea turtles do during their formative years in the gyre. A second focus was to develop a behavioral assay to quickly and reliably quantify magnetic field response under laboratory conditions. Such an assay would enable further research into how organisms respond to a changing magnetic field. The turtles in this study were exposed to rapidly changing magnetic fields and their activity before and after the change was examined and quantified by counting flipper strokes. This could provide a measurement that could be used to determine if an organism is detecting changes in the magnetic field. 5

Materials and Methods: Animals: For both experiments, juvenile loggerhead sea turtles (Fig. 2) were collected from Bald Head Island, North Carolina, during the 2015 (from early August to mid-september) hatching season as they emerged from shallow, underground nests. All turtles were transported within 72 hours to the Lohmann Lab DLAM facility at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, where they were housed in identical tanks and handled according to IACUC protocol. Experiments began in September, 2015, and continued through March, 2016. Figure 2. This photograph shows one of the juvenile turtles in its individual tank in the Lohmann Lab DLAM facility. Orientation Experiment: Set-Up: The aquatic arena and coil system used to create the virtual reality in the experiments consisted of a fiberglass tank filled with salt water that acted as the experimental environment (Fig. 3) and were similar to previous experimental setups (Fuxjager et al., 2011; Kenneth J 6

Lohmann, 1991). The tank had blue LED lights surrounding the rim (not pictured) in order to negate any external light bias. To further eliminate bias, blackout curtains surrounded the entire electromagnetic arena (not pictured). The turtle was tethered in a small, nylon-lycra harness and attached to a lever arm via fishing wire (Fig. 4). A computer-controlled system of power supplies was used to send current through a box of coil wires surrounding the tank and generate specific magnetic fields inside the arena (Merritt, Purcell, & Stroink, 1983). During experiments, the turtle was placed in a small, nylon-lycra harness and tethered with fishing line to a rotatable lever arm in the center of the tank (Fig. 4). The rotatable arm shifted direction as the turtle moved about the tank during the trials. The position of the arm was transmitted by a digital encoder to the computer to track the turtle s direction throughout the trial. In addition, as the turtle swam, the computer program altered the magnetic field around the turtle as if the turtle were swimming in the actual ocean in the same direction. Thus, for example, southward swimming caused generation of a field from further south, while eastward swimming would cause generation of a field from further east. The field generated matched specific inclination and intensity for oceanic locations taken from the International Geomagnetic Reference Field model (Jankowski & Sucksdorff, 1996). Thus, the turtle would be swimming in a virtual reality based on real geomagnetism. It should be noted, however, that the change in magnetic field was very rapid, and was set to reflect unrealistic swimming speeds in an attempt to produce a stimulus that the turtles would notice in a laboratory setting during short trials. 7

Figure 3. This is a diagram of the experimental set-up. It shows the computer control and monitoring system, the aquatic arena, and the surrounding coil system. Protocol: Each turtle was tested individually and according to the same protocol, but never twice in the same day. Testing occurred each day between the hours of 9:00am and 4:00pm. All electronic equipment was calibrated and the power supplies for the magnetic field simulator were turned on by the experimenter. The water temperature was tested to ensure that it was adequate. All external stimuli (lights, air currents, electronic devices) were negated to the best of our abilities. The synthetic magnetic field was set using the computer based on the latitude and longitude of one of three specific locations in the North Atlantic Gyre (Barbados, North East Gyre [right off the coast of Portugal], or Topsail). The turtle was then selected from the housing facility one floor below, placed in a covered plastic container with holes, and transported to the experimental room. The turtle was harnessed and the overhead lights are turned off. It was given five minutes to adjust to the feel of the harness and the new magnetic field in the arena. If the turtle was clearly uncomfortable with the harness (struggling to remove it by biting or scratching), it was repositioned or a new size was used. Another five minutes are given for readjustment. 8

After the adjustment period, a trial was started with the magnetic field changing at 2,000 miles per hour (2K). Caretta, a custom computer software, tracked the position and movement of the turtle using the position of the magnetic arm, which shifted as the turtle swam around the tank. Caretta was also able to change the magnetic field inside of the arena in real-time according to the direction the turtle was swimming. The experimenters took handwritten notes about the turtle s observable behavior during the trial. After fifteen minutes of changing electromagnetic field, the 2K trial ended and the experimenter decided whether the data is useable based on if the turtle swam the entire time (power stroking), treaded water (paddling), or tucked their front flippers and only kicked their back flippers (tucking and rear flipper kicking). If the turtle tucked for too long during the fifteen-minute trial (usually 3 minutes or more), the trial was aborted. The turtle was taken back to its housing tank, and a new individual was brought up. However, if successful, the ending latitude and longitude were recorded in Excel. The overhead lights were turned on for two to three minutes while the experimenter reset the location to one of three of the initial starting points of the experiment. The lights were turned off and the turtle was given two or three minutes to readjust to the dark. The experiment was run again, but this time with the location changing at a speed of 10,000 miles per hour (10K). The data from this 10K trial, if successful, was recorded for analysis. The use of a moving magnetic field was motivated in part by earlier evidence suggesting that turtles are more motivated to attend to a moving field rather than a static one; we thus hoped to maximize the chances to eliciting magnetic orientation (Johnsen & Lohmann, 2005; K. Lohmann & Lohmann, 1996). Analysis: Each trial had a starting location and an ending location with a specific recorded latitude and longitude. Using these two locations, a bearing vector was calculated using a script from www.moveabletype.co.uk. for each successful trial. Only data from the 10K trials was analyzed. The 2K trials served as adaptation periods for the turtles to acclimate to a changing magnetic field. All trials and bearings were analyzed using the computer program Oriana, which uses circular data statistics for vector and orientation analysis. Data was collected for first, second, and third trials (Kovach, 1993). Generally, turtles were able to run a first and 9

second trial at each of the three locations. However, many turtles were unable to run a third trial due to time constraints. Analysis yielded a mean angle from the combined individual turtle trials. A Rayleigh r-value for each distribution was also calculated in order to ensure the determination of a 95% confidence interval. Activity Experiment: Set-Up: Again, the custom-built magnetic coil system was used to create a dynamic virtual magnetic environment which altered the magnetic field as if the turtle were swimming in the specified direction at a specific location in the open ocean. The coil system was similar to the one used in the orientation experiments (Fuxjager et al., 2011; Kenneth J Lohmann, 1991; Merritt et al., 1983). However, in this arena, the turtle was secured in a nylon harness and attached to a stationary arm instead of a rotatable one. Furthermore, two GoPro cameras were positioned at the top of the coil system to record the trial so that activity levels could be assessed via time-lapse at a later time. Protocol: The starting magnetic field of the arena was set to imitate the magnetic field expected offshore of Topsail Island (latitude 34, longitude -75 ). Turtles were harnessed and placed in the experimental tank with all external lights turned off. They were given a five-minute acclimation period in which they adjusted to the harness and the lighting conditions. After these initial five minutes, turtles experienced a five-minute pretreatment period (Fig. 4). During this period, the magnetic field remained static. This period was used to determine baseline turtle activity in a static magnetic field. Then, either a control or experimental trial was started, and activity was recorded for five more minutes. During the control trials, the turtle experienced a static magnetic field (i.e., as if it was moving at 0 miles per hour). During the experimental trials, the turtle experienced a changing magnetic field as if it was moving in one of two directions away from the starting location (45 or 225 ) at a fast pace (10,000 miles per hour). The direction bearing (45 or 225 ) was pre-set depending on the treatment by manipulating the digital encoder before the trial began. Fifteen turtles were used for each of the three treatments, and the order of the trials was random for each turtle. 10

At a later date, all videos were analyzed frame-by-frame. Power strokes, or strong flipper strokes meant to propel the turtle forward during swimming, were counted to determine activity. For each trial, the first five minutes during which the turtle was becoming acclimated were not reviewed. The next five-minute pretreatment periods were reviewed and the number of power strokes was recorded for each minute. An average number of power strokes per minute for the pretreatment period was determined from this. During the final five minutes of each trial, the turtle experienced the control or one of the two experimental conditions (Fig. 4). Again, power strokes were counted and recorded for each minute and an average was determined. Figure 4. This flow-chart demonstrates the timeline of the experimental protocol. It is broken up into three, fiveminute sections: the adjustment period, the pretreatment period, and the treatment period. Analysis: For each treatment condition (control, 45, or 225 ), the ratio of average activity during the trial to the average baseline activity during pretreatment in strokes per minute was calculated. Shapiro-Wilk tests of normality indicated that ratio data were normally distributed 11

in each of the treatments. A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to test for significant differences between the mean ratios of the three treatments. Box-plots were used to represent the median, upper/lower quartiles, range, and outliers for the ratio of each treatment group. All statistical analyses were performed using the statistical program R (R Core Team. & R Development Core Team, 2013). Orientation Experiment Results: Turtles were tested three times under three different magnetic conditions, replicating locations near Topsail, Barbados, or the Northeastern Gyre (a location near Portugal). Analysis of the first trial for each turtle indicate that as a group, turtles oriented significantly towards the Northeast when the starting location was Topsail, but were statistically indistinguishable from random with starting locations of Barbados or NE Gyre. (Fig. 5, Trial 1). Analysis of the second trials demonstrated no significant orientation at any magnetic location (Fig 5, Trial 2). In Trial 3, turtles were significantly oriented only in the NE gyre trial, but samples sizes were low (Fig. 5, Trial 3). For each turtle, an average from all trials was calculated for each magnetic location. As a group, turtles were highly significantly oriented to the NE at the Topsail location, the NW at the NE Gyre location and were statistically indistinguishable from random at the Barbados location (Fig. 5, Individual Averages). 12

Figure 5. For each circle graph, the label above tells whether it is the data gathered from the first, second, or third trial. The last three circle graphs represent the average of all the trials for each turtle at each location. For each graph, the blue arrows indicate a vector showing which direction a turtle swam in that trial at that location. The solid line indicates the average bearing of all of the trials. The curved line on the outside of the circle represents the confidence interval (it is red if the confidence interval is not within 95%). The X-bar represents the average bearing for each location; the sample size, r, Z, and p values are all listed in the box to the right of each graph. 13

Activity Experiment Results: Baseline activity of turtles was recorded before a magnetic field change and compared to treatment activity after a magnetic field change. Two different virtual reality conditions were used: one where the turtle was traveling to the NE (45 ) at 10k miles per hour and another where the turtle was traveling to the SW (225 ) at the same speed. In addition, a control with no magnetic field change was also performed. The ratio of mean control treatment activity to mean baseline activity was 0.9762 (SEM = 0.0691). For the 45 treatment, the mean treatment activity to mean baseline activity ratio was 0.8610 (SEM = 0.1173). For the 225 treatment, the mean treatment activity to mean baseline activity ratio was 0.9829 (SEM = 0.0449). These ratios were not significantly different from one another [ANOVA: F(2,14) = 0.63, p = 0.54; Fig. 5.] (R Core Team. & R Development Core Team, 2013). Treatment : Baseline Ratio of Activity Treatment Figure 6. This image represents the activity ratios determined in each treatment in three box plots. The dark bar inside of the box represents the median, the rectangular box shows the upper and lower quartiles, the capped lines extending from the box show the inferred limits of the nominal range of the data, and any outliers are represented by open circles. 14

Conclusion and Discussion: Based on prior research with hatchling turtles from Florida, it has begun to be accepted that turtles emerge from their nests with innate responses to specific geographic fields that keep them within their nursery habitat (Lohmann et al., 2001). Grounded by these previous findings, each location tested in the orientation experiment had a predicted direction the turtles should travel, given the starting magnetic field. The turtles at the Barbados virtual location were hypothesized to travel North and slightly East in order to enter into the gyre. Those released at a magnetic location close to Europe at the Northeast gyre location were hypothesized to travel South. Those at the Topsail location were hypothesized to travel East or Southeast (Lohmann & Lohmann, 1996; Lohmann et al., 2001; Lohmann et al., 2012). The only set of single trials that had a vector orientation with 95% confidence was the first trial at the Topsail location. Turtles during this trial swam in a mostly North but slightly East direction, (27.75 ) on average (Fig. 5, Trial 1). In the second and third trials at Topsail, turtles swam with the same mean angle but were not significantly oriented (Fig. 5, Trial 2 and Trial 3). The Barbados and North East Gyre trials resulted in data that did not have a confidence interval of 95%. While negative data must be interpreted with caution, it is possible that turtles this age from North Carolina do not have the same responses that turtles from Florida have, due to a plethora of reasons. For example, the lack of response at the North East Gyre suggests the possibility that NC turtles do not normally travel that far East, and may not have evolved responses to this geomagnetic location. Genetic evidence also indicates that NC turtles are underrepresented at the Azores Islands compared to Florida turtles, so there is support for the idea that the different responses mirror different nursery habitats (Richards et al., 2011). Furthermore, turtles from this experiment were taken from nests that were protected from predators on the NC coast by wire cages, which could disrupt the development of magnetoreception abilities (Irwin et al. 2004). The same may be true for Barbados, a location that Florida turtles might reach at a younger age than North Carolina turtles. One interesting trend to note is that the average vectors created with data from each location and for each trial seem to have a Northern bias. When all the data were summed together, a Northern bias could 15

not be demonstrated (data not shown), but the possibility and implication of a slight bias should be considered for future experiments. In this regard, it is interesting to note that other experiments in the Lohmann lab have hinted at the notion that juvenile turtle response to magnetic fields may change with age. At the same time that these experiments were being performed, a fellow undergraduate student, Kendall Bagely, conducted experiments on orientation comparing the vectors turtles traveled in from Topsail and Barbados and how these directions differ with age. Age: 0-5 months Age: 6-10 months Figure 7. The above circular representations show orientation vector findings for the same turtles at the Topsail and Barbados locations. The first pair of circles shows the average direction all turtles aged 0-5 months went while the second pair of circles shows the direction these turtles went when between the ages of 6-to months. Top Left: Significant orientation at Topsail to the northeast (p = 0.003, N = 14, X_ = 27.75, r = 0.626, 95% CI: 357.18-58.53 ). Top Right: No significant orientation at Barbados (p = 0.642, N = 16, X_ = 207.46, r = 0.168). Bottom Left: No significant orientation at Topsail (p = 0.531, N = 7, X_ = 47.45, r = 0.308). Bottom Right: Barbados orientation is significant in the northeast direction (p = 0.017, N = 9, X_ = 331.69, r = 0.651, 95% CI: 295.102-8.267 ). Taken from unpublished data in the Lohmann Lab performed by other researchers. The above data demonstrates how orientation in juvenile turtles shifted with age, possibly indicating ontogenetic behavior. At an older age, the turtles did not have a significant orientation vector at the Topsail location, whereas younger hatchlings did. However, the 16

opposite was true for the Barbados location, which showed that the same turtles used in this study demonstrated a strong orientation response at Barbados in the expected Northward direction after they reached an age of 6 months (Bagley, 2016). These findings suggest that juvenile turtles may have differing responses to changes in the magnetic field as they grow and develop. Based on anecdotal observations during orientation trials, we also hypothesized that juvenile turtles would exhibit higher levels of activity (more power strokes per minute) during the changing field period of the experimental trials than during baseline activity of the pretreatment period. If correct, higher activity ratio means for the experimental treatments than the ratio mean of the control treatment would be expected. The results from this experiment, however, did not support this hypothesis. The mean ratios were not statistically different from one another; there was thus no evidence that a changing magnetic field resulted in a change in activity. Unfortunately, it cannot be said with confidence that the results gained from these experiments support the claim that Loggerhead sea turtles detect and responded to changes in the Earth s magnetic field. We were unable to show that juvenile turtles have an orientation preference or that they change activity levels when exposed to a changing magnetic field. The results produced from these experiments are much less convincing than those produced from prior experiments (Fuxjager et al., 2011; K J Lohmann et al., 2001; Kenneth J. Lohmann et al., 2012). However, there are some differences in experimental method. One difference is the location from which the hatchlings were gathered. The published papers made use of turtles from nests in Florida, whereas these experiments used turtles hatched on North Carolina beaches (K. Lohmann & Lohmann, 1996; Kenneth J. Lohmann & Lohmann, 1994). This could have affected the results, and could point to why the Topsail locations have the highest confidence. Perhaps the North Carolina turtles are more adapt to detecting the Earth s magnetic field closer to where they are nested. The turtles used in these experiments could have been disorientated by the Barbados and North East Gyre locations because they were too far from their natal beaches. Furthermore, the published works used larger sample sizes than these experiments did (Fuxjager et al., 2011; K J Lohmann et al., 2001; Kenneth J. Lohmann et 17

al., 2012). A greater n-value could have yielded clearer results. To compound things, the previous experiments were run in total darkness using hatchlings plucked directly from the shore, whereas these experiments were conducted in a lighted tank and used individuals housed in a DLAM facility for an extended period of time (Avens et al., 2003; K J Lohmann et al., 2001). All of these variables could have affected the difference in actual and expected results. Overall, it was clear, from personal observation, that the comfort of the hatchling in the harness was crucial to obtaining a trial in which the turtle swam instead of just tucked. Additionally, external factors (such as feeding schedule, time of day, and handling during physicals) affected the quality of the turtle s swimming and subsequent trial. The prevailing theory about certain migration capabilities in animals is heavily invested in the notion of magnetic field detection (Lohmann & Lohmann, 1994). The types of experiments and data collection carried out this semester within the Lohmann lab could help shed light on some of the underlying principles of magnetoreception in many different organisms. Additionally, any information gathered on the endangered Loggerhead sea turtle is beneficial to further understanding and protecting the species. Developing an experiment that showed a statistically significant response to a changing magnetic field could serve as an essential assay to future magnetoreception experiments. It could provide quantitative data demonstrating that sea turtles and other migratory animals have an activity response to a changing field would shed light on the evolutionary history of magnetoreception. 18

Acknowledgements: I would like to generously thank: The Lohmann Lab Personnel Dr. Cathy M. F. Lohmann Ph. D, Lab Principal Investigator Dr. Ken J. Lohmann Ph. D, Head of Lohmann Labs David Steinberg Post-doc Mentor and Collaborator M. Vanessa Lynch Head of Turtle Husbandry Kendall Bagley Undergraduate Researcher Dominique Capaldo Undergraduate Researcher Nissa Coit Undergraduate Researcher Cori Lopazanski Undergraduate Researcher My BIOL 692H Peers Anita Simha BIOL 692 H Peer Reviewer Tara McKinnon BIOL 692 H Peer Reviewer Tracue Hayes BIOL 692 H Peer Reviewer Dr. Amy Maddox BIOL 692H Instructor And the Lohmann Lab Sea Turtles References Avens, L., Braun, M. J., Epperly, S., & Lohmann, K. J. (2003). Site fidelity and homing behavior in juvenile loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta). Marine Biology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-003-1085-9 Bagley, K. (2016). Loggerhead Sea Turtle Migration Using the Earth s Magnetic Field: A Virtual Reality Approach. Carr, A. (1987). New Perspectives on the Pelagic Stage of Sea Turtle Development. Conservation Biology. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.1987.tb00020.x Fuxjager, M. J., Eastwood, B. S., & Lohmann, K. J. (2011). Orientation of hatchling loggerhead sea turtles to regional magnetic fields along a transoceanic migratory pathway. The Journal of Experimental Biology. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.055921 Godley, B. J., Blumenthal, J. M., Broderick, A. C., Coyne, M. S., Godfrey, M. H., Hawkes, L. A., & Witt, M. J. (2008). Satellite tracking of sea turtles: Where have we been and where do we go next? Endangered Species Research. https://doi.org/10.3354/esr00060 Irwin, W. P., Horner, A. J., & Lohmann, K. J. (2004). Magnetic field distortions produced by protective cages around sea turtle nests: Unintended consequences for orientation and navigation? Biological Conservation 19

Jankowski, J., & Sucksdorff, C. (1996). Guide for Magnetic Measurements and Observatory Practice. Iaga. Johnsen, S., & Lohmann, K. J. (2005). The physics and neurobiology of magnetoreception. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1745 Kovach, W. (1993). Oriana. KOVACH COMPUTING SERVICES. Retrieved from http://www.kovcomp.co.uk/oriana/oribroc.html Lohmann, K. J. (1991). Magnetic orientation by hatchling loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta). Journal of Experimental Biology. Lohmann, K. J., Cain, S. D., Dodge, S. a, & Lohmann, C. M. (2001). Regional magnetic fields as navigational markers for sea turtles. Science (New York, N.Y.). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1064557 Lohmann, K. J., & Lohmann, C. M. F. (1994). Detection of Magnetic Inclination Angle By Sea Turtles: A Possible Mechanism for Determining Latitude. The Journal of Experimental Biology. Lohmann, K. J., & Lohmann, C. M. F. (1996). Detection of magnetic field intensity by sea turtles. Nature. https://doi.org/10.1038/380059a0 Lohmann, K. J., & Lohmann, C. M. F. (1998). Migratory guidance mechanisms in marine turtles. Journal of Avian Biology. https://doi.org/10.2307/3677179 Lohmann, K. J., Putman, N. F., & Lohmann, C. M. F. (2012). The magnetic map of hatchling loggerhead sea turtles. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.11.005 Lohmann, K., & Lohmann, C. (1996). Orientation and open-sea navigation in sea turtles. The Journal of Experimental Biology. Mansfield, K. L., Wyneken, J., Porter, W. P., & Luo, J. (2014). First satellite tracks of neonate sea turtles redefine the lost years oceanic niche. Proceedings. Biological Sciences / The Royal Society. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3039 Merritt, R., Purcell, C., & Stroink, G. (1983). Uniform magnetic field produced by three, four, and five square coils. Review of Scientific Instruments. https://doi.org/10.1063/1.1137480 R Core Team., & R Development Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Richards, P. M., Epperly, S. P., Heppell, S. S., King, R. T., Sasso, C. R., Moncada, F., Zurita, J. (2011). Sea turtle population estimates incorporating uncertainty: A new approach applied to western North Atlantic loggerheads Caretta caretta. Endangered Species Research. https://doi.org/10.3354/esr00379 20