CHICK GROWTH, SIBLING RIVALRY, AND CHICK

Similar documents
Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

THE BEGGING BEHAVIOR OF NESTLING EASTERN SCREECH-OWLS

You may use the information and images contained in this document for non-commercial, personal, or educational purposes only, provided that you (1)

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL

GULLS (LARUS ARGENTATUS)

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

TEMPORAL PATTERNS IN PRE-FLEDGLING SURVIVAL AND BROOD REDUCTION IN AN OSPREY COLONY

In the summers of 1977 and 1978, at Gibraltar Point, Lincolnshire, I

Piping Plover. Below: Note the color of the sand and the plover s back.

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

What is the date at which most chicks would have been expected to fledge?

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

Cape Hatteras National Seashore Resource Management Field Summary for July 15 July 21, 2010 (Bodie, Hatteras and Ocracoke Districts)

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

FOREIGN OBJECTS IN BIRD NESTS

Toledo, Ohio. The population was located within the city limits

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

EGG SIZE AND LAYING SEQUENCE

( 162 ) SOME BREEDING-HABITS OF THE LAPWING.

OBSERVATIONS OF HAWAIIAN

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus)

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Sat 5/22. Sun 5/23. Bodie District: Bodie Island: PIPLs have been observed this week. No breeding activity was observed.

Barn Swallow Nest Monitoring Methods

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

The Long-term Effect of Precipitation on the Breeding Success of Golden Eagles Aquila chrysaetos homeyeri in the Judean and Negev Deserts, Israel

Co-operative breeding by Long-tailed Tits

Is asynchronous hatching adaptive in herring gulls (Larus argentatus)?

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

FACTORS AFFECTING NESTING SUCCESS OF THE GLOSSY IBIS LYNNE M. MILLER 1 AND JOANNA BURGER

Causes of reduced clutch size in a tidal marsh endemic

Record of Predation by Sugar Glider on Breeding Eastern Rosellas 33Km NE of Melbourne in November 2016

EFFECT OF PREY ON PREDATOR: VOLES AND HARRIERS

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1995) provides a comprehensive account.

Sun 6/13. Sat 6/12. South Beach: A two-egg nest from Pair 12 was discovered on 6/15. One lone male continues to be observed.

Rapid City, South Dakota Waterfowl Management Plan March 25, 2009

Western Snowy Plover Recovery and Habitat Restoration at Eden Landing Ecological Reserve

Bald Eagles in the Yukon. Wildlife in our backyard

The Oysterbed Site Image Log

NARWHALS. The decrease of the Monodon monoceros population. By Caitlin Seppi

HATCHING ASYNCHRONY, BROOD REDUCTION, AND FOOD LIMITATION IN A NEOTROPICAL PARROT

(261) THE INCUBATION PERIOD OF THE OYSTER-CATCHER

Wilson Bull., 94(2), 1982, pp

Husbandry Guidelines Name Species Prepared by

WOOD STORKS (MYCTERIA AMERICANA) IN EAST-CENTRAL GEORGIA

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

In collaboration with the NJ Division of Fish & Wildlife s Endangered and Nongame Species Program

Wilson Bull., 98(2), 1986, pp

OBSERVATIONS ON SWALLOWS AND HOUSE- MARTINS AT THE NEST. BY

by L. W. Oliphant and W. J.P. Thompson c/o Department of Veterinary Anatomy University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon, Saskatchewan S7N OWO

C OLONIAL sea birds are characteristically long lived once they have fledged,

BREEDING BIOLOGY OF AMERICAN CROWS

Great Horned Owl (Bubo virginianus) Productivity and Home Range Characteristics in a Shortgrass Prairie. Rosemary A. Frank and R.

ANALYSIS OF GROWTH OF THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Chapman (1999) provides a comprehensive account.

Polar Biol (1994) 14: Springer-Verlag 1994

PREDATION ON RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD EGGS AND NESTLINGS

Growth and Development of the Black-eared Kite Milvus migrans lineatus

Vancouver Bald Eagle Report 2013

2015 Loon Survey - Madge Lake, Duck Mt. Provincial Park!

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings

How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation?

THE BREEDING BIOLOGY OF THE NORTHWESTERN CROW

NQF Level: 1 US No:

6 Month Progress Report. Cape vulture captive breeding and release programme Magaliesberg Mountains, South Africa. VulPro NPO

Productivity of African Black Oystercatchers Haematopus moquini on Robben Island, South Africa, in the breeding season

The hen harrier in England

Anas clypeata (Northern Shoveler)

ASPECTS OF THE BREEDING BIOLOGY AND PRODUCTIVITY OF BACHMAN S SPARROW IN CENTRAL ARKANSAS

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Caprimulgiformes Family: Podargidae Scientific Name: Podargus strigoides Common Name: Tawny frogmouth

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Thefirst attempt at Brood Manipulation of the Golden Eagle Aquila chrysaetos in Japan

ACTIVITY PATTERNS AND HOME-RANGE USE OF NESTING LONG-EARED OWLS

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

OBSERVATIONS OF PEMBROKE PINES BALD EAGLE NEST - FWC ID# BO-002

Trunk Contents. Crane Flight Feathers (3)

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Reduced availability of refuse and breeding output in a herring gull (Larus argentatus) colony

AS91603 Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants & animals to their external environment

Summary of 2017 Field Season

AVIAN HAVEN Wild Bird Rehabilitation Center

The story of Solo the Turnbull National Wildlife Refuge Male Swan

Siblicide at Northern Goshawk Nests: Does Food Play a Role?

Conflict-Related Aggression

Great Blue Heron Chick Development. Through the Stages

Management of bold wolves

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1996) provides a comprehensive account.

ABSTRACT. (Grus canadensis tabida) that is currently listed as endangered by the Ohio Division of

3. Chicks weigh 86 grams when they hatch and gain 100 grams a day until they are about 50 days old when they are ready to take care of itself.

Pair bond and breeding success in Blue Tits Parus caeruleus and Great Tits Parus major

SIBLICIDE IN SWALLOW-TAILED KITES

Transcription:

CHICK GROWTH, SIBLING RIVALRY, AND CHICK PRODUCTION IN AMERICAN BLACK OYSTERCATCHERS SARAH GROVES Department of Zoology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1W5, Canada ABSTRACT.--I studied chick growth, sibling rivalry, and chick production in a population of American Black Oystercatchers (Haematopus bachmani) on Cleland Island, British Columbia from 1975 to 1978. For these birds, survival from hatching until time of first flight is the critical life-history stage. Chick growth varied with brood size, and heavier chicks had higher survival to the time of first flight. Once chicks could fly, however, their survival to 1 and 2 yr of age was not related to brood size or weight at time of first flight. Dominance hierarchies based on weight existed in two-chick broods, and dominant sibs could control access to parental feedings through sibling rivalry. Sibling rivalry probably increases the likelihood of some chick production in years when food is difficult to obtain. Received 15 March 1983, accepted 12 January 1984. GROWTH and survival of young animals are affected by how parental investment is partitioned among offspring (Smith and Fretwell 1974). In most animals, parental investment is limited to the partitioning of energy among eggs. In animals that also feed dependent young, however, the allocation of parental care among individual offspring affects offspring growth and survival (Brockelman 1975). In principle, when offspring are dependent on parental feeding, it should be possible to relate parental foraging performance and chick feeding to chick production, but in practice this is a difficult task. In this study the relationship between parental foraging performance and chick production in American Black Oystercatchers (Haernatopus bachmani) was examined indirectly by studying: (1) the average chick production from one and two-egg clutches; (2) the relationship between brood size and chick growth; (3) sibling rivalry and factors affecting the establishment and maintenance of weight differences in two-chick broods; and (4) the relationships between chick weight and chick survival. METHODS Study area.--cleland Island (49ø10'N, 126ø05'W), an Ecological Reserve near Torino, British Columbia, was the site of this study. Cleland is a low-lying, basalt Present address: 1935 West 15th Avenue, Vancouver, British Columbia v6j 2L2, Canada. reef about 7.7 ha in area. The tides are mixed, mainly semi-diurnal, with a mean tidal range of 2.8 m. Low tide exposes an extensive intertidal zone inhabited by a diverse community of marine invertebrates (Campbell and Stirling 1968). The climate is mild and wet, with an average annual temperature of 9øC and average annual precipitation of 309 cm recorded for Torino (Climate of British Columbia 1976). Major storms sometimes occur during the summer, resulting in local flooding on the island. Study animal--each year about 35-40 pairs of American Black Oystercatchers breed on Cleland. Nonbreeding oystercatchers and failed breeders (more than 100 birds on some occasions) use the island as a high-tide roost. Birds lay 1-3 eggs in nest cups in basalt crevices lined with bits of broken shell or in scrapes made on beaches of broken shell. Egg laying occurs from mid-may until mid-july. Incubation begins when the clutch is complete and lasts about 26 days. Eggs hatch within a few hours of each other. Chicks hatched first are brooded while the rest of the clutch hatches. American Black Oystercatcher chicks are precocial but dependent on parental feeding for an extended period (Groves 1982). Parent oystercatchers feed their chicks during low-tide periods. Very young chicks remain near the nest, and parents take turns guarding the chicks and carrying food, item by item, from the intertidal area to the chicks. As they become bigger and more agile, chicks follow their parents into the intertidal zone to be fed. Around the time they begin to fly (35+ days), chicks begin to capture small prey items for themselves, but the most profitable prey items are still provided by their parents (Groves 1982). Chick growth, survival, and feeding by parents.--de- 525 The Auk 101: 525-531. July 1984

526 SARAH GROVES [Auk, Vol. 101 TABLE 1. Clutch size of American Black Oystercatchers over 6 yr, Cleland Island. Number of clutches Total Year 1 egg 2 eggs 3 eggs clutches Mean clutch size SD 1970' 14 38 9 61 1.92 0.61 1971 a 8 26 14 48 2.13 0.67 1972 a 11 34 13 58 2.03 0.65 1976 11 25 18 54 2.13 0.73 1977 12 38 10 60 1.97 0.61 1978 b 5 15 9 29 2.14 0.69 Total 61 176 73 310 2.04 0.66 Data from Hartwick (1974). Data based on five visits to the island. tails of nest histories, chick growth, and survival were recorded in 1976 and 1977. Nests were located as clutches were commenced and checked every 3 days to obtain nest histories. After eggs pipped, nests were checked daily to determine the date of hatching. A unique combination of colored plastic leg bands and numbered metal Wildlife Service bands was put on each chick to ensure positive identification. Chick weights and exposed culmen lengths were measured every third day (as weather permitted) until chicks flew or disappeared. All weighings were done within 1.5 h of high tide, when most intertidal feeding areas had been submerged for several hours. This minimized the contribution of recently filled crops to chick weights. Analysis of variance was used to analyze chick weights according to brood-size for 3-day intervals of age. Because not all chicks were weighed during each 3-day interval, each set of chick weights for a 3-day interval of age was treated as a separate sample. No attempt was made to compare growth rates. Records of parents feeding chicks were made in 1977. Observations were made with a 20 x spotting scope during low~tide periods. When a chick was fed, prey type and size, chick identification, parent identiffcation, and time of feeding were recorded. Interactions between siblings were also recorded. The data presented here focus on four oystercatcher families with two-chick broods. They were observed for 43.2 h of active feeding during 14 observation periods. The chicks observed ranged in age from 14 to 53 days (day of hatching is day 0). RESULTS Clutch size and chick production.--the clutch size of American Black Oystercatchers ranges from 1 to 3 eggs. Data on 310 clutches gathered in three seasons of this study and three seasons of Hartwick's (1974) study are presented in Table 1. The mean size of all 310 clutches was 2.04 eggs (SD = 0.66). Of the 114 clutches corn- pleted in 1976 and 1977, only 23% (26 clutches) survived to hatch chicks, and just 12% (14 clutches) produced flying young. In 1976 and 1977 1-egg and 2-egg clutches did not consistently produce different numbers of chicks per clutch (Table 2), but these results are tentative because of small sample sizes. The number of chicks surviving to fly did not differ significantly between clutch sizes (X 2 = 0.57, df = 1, NS). An average of 0.50 (_+0.53) chicks per clutch survived to fly from 1-egg clutches, whereas 0.75 (_+0.77) chicks per clutch survived to fly from 2-egg clutches. (No comparable data on 3-egg clutches exist because no 3-egg clutches hatched 3 chicks during 1976 and 1977.) Chick growth and sibling rivalry.--the growth of American Black Oystercatcher chicks from hatching to time of first flight varied with brood size and a chick's rank (based on weight) in the brood (Fig. 1). Weights of chicks in 1-chick and 2-chick broods did not differ significantly until chicks were 14-16 days old. At that age, average weights of chicks in 1-chick broods were greater than weights of chicks in 2-chick broods (F = 8.89, df = 1,22, P < 0.005), and these dif- TABLE 2. Chick production by American Black Oystercatchers from 1-egg and 2-egg clutches in 1976 and 1977. Number Clutch size of 1976 1977 clutches fledging 1 egg 2 eggs 1 egg 2 eggs 0 chicks 1 4 4 3 1 chick 1 1 4 5 2 chicks -- 2 -- 1

July 1984] Oystercatcher Chick Growth and Production 527 500' 20 I I I I I I I I 5-9 10-12 14-16 17-19 20-22 23-25 26-29 34-37 AGE (DAYS) Fig. 1. Average chick weights (log scale) are plotted against chick age in days. Data are plotted according to brood size and chick rank in brood (for 2-chick broods). x = 1-chick brood; ß = big sib in 2-chick brood; + = little sib in 2-chick brood. (Vertical bars are 95% confidence limits.) ferences persisted until chicks could fly. Average weights of siblings in two-chick broods did not differ statistically, but in some broods, the larger chick weighed up to 48% more when chicks began to fly. Weight differences between siblings in twochick broods were possibly affected by dominance relations between sibs and the resulting unequal distribution of food. As a result of chasing and dominance, big sibs could control access to parental feedings. Big sibs initiated 36 of 38 overt chases observed (X 2 = 30.42, df = 1, P < 0.005). During observation sessions when chasing was observed, little sibs were fed much less often than big sibs (Fig. 2, X 2 = 85.78, df = 1, P < 0.001). During sessions when chasing did not occur, however, little sibs were fed more often than big sibs (X 2= 40.03, df = 1, P < 0.005). On one occasion, I observed the establishment or reinforcement of dominance between 15 day-old sibs weighing 158 g and 128 g. No conflict between these sibs had been previously observed. Both chicks reached a parent returning with food at the same time. The big sib turned, pecked the little sib on the head and neck, and then chased the little sib for several meters. Afterwards the big sib returned to the parent and took the food. Subsequently, the little sib avoided the big sib or parent with food whenever the big sib exhibited agonistic behavior (overt or merely an approach). In this and other two-chick broods, dominance relations persisted after the chicks were flying and still being fed by their parents (pers. obs.). Chick survival.--heavier oystercatcher chicks had a better chance than lighter chicks of surviving to fly. Often the cause of chick mortality could not be determined, but important sources of mortality included storms, crow predation on eggs, and gull predation on chicks. Chicks that survived to fly were heavier at 20 days of

528 SARAH GI OVES [Auk, Vol. 101 z 240 u: 160' uj Li_ LI_ UJ - 80' ) 0 FEEDINGS Wl THOUT CHASES FEEDINGS WITH CHASES Fig. 2. Frequency of parental feedings and sibling chases in 2-chick broods. Hatched bars are big siblings; open bars are little siblings. age than chicks that did not fly (Fig. 3, t = 2.36, df = 21, P < 0.05). All chicks heavier than 200 g at 20 days of age (n = 15) eventually flew except one that was probably eaten by a river otter (Lutra canadensis) (pers. obs.). Of 10 chicks weighing 200 g or less at 20 days of age only 5 survived to fly (P = 0.035, Fisher's Exact Test). Chick survival from hatching to time of first flight varied with brood size and rank in brood (Fig. 4). Mortality was highest during the first week after hatching. In one-chick broods all of the mortality occurred during this period. In two-chick broods mortality occurred until chicks began flying. Post-fiedging survivai.--there was no evidence that survivorship or weights of fledged birds from one- and two-chick broods differed. Similar proportions of color-banded American Black Oystercatchers that had fledged from 1- chick and 2-chick broods in 1976 and 1977 were resighted as 1 and 2 yr olds in the high-tide roost area by September 1978 (1-chick broods = 7 chicks fledged, 3 resighted; 2-chick broods = 10 chicks fledged, 6 resighted; P = 0.30, Fisher's Exact Test). Weights at the time of first flight did not differ between birds resighted 1 or 2 L 15%175 75-200 20 225 225-250 25 275 275-300 300-325 GRAMS Fig. 3. Chick survival to fly according to weight at 20 days of age. Hatched bars are chicks that survived to fly; open bars are chicks that did not fly.

July 1984] Oystercatcher Chick Growth and Production 529 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 I I I I I I I 0 7 14 21 2e, 35 42 49 ACo E (DAYS) Fig. 4. Chick survivorship, 1976 and 1977 combined. Chick survival from hatching is plotted against chick age in days. x = 1-chick broods; ß = big sibs in 2-chick broods; + = little sibs in 2-chick broods. yr later and birds not resighted (resighted: œ = 351 g, SD = 45.1, n = 9; not resighted: œ = 350 g, SD = 68.7, n = 8; t = 0.32, df = 15). DISCUSSION Chick growth and sibling rivalry.--variations in American Black Oystercatcher chick growth with brood size (Fig. l) suggesthat the ability of parents to provide food limits chick growth. Siblings of different sizes represent different amounts of parental investment (Trivets 1972), and size differences of siblings may facilitate rapid responses to sudden changes in food availability through brood reduction (Ricklefs 1965). Both sibling rivalry (O'Connor 1978) and parental behavior can maintain or increase differences in sizes of siblings. Two-chick broods are more demanding of parents' time and energy than 1-chick broods, and differences in chick weights related to brood size were established by the time chicks were 2 weeks old. Chick-growth patterns suggest that increased parental investment and in- creasing brood size are not proportional. A less than proportional increase in parental investment with increasing brood size has been reported in numerous other bird species (for summary, see Klomp 1970), and Norton-Griffiths (1969) has presented an hypothesis that accounts for this phenomenon. He hypothesized that parental feeding of Common Oystercatcher chicks (H. ostralegus) is motivated by parental hunger and that chick feeding will occur primarily during periods when parents are motivated to feed themselves. Sibling rivalry also appears to affect chick growth and survival. Safriel (1981) reported a similar situation in Common Oystercatchers in Europe. In both this study and Safriel's, prey availability to chicks was controlled by parental delivery of prey, and chicks, especially large chicks, appeared to be rarely satiated. Hunger and the sight of a parent carrying food are likely stimuli for the occurrence of sibling rivalry. Hunger alone did not stimulate dominance behavior in American Black Oystercatcher chicks in this study, as siblings that expressed

530 s^r^ Gl oves [Auk, Vol. 101 well-established dominance relations in the Once established, dominance relations between siblings in this study persisted, and heavier siblings were dominant. Other studies (Ricklefs and Hainsworth 1967, Meyburg 1978) suggesthat dominance is quickly and irreversibly decided in one fight. Dominance usually follows weight or age differences attributed to asynchronous hatching (e.g. Bryant 1978, Ingram 1962, Meyburg 1973, Miller 1973, Procter 1975, Safriel 1981), but exceptions have been reported (Ricklefs and Hainsworth 1967, Meyburg 1978). Chick production and chick survival--chick production by American Black Oystercatchers was low and varied between years. Some pairs did not produce any flying chicks in four seasons (Groves 1982). Predation and unpredictable bouts of severe weather, factors unrelated to parental foraging performance, constrained chick production. Breeding success and chick production for Cleland Island American Black Oystercatchers were much lower than Harris (1967) reported for Common Oystercatchers on Skokholm Island, Wales. There, 37% and 59% of eggs produced flying young in two consecutive years. On Cleland Island, Hartwick (1974) reported that 13%, 9%, and 13% of eggs produced flying young in 3 yr (1970-1972), and during this study only 5% of eggs produced flying young in 1976 and 9% in 1977. During this study, the number of chicks produced from 1- and 2-chick broods did not differ significantly (Table 2). Lower chick growth and the occurrence of sibling rivalry in 2-chick broods suggest that these chicks were not as well provisioned by their parents as chicks in 1-chick broods. In only one brood in this study did these factors apparently contribute to star- presence of a parent with food would spend hours crouched side-by-side in hiding places between feedings. The degree of food limitation, however, may affect dominance behavior. Ingram (1959) found that cannibalism among nestling Short-eared Owls (Asio fiammeus) was most frequent in years of extreme food shortage, when parents were unable to provision their nest by caching food within reach of bigger chicks. Similarly Procter (1975) demonvation of a chick. In seasons of unusually stormy weather, however, weight differences maintained by sibling rivalry could affect chick production. Fledging success varied with brood size and chick weight. Higher rates of chick loss in twochick broods may be due to the behavior of parent oystercatchers. Chicks are frequently brooded by their parents during the first week strated that the nutritional condition of South after hatching, and predators are able to locate Polar Skua chicks (Catharacta maccormicki) (measured as percentage of standard weight at a particular age) was positively correlated with the occurrence and intensity of aggression between sibs in naturally and artificially created chicks by observing the behavior of parents. As chicks grow older and are brooded less, predators are no longer able to locate chicks directly by observing their parents (Y. Yom-Tov pers. comm.). Instead, predators must search for two-chick broods. cryptically colored chicks. A predator probably has a better chance of finding a chick on a territory with a 2-chick brood than on a territory with a 1-chick brood. Furthermore, parent oystercatchers may be able to defend 1 chick more effectively than 2, especially as both parents are often needed to drive a gull away from a chick (pers. obs.). Chick behavior may also affect patterns of chick mortality. In experimental enclosures, oystercatcher chicks that lost fights over food often remained in open areas, begged, and ran around, while siblings that won fights and obtained food sat quietly under cover (Safriel 1981). The conspicuous behavior of subordinate, hungry chicks probably makes them more vulnerable to predation. Heavier chicks had a better chance of surviv- ing to fly (Fig. 3). There were no differences in weight at the time of first flight between birds that were and were not resighted as 1 and 2-yr olds, nor were there any differences related to brood size in the proportion of birds resighted 1 and 2 yr later. This suggests that the critical life-history period for American Black Oystercatchers is survival from hatching to first flight. Once a bird can fly, its prospects for survival are apparently quite good, although no data are available on eventual recruitment of flying young to the breeding population. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I am grateful to many friends who assisted in the field, especially to L. Dick, R. Jaremovic, L. Paull, M. J. Taitt, and J. N.M. Smith. J. B. Foster, coordinator of the British Columbia Ecological Reserves, made it possible to work on Cleland Island. Computer sup-

July 1984] Oystercatcher Chick Growth and Production 531 port at the University of British Columbia was made available by J. N.M. Smith, who also read and commented on the manuscript. M. Weston advised on statistics. C. Marsh and C. C. Smith reviewed an earlier version of the manuscript and provided helpful suggestions for revisions. LITERATURE CITED BROCKELMAN, W.Y. 1975. Competition, the fitness of offspring, and optimal clutch size. Amer. Natur. 109: 677-699. BRYANT, D.M. 1978. Establishment of weight hierarchies in the broods of house martins Delichon urbica. Ibis 120: 16-26. CAMPBELL, R. W., & D. STIRLING. 1968. Notes on the natural history of Cleland Island, British Columbia, with emphasis on the breeding bird fauna. Victoria, Brit. Columbia, Brit. Columbia Prov. Mus. Rept. for 1967. CLIMATE OF BRITISI I COLUMBIA, 1976. Tables of temperature, precipitation, and sunshine. Victoria, Brit. Columbia, Brit. Columbia Ministry Agric. GROVES, $. 1982. Aspects of foraging in black oystercatchers (Aves: Haematopodidae). Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. Vancouver, Brit. Columbia, Univ. Brit. Columbia. HARRIS, M.P. 1967. The biology of oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus on Skokholm Island, S. Wales. Ibis 109: 180-193. HARTWICK, E.B. 1974. Breeding ecology of the black oystercatcher (Haematopus bachmani Audubon). Syesis 7: 83-92. INGRAM, C. 1959. The importance of juvenile cannibalism in the breeding biology of certain birds of prey. Auk 76: 218-226. 1962. Cannibalism by nestling Short-eared Owls. Auk 79: 715. KLOMe, H. 1970. The determination of clutch-size in birds: a review. Ardea 58: 1-151. MEYBURG, B. 1973. Sibling aggression and mortality among nestling eagles. Ibis 116: 224-228. --. 1978. Sibling aggression and cross-fostering of eagles. Pp. 195-200 in Endangered birds: management techniques for preserving threatened species (S. A. Temple, Ed.). Madison, Wisconsin, Univ. Wisconsin Press. MILLER, R. $. 1973. The brood size of cranes. Wilson Bull. 85: 436-441. NORTON-GRIFFITHS, M. 1969. The organisation, control, and development of parental feeding in the oystercatcher (Haematopus ostralegus). Behaviour 34: 55-112. O'CONNOR, R.J. 1978. Brood reduction in birds: selection for fratricide, infanticide, and suicide. Anim. Behav. 26: 79-96. PROCTER, D. L.C. 1975. The problem of chick loss in the south polar skua Catharacta maccormicki. Ibis 117: 452-459. RICKLEES, R.E. 1965. Brood reduction in the Curvebilled Thrasher. Condor 67: 505-510. --, & F. R. HAINSWORTH. 1967. The temporary establishment of dominance between two handraised juvenile Cactus Wrens (Campylorhynchus brunneicapillus). Condor 69: 528. $AFRIEL, U. N. 1981. Social hierarchy among siblings in broods of the Oystercatcher (Haematopus ostralegus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 9: 59-63. SMITH, C. C., &: $. D. FRETWELL. 1974. The optimal balance between size and number of offspring. Amer. Natur. 108: 499-506. TRIVERS, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Pp. 139-179 in Sexual selection and the descent of man (B. Campbell, Ed.). Chicago, A1- dine.