Visual Acuity of the Midland Banded Water Snake Estimated from Evoked Telencephalic Potentials

Similar documents
Wagner, 1980; Schuurmans, 1981). Recently several studies have concluded that the

Squamates of Connecticut

Distribution Unlimited

UC Santa Barbara UC Santa Barbara Previously Published Works

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

Development of Neuronal Response Properties in the Cat Dorsal Lateral Geniculate Nucleus During Monocular

THE VISUAL MECHANISMS OF TENEBRIO MOLITOR: VARIATIONS TAKING PLACE IN THE ERG OF PUPA AND ADULT DURING DEVELOPMENT

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified):

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar

deprived eye (reverse occlusion). beyond 1 year of age; only two of six animals recovered sufficient vision to enable

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Elk (Cervus canadensis) Extra credit assignment. Sad Underwing (Catocala maestosa) 10/11/2017

How the eye sees. Properties of light. The light-gathering parts of the eye. 1. Properties of light. 2. The anatomy of the eye. 3.

Effects of Early Monocular Lid Suture on Spatial and Temporal Sensitivity of Neurons in Dorsal Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of the Cat

Variations of blood flow at optic nerve head induced by sinusoidal flicker stimulation in cats

Aging Dogs Manifest Myopia as Measured by Autorefractor

Your Eye, My Eye, and the Eye of the Aye Aye: Evolution of Human Vision from 65 Million Years Ago to the Present

Although owls can t move their eyes, many other adaptations help these raptors spot prey.

Spatial and Temporal Sensitivity of Normal and Amblyopic Cats

Title: Phylogenetic Methods and Vertebrate Phylogeny

UTILITY OF THE NEUROLOGICAL EXAMINATION IN RATS

Veterinary Ophthalmology

a retinal gross potential, the electroretinogram (ERG), recorded the radiance of a flickering monochromatic test light is

PATTERN EVOKED RESPONSE DEFICIENCY IN PATTERN DEPRIVED CATS 1

Squamates of Connecticut. May 11th 2017

Binocular Interactions in Striate Cortical Neurons of Cats Reared with Discordant Visual Inputs

Pet-Temp PT-300 Ear Thermometer Frequently Asked Questions

Multi-Frequency Study of the B3 VLA Sample. I GHz Data

CLARSBISHOP AREA IN THE CAT: LOCATION AIVD RETINOTOPICAL PROJECTION

Carphophis amoenus Family Colubridae Subfamily Xenodontidae

Area Centralis Position Relative to the Optic Disc Projection in Kittens as o Function of Age

Grade Level: 1-2. Next Generation Sunshine State Standards SC.1.L.14.1; SC.1.L.17.1; SC.1.N.1.1 SC.2.L.17.1; SC.2.L.17.2; SC.2.N.1.

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

Mistaken identity? Visual similarities of marine debris to natural prey items of sea turtles

GUIDELINE 1: MICROCHIP TECHNOLOGY FOR RADIO FREQUENCY IDENTIFICATION OF ANIMALS

It Is Raining Cats. Margaret Kwok St #: Biology 438

Bio4009 : Projet de recherche/research project

Invertebrates. Brain. Brain 12/2/2017. The Invertebrate Brain. The Invertebrate Brain. Invertebrate brain general layouts some specific functions

DLS Sample Preparation Guide

THE PRETRIGEMINAL CAT AS AN INSTRUMENT FOR INVESTIGATION OF THE OCULAR FIXATION REFLEX

preferring rightward movement. A changeover later than 5 weeks of age peak of the critical period for directional deprivation may occur earlier

Reductions in Taurine Secondary to Photoreceptor Loss in Irish Setters with Rod-Cone Dysplasia

Biology. Slide 1of 50. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Seafinding revisited: how hatchling marine turtles respond to natural lighting at a nesting beach

This article is downloaded from.

Time of Day. Teacher Lesson Plan Nocturnal Animals Pre-Visit Lesson. Overview

Anatomy. Name Section. The Vertebrate Skeleton

Experimental analysis of amblyopia

The Critical Period for Ocular Dominance Plasticity in the Ferret s Visual Cortex

MGL Avionics EFIS G2 and iefis. Guide to using the MGL RDAC CAN interface with the UL Power engines

Rules of Connectivity between Geniculate Cells and Simple Cells in Cat Primary Visual Cortex

Properties of the flash visual evoked potential recorded in the cat primary visual cortex

A case of achromatopsia. Perceptual Colour Space. Spectral Properties of Light. Subtractive Colour Mixture. Additive Colour Mixture

Perception & Attention Course. George Mather

Species Results From Database Search

Effects of Retinal Image Degradation on Ocular Growth in Cats

Teaching Workshop: Color Vision in Primates and Other Mammals

RETINITIS PIGMENTOSA*

Differences in Visual Signal Design and Detectability between Allopatric Populations of Anolis Lizards

Proceeding of the SEVC Southern European Veterinary Conference

Differential Effects of Early Monocular Deprivation on Binocular and Monocular Segments of Cat Striate Cortex

SECTION 3 IDENTIFYING ONTARIO S EASTERN MASSASAUGA RATTLESNAKE AND ITS LOOK-ALIKES

Lens luxation when the lens gets wobbly

Grade Level: 3-5. Next Generation Sunshine State Standards SC.3.L.15.1 SC.4.L.16.2; SC.4.L.17.4 SC.5.L.15.1; SC.5.L.17.1

Supporting Online Material for

Neuroscience Letters

Color Vision by Prof/Faten zakareia King Saud University Physiology Dept

NAME: DATE: SECTION:

Reptilian Physiology

Consequences of alternating monocular deprivation on eye alignment and convergence in cats. Randolph Blake, M. L. ]. Crawford, and Helmut V. B.

from an experimental bag net SHIODE, DAISUKE; TAKAHASHI, MUTSUKI Proceedings of the 6th Internationa SEASTAR2000 workshop) (2011): 31-34

(Received 22 November 1984) studies were made on twenty such pairs; eight X on-centre, seven Y on-centre, two

Cardiac MRI Morphology 2004

lowering of the visual acuity. When closure was extended through the first by varying the age at eye closure. Waiting until 1 month of age

Field Herpetology Final Guide

Biology Slide 1 of 50

Do the traits of organisms provide evidence for evolution?

Stuart S. Sumida Biology 342. Simplified Phylogeny of Squamate Reptiles

Rodent Husbandry and Care 201 Cynthia J. Brown and Thomas M. Donnelly

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

Who Really Owns the Beach? The Competition Between Sea Turtles and the Coast Renee C. Cohen

Grooming the Kerry Blue Terrier

IN-WATER SEA TURTLE DISTRIBUTION AND ABUNDANCE MONITORING ON PALM BEACH COUNTY NEARSHORE REEFS FOR:

Overall structure is similar to humans, but again there are differences. Some features that are unique to mammals: Found in eutherian mammals.

muscles (enhancing biting strength). Possible states: none, one, or two.

Representation, Visualization and Querying of Sea Turtle Migrations Using the MLPQ Constraint Database System

ENGINEERING TEST SPECIFICATION

Gulf and Caribbean Research

Comparative Physiology 2007 Second Midterm Exam. 1) 8 pts. 2) 14 pts. 3) 12 pts. 4) 17 pts. 5) 10 pts. 6) 8 pts. 7) 12 pts. 8) 10 pts. 9) 9 pts.

abnormal lateral geniculate body. His anatomical study suggested that chiasm instead of remaining uncrossed. They thus reach the wrong hemispheres,

d. Wrist bones. Pacific salmon life cycle. Atlantic salmon (different genus) can spawn more than once.

DALE RITTER Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Box G, Walter Hall, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA. Accepted 27 June 1995

REPTILES. Scientific Classification of Reptiles To creep. Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Chordata Subphylum: Vertebrata Class: Reptilia

Y Use of adaptive management to mitigate risk of predation for woodland caribou in north-central British Columbia

Reptiles Notes. Compiled by the Davidson College Herpetology Laboratory

Visual and Instrumental Evaluation of Mottling and Striping

Characteristics of a Reptile. Vertebrate animals Lungs Scaly skin Amniotic egg

ELMED. Pediatric Laparoscopy Catalog WE DESIGN, MANUFACTURE & SELL THE TOOLS THE SURGEONS USE

Optoacoustic imaging of an animal model of prostate cancer

1. Examine the specimens of sponges on the lab table. Which of these are true sponges? Explain your answers.

spider monkeys by recording extracellularly from single units and stimulating

Transcription:

Revised ms. to Journal of Comparative Physiology A Visual Acuity of the Midland Banded Water Snake Estimated from Evoked Telencephalic Potentials By Robert A. Baker 1 Timothy J. Gawne 2 Michael S. Loop 2 And Sheena Pullman 3 2 Department of Vision Sciences School of Optometry University of Alabama at Birmingham Birmingham, AL 35294-4390 USA 205/934-6751 (phone) 205/934-5725 (fax) e-mail: loop@uab.edu 1 Animal Resources Program, University of Alabama at Birmingham 3 Oakwood College, Huntsville, AL 35896, USA Running title: snake visual acuity 1

Abstract The visual acuity of seven midland banded water snakes was measured by recording evoked responses from telencephalon to temporally modulated square wave grating patterns. Using conventional electrophysiological techniques and signal averaging, high contrast square wave gratings of different spatial frequencies were presented. Acuity was estimated by extrapolating relative response amplitude / log 10 spatial frequency functions which yielded an average acuity of 4.25 cycles/deg. Refractive state was also estimated by recording evoked potentials to intermediate spatial frequencies with different lenses in front of the eye. Polynomial fits indicated that under the experimental conditions the snakes were around 6.4 diopters hyperopic suggesting a corrected acuity of 4.89 cycles/deg. Reduction of grating luminance resulted in a reduction in evoked potential acuity measurements. These results indicate that the spatial resolution of midland banded water snakes is the equal of cat; about 20/120 in human clinical terms. Key words: snake, vision, acuity, evoked potential 2

Introduction Snakes are commonly said to see poorly yet we know of few systematic studies of their vision (Jacobs, et al, 1992; but see Sillman, et al., 1997). Thought to have evolved from lizards under the selective pressures of a burrowing life style, snakes subsequently emerged from this fossorial existence to occupy virtually every major ecological niche (Bellairs and Underwood, 1951; Apesteguia and Zaher, 2006). Several differences between the visual systems of snakes and lizards are thought to reflect this period of subterranean living followed by a return to the surface and a renewed selective advantage for sight. Walls (1942), for example, notes that while lizard retinas are dominated by cone photoreceptors within snakes a wide range of rod/cone ratios can be found. Significant differences in the primary visual system have also been noted between snakes and lizards (Repérant, et al., 1992). The dorsal geniculate complex is more developed in snakes while the superficial layers of the optic tectum are regarded as impoverished. Considering the unique evolutionary history of snakes, and its conjectured manifestations in visual system structure, interesting structure/function questions suggest themselves. For example, do similar rod/cone photoreceptor populations in species of snakes and lizards result in similar sensitivity under different levels of illumination? Do the impoverished superficial layers of optic tectum and hypertrophied dorsal geniculate complex in snakes produce any visual disabilities, or abilities, in comparison to lizard species with similar ecology? We chose to begin with an investigation of spatial vision by recording visually evoked potentials from the telencephalon of banded water snakes. In humans, cats and dogs, acuity estimates based upon evoked potentials from primary visual cortex to grating 3

stimuli have been shown to coincide quite closely with behavior measures of acuity (Campbell and Maffei, 1970; Berkley and Watkins, 1973; Murphy, et al., 1997). In addition, visually evoked potentials have also been used to estimate the acuity of two turtle species. The freshwater red-eared slider (Pseudemys scripta elegans) had an average visual acuity of 6.1 cycles/deg (Northmore and Granda, 1991) and the loggerhead sea turtle (Caretta caretta) an average acuity of 5.6cycles/deg (Bartol, et al., 2001). Dudziak (1955, in Northmore and Granda) reported behavioral acuity as high as 10cycles/deg in the freshwater turtle Emys orbicularis which Northmore and Granda (1991) noted corresponded to the intercone spacing in red-eared slider and the upper range of their evoked potential acuity estimates. We selected water snakes for study because they are widely distributed, use vision to both feed (Drummond, 1985) and flee, their refractive state has been studied (Schaeffel, 1991), they are active both day and night and are common in local streams. Method Subjects Midland banded water snakes (Nerodia sipedon pleuralis), collected in Birmingham, AL from Shades Creek between the Elder St. and Groover Dr. bridges (weights 23 to 212 grams), were studied. The snakes were maintained around 22 C on a 12hr light cycle in 38 liter aquaria with free access to drinking water and monthly feedings of fish. No experiments were conducted on snakes during their shed cycle. 4

Visual stimulation and signal processing The visual stimuli were black/white vertical square wave gratings presented on a 43 by 30 cm KDS video monitor located 40 cm from the snake so that the stimulus display occupied a visual angle of 56deg by 41deg. The mean luminance of the display was 34cd/m 2 and the Michelson contrast of the gratings was maintained at 0.96. The spatial frequency of the gratings was computer controlled and ranged from 0.038 to 5.09 cycles / deg. When no grating was present the screen was at 0.0 contrast and 34 cd/m 2. A stimulus epoch lasted 3sec and consisted of the grating appearing (time 0) followed by instantaneous one-half cycle displacement at 1sec and 2sec. Thus the first contrast modulation was off-on and the next two counter-phased. A stimulus epoch repeated every 0.5sec. Evoked potentials were generally recorded from over the snake s telencephalon and referenced to a subdermal site in the upper neck using Ag-AgCl electrodes ( E215P probe & E255-A wire, In Vivo Metric, Healdsburg, CA). Neural signals were amplified, low/high pass filtered at 0.3-300hz, displayed on a digital oscilloscope and averaged by a laboratory computer for subsequent analysis. Responses to around 20-30 stimulus epochs were averaged for each stimulus condition. Evoked potential amplitudes were measured from baseline to the peak of the response. Procedures Snakes were initially anesthetized in a 1.89 liter clear plastic container with 0.5cc of isoflurane. Once the righting reflex disappeared (around 12-15min) the snake was quickly 5

intubated with a 2in 18 gage IV catheter which was connected by a 1in length of tubing to a Versaflex breathing device (EZ -104, Euthanex Corp, Palmer, PA). Once stabilized with 5% isoflurane in 0.5 liter/min of O 2, isoflurane was reduced to 3% for evoked potential recording. Isoflurane has been used in conjunction with sensory stimulation for both mammals and reptiles (Hartikainen and Rorarius, 1999; Moon and Terashima, 2002; Goris, et al., 2003; but see Rojas, et al., 2006) Expired gases were vented to a charcoal filter canister (EZ-250). The snake was upon a wooden platform with its head positioned over a 3 x 3in hole up through which the anesthesia tube was double taped to its open lower jaw. A wooden craft stick, under its upper jaw, spanned the hole and a few pieces of medical tape positioned the snake on the platform. An electrode carrier from a rat stereotaxic was attached to the platform and located to the right of the snake s head. The platform itself was atop a 13in high plastic cylinder mounted upon a turntable such that the snake s orientation to the stimulus could be varied without changing the distance to the stimulus screen. A lens / filter holder was used to position a refraction lens, or neutral density filter, in front of the snake s left eye. Initial experiments recorded from the dura surface through a small hole in the skull. Subsequently we found that very satisfactory potentials could be recorded from the surface of the skull overlying telencephalon after simply removing a small flap of skin. During an experimental session an acuity experiment was conducted first by presenting stimulus epochs of different spatial frequencies ranging from 0.038 to 5.09 cycles / deg. The spatial frequency sequence of the stimuli was interleaved between epochs and average responses were based upon 20-30 epochs / spatial frequency. The 6

initial acuity experiment was often followed by a refraction experiment which entailed recording evoked potentials to an intermediate spatial frequency (0.66 or 1.27 cycles / deg) with different clinical trial lenses placed 1.0 cm in front of the snakes left eye and the snake oriented at 45 deg to the stimulus screen. Most recording sessions involved several acuity experiments. The most frequent acuity experiments were conducted with the snake oriented at 45 deg to the stimulus (n=8) as this angle was estimated to put the plane of the pupil parallel to the stimulus display. Four acuity experiments were conducted with the snake oriented directly at the stimulus screen so as to allow binocular stimulation. To investigate the effect of stimulus luminance upon acuity, three acuity experiments were conducted on one snake with neutral density filters (ND 1.8, 1.1, and 0.63) positioned in front of the snake s eye and the snake oriented at 45 deg to the stimulus screen. These luminance acuities were conducted following 10min of no stimulation with the ND 1.8 filter in front of the snake s eye. ND 1.8 testing was followed by testing with ND 1.1 and ND 0.63 filters. The snake was either oriented at rightward 45 deg (favoring left eye stimulation) or 0 deg to the stimulus screen and we usually recorded from over the right telencephalon (n= 9 experiments) but occasionally over the left telencephalon (n= 4 experiments). One acuity experiment was conducted recording over the left optic tectum with the snake oriented at 0 deg. Recording sites were just lateral of the midline and located as multiples of the distance between the snake s snot and middle of the pupil (range 8.5mm to 12mm); telencephalon: 1.5, optic tectum: 1.75. These locations agree well with Figure 1 of Northcutt and Butler (1974). 7

Results Acuity Figure 1 presents the averaged potentials comprising an acuity series Insert Figure 1 about here recorded from the right telencephalon with the snake oriented at 45 deg to the stimulus screen and a 2.0 diopter lens in front of the snake s eye. Typical of the potentials we recorded are a) decreasing amplitude with increasing spatial frequency, b) the first potential (a response to the gratings presentation from 0.0 contrast) is smaller than the next two (both responses to half cycle displacements of the grating) and c) there are generally two peaks in each response. All analysis was based upon the average of the second and third responses because both were to grating counter-phase and were always larger than the first response. Relative amplitude was derived by taking the largest potential in any experiment as 1.0. Figure 2 illustrates the relative evoked potential amplitude as a function of Insert Figure 2 about here grating spatial frequency for both the potentials illustrated in Figure 1 (using a 2.0 diopter lens - squares) and using a plano (0.0 diopter - diamonds) lens. The regression lines were extrapolated to zero on the spatial frequency axis and correspond to acuity estimates of 8

7.91 cycles/deg with the 2.0 diopter lens and 4.77 cycles/deg with the plano lens; the correlation coefficients (r 2 ) were 0.92 and 0.96 respectively. Most acuity experiments were conducted with no refractive correction because data were analyzed later, there was a relatively small effect of focus upon evoked potential amplitude and an appropriate refractive correction (see below) could be applied post hoc. The average of these acuity experiments, with a stimulus orientation of 45 deg, was 4.25±1.7sd cycles/deg with an average r 2 = 0.95±0.04sd (n=8). When a snake was oriented at 0 deg to the stimulus the average acuity was 7.89±7.8 cycles/deg with an average r 2 =0.94±0.05sd (n=4). The difference in snake orientation to the stimulus screen was not statistically significant (one-tailed, t = -1.23, df =10, p = 0.12). Acuities obtained from the left telencephalon (n=2) fell within the range of those obtained from the right telencephalon (n=9) as did the acuity obtained from optic tectum (n=1). Refraction Figure 3 presents the results of one refraction experiment and illustrates relative evoked Insert Figure 3 about here potential amplitude, for a 1.27 cycle/deg grating, as a function of lens focus. The data points are fit to a polynomial equation (y = -0.0037x 2 + 0.0788x + 0.5464; r 2 = 0.90) and indicate that a maximum amplitude evoked potential would be obtained with a 10.5 diopter lens in front of the snake s eye, i.e. the snake is 10.5 diopters hyperopic in the experimental situation. Four refraction experiments were conducted and the average best refraction was 5.75 diopters (average r 2 = 0.81±0.12sd) based upon the average of 9

the individual polynomial fits. Another approach was based upon a polynomial computed by averaging the terms of the individual polynomials (y = -0.0028x 2 + 0.0354x + 0.731) and indicated 6.5 diopters as the best lens. Effect of luminance upon acuity Figure 4 presents the effect of luminance upon acuity with the snake oriented at 45 Insert Figure 4 about here deg to the stimulus screen. The three lowest luminance (0.54, 2.7 and 8 cd/m 2 ) acuities were derived from individual relative evoked potential amplitude/spatial frequency functions (average r 2 = 0.97±0.03sd) conducted with one snake while the value for 34 cd/m 2 was taken from the average of all other acuities functions under these conditions (n = 8). As can be seen acuity was highest at 34 cd/m 2 and declines as mean luminance was reduced. The solid line is a least square regression while the dashed line simply connects the individual data points. Discussion Acuity Our most common acuity experiments were with the snake oriented at 45 deg to the stimulus screen, with no corrective lens in place, and yielded a mean acuity of 4.25 cycles/deg. While the differences between orientations of 45 deg and 0 deg were not 10

statistically significant, the two highest acuities observed were with 0 deg orientation: 18.8 cycles/deg from left telencephalon and 14.2 cycles/deg from left optic tectum. We are at a loss to explain the high variability in the 0 deg orientation acuity measurements. While it might be thought that a pronounced retinal specialization could produce this variability, because the visual display subtended 56deg by 41 deg, we think this unlikely. The mean acuity of 4.25 cycles/deg is without refraction. However the average polynomial fit of the four refraction experiments indicated a largest evoked potential response using a 6.4 diopter lens and a 15% smaller response without refraction. These estimates suggest a correction of 1.15 should be applied when no refraction was used bringing the mean acuity to 4.89 cycles/deg. In the one acuity experiment we used a corrective lens (2.0 diopters) and the acuity was improved from 4.77 to 7.91 cycles/deg (i.e. Figure 2). The refraction experiment from this session (i.e. Figure 3) suggested that a 2.0 diopter lens would increase uncorrected acuity from 4.77 to 6.07 cycles/deg. A water snake visual acuity of approximately 5 cycles/degree can be compared with the turtle acuities estimated from evoked potentials. Recording from optic tectum Northmore and Granda (1991) and Bartol et al (2001) reported average acuities of 6.12 and 5.6 cycles/deg respectively. While these reptile acuities do not match Old World primates (around 30 cycles/deg) they are, as Northmore and Granda noted..good acuity for a small eye.. and compare favorably to some well studied non-primate mammals such as rat at 1.0 cycles/deg (Prusky, et al., 2000) and cat at 6.5 to2.5 cycles/deg (Berkley and Watkins, 1973; Pasternak and Merigan, 1981). In human clinical terms the acuity of the midland banded water snake is approximately 20/120. 11

The acuity of the midland banded water snake is good for a small eye, and better than some mammals with larger eyes. However we suspect that some other more visual snakes with larger eyes may have even better acuities. In this regard racers (Coluber) and coachwhips (Masticophis) come to mind. Refraction Relevant to the study at hand are the refractive measurements of Sivak (1977). Measurements were made with a retinoscope, under both unanaesthetized and ether anaesthetized conditions, from black racer (Coluber constrictor), yellow rat snake (Elaphe quadravittata) and red rat snake (Elaphe guttata). Sivak s (1977) mean unanaesthetized refraction was 0.44 diopters while the mean anaesthetized refraction was 9.6 diopters. In the present study with anaesthetized (isoflurane) water snakes, the mean refraction was 5.75 to 6.5 diopters and was obtained from evoked potential measurements. The fair agreement between Sivak (1977) and our anaesthetized refractive state estimates is complicated by the fact that retinoscopy systematically yields excess hyperopic values for small eyes. This small eye effect in retinoscopy, with an eye diameter of about 4mm, should produce an error of about 10 diopters (Glickstein and Millodot, 1970; Schaeffel and De Queiroz, 1990). However Glickstein and Millodot (1970) also found no effect of anesthesia upon retinoscopy measurements so the smalleye retinoscopy effect should have also affected Sivak s unanaesthetized measurements. A somewhat clearer picture emerges from Northmore and Granda (1991) who directly compared evoked potential and retinoscopically determined refractions in turtle. 12

Retinoscopy indicated about 4.8 diopters of hyperopia, in keeping with the small-eye effect. The evoked potential estimates suggesting the turtles were, on average, free of refractive error. Using infrared retinoscopy and three species of European water snakes, Schaeffel (1991) found about 10.0 diopters of hyperopia and concluded, considering the effect of eye size, these snakes likely properly focused in air. The results of the study at hand indicate that, under isoflurane anesthesia with a viewing distance of 40cm, the snakes were around 6 diopters hyperopic. However, because the equivalent power of snake eyes is high, well over 150 diopters based upon the relevant measurements of Sivak (1977), small refractive errors should produce little effect upon spatial resolution. Effect of luminance upon acuity Figure 4 illustrates the effect of mean luminance upon acuity. This result is preliminary as the data for the three lowest luminance levels are based upon one snake while the data for 34cd/m 2 are based upon all other snakes (n=6). The solid line is a linear regression fit to the data points and indicates a decline in spatial resolution as luminance is reduced. The dashed line, on the other hand, simply connects the data points but may suggest a branched effect of luminance upon acuity which is generally attributed to rod and cone function in many species including humans (Shaer, 1937) and cats (Pasternak and Merigan, 1981). If this branched interpretation is correct, our acuity estimate of around 5 cycles/degree at 34 cd/m 2 is based upon solid cone function as the level section of such curves is typically taken as mesopic illumination of mixed rod and cone function. 13

Acknowledgements This work was supported by NIH Grant P30/EY03039 through the assistance of Mr. Jerry Millican. SP was an NSF SPIN (DBI-0453429) summer student. The experiments comply with the Principles of Animal Care (NIH publication No.86-23, revised 1985) and were approved by the UAB Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. We thank Adam Gordon for his shared insights regarding equivalent power of snake eyes. 14

References Apesteguia S, Hussam Z (2006) A Cretaceous terrestrial snake with robust hindlimbs and a sacrum. Nature, 440:1037-1040 Bartol SM, Musick JA, Ochs AL (2001) Visual acuity thresholds of juvenile loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta): an electrophysiological approach. J Comp Physiol A 187:953-960 Bellairs Ad A, Underwood G (1951) The origins of snakes Biol Rev 26:193-237 Berkley MA, Watkins DW (1973) Grating resolution and refraction in the cat estimated from evoked cerebral potentials. Vis Res 13:403-415 Campbell FW, Maffei L (1970) Electrophysiological evidence for the existence of orientation and size detectors in the human visual system. J Physiol 207:635-652 Drummond H (1985) The role of vision in the predatory behavior of natricine snakes. Anim Behav 33:206-215 Dudziak J (1955) Ostrość widzenia u zolwia blotnego (Emys orbicularis L.) przy patrzeniu w środowisku powietrznym i wodntm. Fol Biol 3:205-228 Glickstein M, Millodot M (1970) Retinoscopy and eye size. Science 168:605-606 15

Goris RC, Atobe Y, Nakano M, Hisajima T, Funakoshi K, Kadota (2003) The microvasculature of python pit organs: morphology and blood flow microkinetics. Microvasc Res 65:179-185 Hartikainen K, Rorarius MGK (1999) Cortical responses to auditory stimuli during isoflurane burst suppression anaesthesis. Anaesthesia 54:210-214 Jacobs GH, Fenwick JA, Crognale MA, Deegan JF (1992) The all-cone retina of the garter snake: spectral mechanisms and photopigment. J Comp Physiol A 170: 701-707 Moon C, Terashima S (2002) Responses of the infrared receptors of a crotaline snake to ethanol. Neurosci Lett 334:29-32 Murphy CJ, Mutti DO, Zadnik K, Ver Hoeve K (1997) Effect of optical defocus on the visual acuity in dogs. Amer J Vet Res 58:414-418 Northcutt RG, Butler AB (1974) Retinal projections in the northern water snake Natrix sipedon sipedon (L.). J Morph 142:117-136 Northmore DPM, Granda AM (1991) Refractive state, contrast sensitivity, and resolution in the freshwater turtle, Pseudemys scripta elegans, determined by tectal visual-evoked potentials Vis Neurosci 7:619-625 16

Pasternak T, Merigan WH (1981) The luminance dependence of spatial vision in the cat. Vis Res 21:1333-1339 Prusky GT, West PWR, Douglas, RM (2000) Behavioral assessment of visual acuity in mice and rats. Vis Res 40:2201-2209 Repérant J, Rio J-P, Ward R, Hergueta S, Miceli D, Lemire M (1992) Comparative analysis of the primary visual system of reptiles. In: Gans C, Ulinski PS (eds) Biology of the Reptilia. Vol 17 University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp:175-240 Rojas MJ, Navas JA, Rector DM (2006) Evoked response potential markers for anesthetic and behavioral states. AM J Physiol Regulatory Integrative Comp Physiol 292:189-196 Schaeffel F (1991) Underwater vision in semi-aquatic European snakes Naturwissenschaften 78:373-375 Schaeffel F, De Queiroz A (!990) Alternative mechanisms of enhanced underwater vision in the garter snake Thamnophis melanogaster and T. couchii. Copeia (1):50-58 Shlaer S (1937) The relation between visual acuity and illumination. J gen Physiol 21:165-188 17

Sillman AJ, Govardovskii VI, Röhlich, Southard JA, Loew ER (1997) The photoreceptors and visual pigments of the garter snake (Thamnophis sirtalis): a microspectrophotometric, scanning electron microscope and immunocytochemical study. J Comp Physiol A 181:89-101 Walls G (1942) The vertebrate eye and its adaptive radiation. Bull Cranbrook Inst Sci 19:1-785 18

Figure captions Figure 1: Evoked potentials recorded from the telencephalon in response to square-wave gratings of different spatial frequencies with a 2.0 diopter lens. X-axis is time (msec) from start of stimulus epoch; y-axis is computer referenced voltage. (prep. WS 7-25-06). Figure 2: Relative evoked potential amplitude as a function of grating spatial frequency with a plano lens (diamonds) and a 2.0 diopter lens (squares). Snake was oriented at 45 deg to the stimulus screen. Extrapolation of the least squares lines to the x axis indicated acuities of 4.7 cycles/deg (plano lens) and 7.9 cycles/deg (2.0 diopter lens) (prep. WS 7-25-06). Figure 3: Relative evoked potential amplitude as a function of refraction lens power for a spatial frequency of 1.2 cycles/deg. Function peak corresponds to a best lens of 10.5 diopters (prep. WS 7-25-06). Figure 4: Evoked potential estimated acuity as a function of stimulus luminance. The solid straight line is a least squares regression while the dashed line directly connects the data points (prep WS 7-28-06). 19

FIGURE 1- Baker, Gawne, Loop and Pullman 20

Figure 2 - Baker, Gawne, Loop and Pullman 21

FIGURE 3 - Baker, Gawne, Loop and Pullman 22

FIGURE 4 - Baker, Gawne, Loop and Pullman 23