Report. Maternal Effects Contribute to the Superior Performance of Extra-Pair Offspring

Similar documents
Benefits of extra-pair mating may depend on environmental conditions an experimental study in the blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus)

Polygyny and extra-pair paternity enhance the opportunity for sexual selection in blue tits

doi: /osj.9.161

University of Groningen

Ecological, social, and genetic contingency of extrapair behavior in a socially monogamous bird

EXTRA-PAIR PATERNITY OF TREE SPARROW (PASSER MONTANUS) IN A SEMI-URBAN POPULATION

The effects of environmental and individual quality on reproductive performance Amininasab, Seyed Mehdi

Egg size, offspring sex and hatching asynchrony in zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

ALLOCATION OF PARENTAL INVESTMENT IN BIRDS

Interaction between maternal effects: onset of incubation and offspring sex in two populations of a passerine bird

Extra-pair paternity among Great Tits Parus major following manipulation of male signals

Survivorship. Demography and Populations. Avian life history patterns. Extremes of avian life history patterns

University of Groningen. The illusion of monogamy Bouwman, Karen Marian

Inferring female extra-pair mating behaviour from observed patterns of extra-pair paternity with a process-based model

Male parental care and monogamy in snow buntings

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

Multiple paternity and offspring quality in tree swallows

Factors Influencing Local Recruitment in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor

769 q 2005 The Royal Society

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Fitness cost of incubation in great tits (Parus major) is related to clutch size de Heij, Maaike E.; van den Hout, Piet J.

Seasonal Variation in the Song of Male House Wrens (Troglodytes aedon) Honors Research Thesis

Effects of early incubation constancy on embryonic development: An experimental study in the herring gull Larus argentatus

Manipulating rearing conditions reveals developmental sensitivity in the smaller sex of a passerine bird, the European starling Sturnus vulgaris

Nest size in monogamous passerines has recently been hypothesized

Maternal investment during egg laying and offspring sex: an experimental study of zebra finches

Hatching asynchrony and brood reduction influence immune response in Common Kestrel Falco tinnunculus nestlings

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

Maternal yolk testosterone in canary eggs: toward a better understanding of mechanisms and function

Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are breeding earlier at Creamer s Field Migratory Waterfowl Refuge, Fairbanks, AK

Pair bond and breeding success in Blue Tits Parus caeruleus and Great Tits Parus major

The effect of climate change on the correlation between avian life-history traits

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite

Testosterone, Plumage Colouration and Extra-Pair Paternity in Male North-American Barn Swallows

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics

Quantitative genetics of plumage color: lifetime effects of early nest environment on a colorful sexual signal

Below, we present the methods used to address these objectives, our preliminary results and next steps in this multi-year project.

Incubation feeding in snow buntings: female manipulation or indirect male parental care?

High levels of extra-pair paternity in an isolated, low-density, island population of tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor)

doi: /

1 - Black 2 Gold (Light) 3 - Gold. 4 - Gold (Rich Red) 5 - Black and Tan (Light gold) 6 - Black and Tan

Perceived risk of ectoparasitism reduces primary reproductive investment in tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

CU Scholar. University of Colorado, Boulder. Kelley Mccahill Spring 2017

Differing rates of extra-group paternity between two populations of the Australian magpie (Gymnorhina tibicen)

University of Groningen. Offspring fitness and individual optimization of clutch size Both, C; Tinbergen, Joost; Noordwijk, Arie J.

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: Version: Accepted Version

Multiple paternity in clutches of common lizard Lacerta vivipara: data from microsatellite markers

INFO SHEET. Cull Eggs: What To Expect And How To Reduce The Incidence.

Worksheet for Morgan/Carter Laboratory #9 Mendelian Genetics II: Drosophila

As with males, female birds engaging in extrapair copulations (EPCs) also risk contracting parasites

EGG SIZE AND LAYING SEQUENCE

HATCHING ASYNCHRONY, BROOD REDUCTION, AND FOOD LIMITATION IN A NEOTROPICAL PARROT

Offspring sex ratio in red-winged blackbirds is dependent on

Blue structural coloration of male eastern bluebirds Sialia sialis predicts incubation provisioning to females

Nestling growth in the Great Tit Parus major and the Willow Tit P. montanus

Structural and melanin coloration indicate parental effort and reproductive success in male eastern bluebirds

Nest mass variation over the nesting cycle in the Pied Flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca)

Co-operative breeding by Long-tailed Tits

THE EVOLUTION OF SEXUAL SIZE DIMORPHISM IN THE HOUSE FINCH. V. MATERNAL EFFECTS

Within clutch co-variation of egg mass and sex in the black-headed gull Mueller, Wendt; Groothuis, Ton; Eising, Corine; Daan, S; Dijkstra, C

HOW MANY BASKETS? CLUTCH SIZES THAT MAXIMIZE ANNUAL FECUNDITY OF MULTIPLE-BROODED BIRDS

A Bolt on Year in Sustainability Studies. Dr Colin RA Hewitt

Variation in egg mass in the Pied Flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca: An experimental test of the brood survival and brood reduction hypotheses

REPORTS BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS SKEW HOST OFFSPRING SEX RATIOS. Department of Biology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5B7, Canada 2

Sex-biased initial eggs favours sons in the slightly size-dimorphic Scops owl (Otus scops)

Question 3 (30 points)

Who's Your Daddy? A Study of Extra-Pair Copulation and Mating Behaviors of Protonotaria citrea

Melanin-based colorations signal strategies to cope with poor and rich environments

High Fidelity No Evidence for Extra-Pair Paternity in Siberian Jays (Perisoreus infaustus)

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING.

A future cost of misdirected parental care for brood parasitic young?

The social and genetic mating system in flickers linked to partially reversed sex roles

Nest predation, food, and female age explain seasonal declines in clutch size

Brood size and body condition in the House Sparrow Passer domesticus: the influence of brooding behaviour

Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD

ethology Ethology Mark C. Mainwaring*, David Lucy & Ian R. Hartley*

Lecture 9 - Avian Life Histories

Niche separation and Hybridization -are nestling hybrid flycatchers provided with a broader diet?

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Developmental periods, such as incubation and nestling. Sex-biased maternal effects reduce ectoparasite-induced mortality in a passerine bird

Song in the city: the effects of urban noise on communication patterns and population genetics of an Australian passerine

Biol 160: Lab 7. Modeling Evolution

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below.

Hatching Asynchrony in European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris)

Comments on the Ridge Gene, by Clayton Heathcock; February 15, 2008

Hamilton and Zuk (1982) proposed that individuals that

Reproductive success and symmetry in zebra finches

Reproductive physiology and eggs

Diversity of social-genetic relationships in the socially monogamous pied flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca) breeding in Western Siberia

Punnett Squares. and Pedigrees. How are patterns of inheritance studied? Lesson ESSENTIAL QUESTION. J S7L3.b Reproduction and genetic variation

Warmer springs lead to mistimed reproduction in great tits (Parus major) Visser, M.E.; Noordwijk, A.J. van; Tinbergen, Joost; Lessells, C.M.

FEEDING CHINESE RINGNECK PHEASANTS FOR EFFICIENT REPRODUCTION. Summary *

THE INTERACTIONS BETWEEN GENOTYPES AND HOUSING ENVIRONMENTS IN THE DOMESTIC HEN

Hole-nesting birds. In natural conditions great and blue tits breed in holes that are made by e.g. woodpeckers

Molecular evidence for extra-pair paternity and intraspecific brood parasitism in the Black-headed Gull

Do the traits of organisms provide evidence for evolution?

Transcription:

Current Biology 19, 792 797, May 12, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.068 Maternal Effects Contribute to the Superior Performance of Extra-Pair Offspring Report Michael J.L. Magrath, 1,2, Oscar Vedder, 1 Marco van der Velde, 1 and Jan Komdeur 1 1 Animal Ecology Group Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Studies University of Groningen P.O. Box 14 9750 AA Haren The Netherlands 2 Department of Zoology University of Melbourne Victoria 3010 Australia Summary The explanation for extra-pair mating in female birds remains poorly understood and contentious [1 7]. Several leading hypotheses propose that females benefit indirectly by enhancing the genetic quality of their offspring, through good genes or genetic compatibility effects [1, 8, 9]. Supporting this idea, recent studies have identified a range of fitness-related traits for which extra-pair offspring (EPO) are superior to their within-pair (WP) half-siblings [10 21]. However, such performance differences may result from nongenetic maternal effects if EPO are positioned earlier in the laying order and benefit from the advantages of earlier hatching [22, 23]. Here we show that EPO are larger, heavier, and more likely to fledge than their WP half-siblings in a population of blue tits, Cyanistes caeruleus. However, extra-pair paternity declined markedly with laying order, resulting in EPO generally hatching earlier. After correcting for variation in hatch time, none of the observed disparities between EPO and their WP half-siblings remained significant. These findings indicate that phenotypic comparisons between maternal half-siblings must consider potential hatchingorder effects and suggest that the evidence for genetic benefits from extra-pair copulation may be less compelling than currently accepted. Moreover, the overrepresentation of EPO early in the laying order may help explain female extra-pair mating. Results and Discussion In many socially monogamous birds, individuals commonly participate in extra-pair copulations (EPC) that can result in extra-pair offspring (EPO) [1]. Although males may clearly benefit by siring more offspring, explanations for female participation in EPC remain contentious and poorly understood [1 7]. Most attention has focused on the idea that females engage in EPC to improve the genetic quality of their offspring, by mating with extra-pair males that are either genetically more compatible (e.g., more dissimilar) or of higher quality (the good-genes hypothesis) than their social partner [1, 8]. This hypothesis predicts that EPO should be both Correspondence: magrath@unimelb.edu.au genetically and phenotypically superior to their within-pair (WP) half-siblings [1, 9] and has been tested by comparing the viability of EPO and within-pair offspring (WPO) from the same nest because, all else being equal, they should only differ in relation to the genetic contribution of the father [10, 11]. In accord with this prediction, numerous previous studies have revealed that EPO are superior to their maternal half-siblings in a range of fitness-related traits including growth rate [12, 13], fledging body mass, size and/or condition [11, 14 16], survival to fledging [10, 17], immune function [12, 18, 19], and recruitment to the local breeding population in subsequent years [20, 21]. Collectively, these studies represent the strongest support for the idea that females engage in EPC to improve the genetic quality of their offspring [1, 6]. However, such performance differences could also arise through nongenetic effects if EPO tend to be placed earlier in the laying sequence [22, 23]. Because incubation commonly starts before clutch completion, early-laid eggs often hatch before late-laid eggs (hatching asynchrony), and these differences in hatching time typically favor the growth and survival prospects of older siblings [24 26]. In this study, we examined the relationship between paternity and laying order in blue tits, Cyanistes caeruleus, by hatching out 154 wild-collected clutches in incubators (see Experimental Procedures). Individual chicks were returned to their nest soon after hatching, allowing us to assess whether position in the laying order favored the performance of EPO compared to their WP half-siblings. Blue tits are well suited to address this question because (1) they produce large clutches, with a mean of 11.5 6 2.1 (SD) in this study (2); EPO are common in blue tits [10, 17, 27 29], typically occurring in over 40% of broods; and (3) clutches often hatch asynchronously over several days, resulting in distinct size hierarchies and frequent mortality of smaller, late-hatching chicks [30, 31]. Furthermore, previous studies in blue tits have reported that EPO are larger and heavier in some circumstances [16] and are more likely to fledge than their WP half-siblings [10, 17]. Laying Order and Paternity Among the 122 clutches for which both social parents were caught, 12.8% of the 1176 genotyped offspring were sired by extra-pair males, and 46.7% of these clutches contained at least one EPO. Within the 55 mixed-paternity clutches, the proportion of EPO declined markedly with laying order (c 2 = 49.04, p < 0.001; Figure 1; see also Table S1 available online, models 1a and 1b). Overall, 74% of EPO were positioned within the first half of clutches. The strong relationship between paternity and laying order did not vary with year of the study; sex of embryo; or laying date, size, or hatching asynchrony of the clutch (p > 0.30 for the interaction between these variables and laying order; Table S1, models 1a and 1b). Hatching Time and Paternity The hatching asynchrony of clutches in incubators ranged from 9.5 to 127 hr (mean = 53.4; SD = 22.2; n = 118 clutches), which was similar to that observed among naturally hatching clutches in the same population in the 2008 breeding season

Laying Order and Extra-Pair Paternity 793 70 60 n = 121 broods, 1217 hatchlings 50 Hatch time (hr) 40 30 20 10 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 Relative laying order Figure 1. Proportion of Extra-Pair Offspring in Relation to Laying Order of Eggs in Mixed-Paternity Clutches of Blue Tits Actual laying order was transformed to a scale of 0 to 1 ( relative laying order ) to standardize for variation in clutch size (see Experimental Procedures). Points represent mean values of the raw data, separated into ten equally distributed categories of relative laying order, together with the binomial standard error. The solid line shows the model-predicted decline in probability of extra-pair offspring with relative laying order (see Table S1, model 1a for full model details). A similar relationship was evident with actual rather than relative laying order (see Table S1, model 1b). (mean = 57.1 6 19.7 [SD]; n = 39). Within clutches, the average chick hatched 15.87 hr (SE = 0.64; n = 118 clutches, 1217 chicks) after the first chick (or chicks). There was generally a strong, nonlinear increase in hatch time (hr since hatching of the first chick [or chicks]; see Experimental Procedures) with laying order (c 2 = 223.11; p < 0.001), and unsurprisingly, the gradient of this relationship was dependent on the level of hatching asynchrony (c 2 = 50.22; p < 0.001; Figure 2; Table S1, model 2a). Within mixed-paternity clutches, EPO hatched an average of 9.28 6 2.12 hr earlier than their WP half-siblings (c 2 = 21.22; p < 0.001; n = 43 broods, 419 hatchlings; Table S1, model 2b). Inclusion of hatching asynchrony in this model revealed that the difference in hatch time between EP and WP half-siblings increased with hatching asynchrony (c 2 = 4.68; p = 0.03; Figure 3; Table S1, model 2c). However, both the mean difference in hatch time and interaction with hatching asynchrony were absent after statistically controlling for the effect of laying order (p = 0.48 and 0.71, respectively; see Table S1, model 2d). Hence, there was no evidence for a difference in the rate of embryonic development between EPO and their WP half-siblings. Hatching Time, Paternity, and Nestling Performance An average of 69.5% 6 3.1% (SE) of hatchlings (n = 141 broods, 1309 hatchlings) survived to brood age 15 (oldest chick [or chicks] = day 15), several days prior to the usual age of fledging. The probability of survival to brood age 15 declined markedly with hatch time (Figure 4A; Table S1, model 3a). Within mixed-paternity broods, EPO had a greater likelihood of surviving to brood age 15 compared to their WP half-siblings (p = 0.04; n = 49 broods), but this difference was no longer significant (p = 0.48) after correction for variation in hatch time (Figure 5; Table S1, models 3b and 3c). Figure 2. Hatch Time Relative to First Chick in Relation to Relative Laying Order of Eggs in Clutches of Blue Tits Vertical bars represent standard error about the means of the raw data separated into ten equally distributed categories of relative laying order. See Table S1, model 2a for model details. Within 15-day-old broods, body mass, wing length, and tarsus length all declined strongly with hatch time (Figures 4B 4D; Table S1, models 4a, 5a, and 6a). Consequently, within mixed-paternity broods, EPO had longer wings (p = 0.04) and tarsi (p = 0.001) and were heavier (p = 0.01) than their WP half-siblings (Figure 5; Table S1, models 4b, 5b, and 6b). Moreover, the disparity in wing length increased with the degree of hatching asynchrony (p = 0.01; Table S1, model 4c). Again, however, these differences were not significant after correction for hatch time (Figure 5; Table S1, models 4d, 5c, and 6c), although EPO still tended to be heavier (p = 0.09) and have longer tarsi (p = 0.07). Comparisons between Within-Pair and Extra-Pair Offspring In line with previous studies (see references above), including several on blue tits [10, 16, 17], we found that EPO were larger, heavier, and more likely to fledge than their WP half-siblings. However, EPO were three times more likely to be produced in the first half of the clutch, translating to EPO hatching earlier than their WP half-siblings among asynchronously hatching clutches. Moreover, this difference in hatching time completely accounted for the disparities we observed in survival and wing length, whereas the differences in tarsus length and body mass were still evident but were no longer statistically significant. These findings show that position in the laying order favored EPO in this population. Indeed, the real disparities in hatching time and survival were most likely greater than observed because some chicks disappeared from the nest before blood sampling, and these chicks were typically from eggs produced late in the clutch and presumably more likely to be WPO. More generally, we suggest that most of the fitness-related traits for which EPO have previously been identified as superior to their WP half-siblings could be influenced by hatching time. Immune function is known to improve with nestling age, and earlier-hatching chicks exhibit a greater response to immune challenges than their younger siblings [32].

Current Biology Vol 19 No 9 794 Difference in mean hatch time between EPO and WPO (hr) 50 30 10-10 -30 n = 43 broods, 419 hatchlings 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 Hatching asynchrony (hr) Figure 3. Difference in Hatch Time between Extra-Pair Offspring and Their Within-Pair Half-Siblings in Relation to Degree of Hatching Asynchrony in Mixed-Paternity Clutches of Blue Tits Points show the difference between the mean hatch time of extra-pair offspring (EPO) versus within-pair offspring (WPO) in each clutch. The solid line shows the model-predicted difference in hatch time between EPO and WPO (paternity 3 hatching asynchrony interaction; c 2 = 4.68; p = 0.03; see Table S1, model 2c for details). Moreover, many studies, including our own, have shown that late-hatching individuals are less likely to fledge and/or are smaller and lighter than their older siblings [30 32]. Significantly, greater size and mass at fledging have been associated with greater future survival probability and reproductive success in a range of birds [32]. In the absence of genetic differences, there are at least two reasons to expect phenotypic disparities between early- and late-hatching chicks when brood members are measured at a given point in time. First, earlier-hatching chicks should be developmentally more advanced simply because they are older. Second, developmental rates may be modified by sibling competition for food, such that the youngest, less competitive chicks may not reach the same size or weight even after accounting for age differences [32, 33]. One approach to dealing with unknown variation in hatching time has been to statistically correct for age-related morphological traits [e.g., 12, 13, 18, 19]. Such corrections are not perfect because of (1) the nonlinear growth of most morphological traits, (2) likely differences in growth trajectories related to hatching time, (3) masking of genuine differences in growth, and/or (4) catch-up growth over the nestling period that may occur at the expense of immune function or other components of fitness expressed later in life [34 37]. Nevertheless, in the absence of exact hatching times, corrections for traits early in the nestling period, such as body mass and wing length, should go some way to controlling for disparities in size caused by variation in hatch time. We observed that the difference in hatching time and wing length between EPO and their WP half-siblings increased across broods with the degree of hatching asynchrony. These findings reveal how variation in hatching asynchrony could also contribute to explaining some of the reported contextual A B C D Survival probability Relative body mass (g) Relative wing length (mm) Relative tarsus length (mm) 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.2 0.0-0.2-0.4-0.6-0.8 2 0-2 -4-6 -8-10 0.2 0.0-0.2-0.4-0.6-0.8 n = 141 broods, 1309 hatchlings Mean = 11.38 ± 0.05 g n = 119 broods, 937 hatchlings Mean = 30.32 ± 0.19 mm n = 120 broods, 949 hatchlings Mean = 16.74 ± 0.03 mm n = 119 broods, 937 hatchlings 20 40 60 80 100 Hatch time (hr) Figure 4. Hatch Time of Chicks in Clutches of Blue Tits Relative to the First Egg in Relation to Measures of Performance at Brood Age 15 Days (A) Probability of surviving to brood age 15 days. (B) Body mass. (C) Wing length. (D) Tarsus length. These morphological measurements have been centered about brood means to correct for between-brood variation. The solid lines show the within-brood model-predicted relationships (see Table S1 for full model details). The error bars represent the standard error of the model-predicted values for each 10 hr interval in hatch time. The horizontal dotted lines in (B) (D) represent zero deviation from the brood mean values. effects of EPO superiority that have previously been attributed to genotype-by-environment interactions [e.g., 21]. Indeed, any circumstances that promote hatching asynchrony (e.g., reduced food availability) should magnify differences between EPO and WPO where there is a relationship (in either direction)

Laying Order and Extra-Pair Paternity 795 % Difference between EPO and their WP half-sibs 10 8 6 4 2 0-2 Not corrected for Hatch time Corrected for Hatch time n = number of broods Survival Wing Tarsus Body mass Offspring trait Figure 5. Estimated Differences in Survival and Body Size between Extra- Pair Offspring and Their Within-Pair Half-Siblings, with and without Correction for Hatch Time Survival and body size measures were assessed at brood age 15 days. Bars show the mean difference with standard error, expressed as a percentage derived by dividing the model-predicted difference (see Table S1) by the mean trait values (see Figure 4). Asterisks indicate significance difference from zero (p < 0.05). See text for specific p values and Table S1 for model details. between paternity and laying order. In concert with hatching asynchrony, the level of brood reduction may also be expected to influence disparities between EPO and WPO because brood reduction will disproportionately affect late-hatching chicks but may also reduce morphological variation among the remaining offspring. Why Are Extra-Pair Offspring Overrepresented Early in the Laying Sequence? Despite the confounding effects of laying order, our findings do not preclude the possibility that EPO are genetically superior to WPO. Genetic benefits to EPO may be expressed later in life (e.g., attractiveness, fecundity, longevity) or only in certain contexts related to environmental circumstances [12, 21]. Moreover, these indirect genetic benefits may be small and difficult to detect [1, 38]. Indeed, we observed a tendency for EPO to be both heavier and larger than their WP half-siblings, even after accounting for variation in hatching time, which may reflect real differences in genetic quality. Consequently, the early placement of EPO in the laying order may represent an adaptive maternal strategy to provide them with the advantages of earlier hatching [22, 23], assuming that EPC has some cost. (Otherwise, it would pay females to produce EPO throughout the laying order.) Alternatively, the opportunity for females to solicit EPC may decline once laying commences. For example, male mate guarding may become more effective because pair males have better information about their partner s fertility status, or females may be constrained by the need to acquire resources for egg production [22]. Early production of EPO in the laying sequence is also consistent with the fertility-insurance hypothesis [23]. EPC before laying commences increases the likelihood that even the earliest laid eggs will be fertilized in the event of pairmale infertility, whereas EPC performed after the start of laying can only fertilize later eggs. Furthermore, because viable sperm can be stored for an extended period [39], EPC motivated by fertility insurance could cease well before clutch completion, resulting in the observed decline in EPO over the laying sequence when the social male is fertile. Rapid social mate switching early in the female s fertile period could also account for some cases of apparent EPO but is unlikely to provide a general explanation because in all cases in which the identity of a pair was established during nest building (n = 16), the pair remained together. Similarly, mate switching during the prelaying period was not observed in a Belgian population of blue tits [10]. Conclusions Our findings indicate that potential effects of laying order must always be considered when comparisons are drawn between maternal half-siblings and add to growing doubts about the strength of the empirical evidence for the genetic-benefit hypotheses of extra-pair mating ([3 6, 29], but see [2, 7]). At least one other study has reported a decline in EPO with laying order [23], but further studies are clearly warranted to assess the generality of this pattern. Indeed, variation in the nature of the relationship between paternity and laying order, coupled with population differences in the degree of hatching asynchrony and brood reduction, may contribute to explaining the inconsistent findings reported across the many studies that have compared EPO with WPO [6]. We have suggested several possible explanations for the decline in EPO with laying order, but most are not mutually exclusive and may be difficult to disentangle. Nevertheless, an explanation is clearly worth seeking because it may well help reveal why female birds engage in EPC. Experimental Procedures Field Procedures This study was conducted over two seasons (2006 and 2007) on a nest-box population of blue tits in De Vosbergen estate near Groningen in the Netherlands (53 08 N, 06 35 E). All active nests were visited daily during laying, and eggs were numbered in the order of laying. Clutches were collected 1 2 days before hatching was anticipated (11 12 days after incubation commenced) and replaced with plastic model eggs to prevent desertion. Eggs were placed into individual compartments within incubators (Therbo-S) maintained at between 36.0 C and 37.5 C and between 50% and 80% relative humidity. Chicks hatching during the day (0630 2100) were returned to their nest within 2 hr, and those hatching overnight were returned by 0800 the following morning. Prior to return, chicks were individually marked by clipping the tip of one or two toenails. The time of return to the nest was regarded as a chick s hatch time because feeding could only commence after this point. Brood age was defined as the number of days after the hatching of the oldest chick. Of 1732 eggs placed in incubators, 93% hatched successfully, 4% were infertile or showed minimal embryonic development, and 3% contained embryos but failed to hatch. Dead embryos were collected for subsequent molecular analysis. When broods were 3 to 6 days old, all surviving chicks were blood sampled (5 25 ml) and any dead chicks were collected. Overall, 88.3% of 1668 hatchlings were genotyped. Chicks were fitted with uniquely numbered leg rings when broods were between days 8 15. When broods were 15 days old, we measured body mass (to the nearest 0.1 g), tarsus length (to the nearest 0.1 mm), and length of the third (outermost) primary feather (to the nearest 0.5 mm; referred to as wing length) of the remaining chicks. Social parents were caught with nest-box traps for blood sampling when broods were 8 10 days old. Molecular Parentage and Sexing We isolated DNA from blood samples via a chelex extraction procedure [40] and from tissue via a salt extraction procedure [41]. Sex of offspring was

Current Biology Vol 19 No 9 796 determined as described in [42]. Polymerase chain reaction (PCR) products were separated by electrophoresis on a 2% agarose gel or an AB3730 DNA analyzer. To exclude paternity, we genotyped parents and offspring for six microsatellite loci: Pca3, Pca7, Pca8, and Pca9 [43]; Pocc6 [44]; and Pdo5 [45]. PCR reactions were carried out in a 10 ml volume containing 20 50 ng DNA, 0.2 mm each deoxyribonucleotide triphosphate (dntp), 0.25 mm forward and reverse primer, 10 mm Tris-HCl, 50 mm KCl, 1.5 mm MgCl 2, and 0.125 U Taq DNA polymerase (Roche Diagnostics GmbH). The PCR program was 94 C for 1 min, 40 cycles of 94 C for 30 s, T a for 45 s and 72 C for 45 s, followed by 72 C for 2 min. T a was 55 C for Pca3, Pca8, Pocc6, and Pdo5; 60 C for Pca7; and 63 C for Pca9. Fluorescently labeled PCR products were separated on an AB3730 DNA analyzer. Subsequently, allele lengths were determined with GeneMapper 4.0 software. Using CERVUS 3.0 [46], mean exclusion probability of the six markers was calculated to be 0.99935 for the first (female) parent and 0.99997 for the second (male) parent (given the genotype of the first parent). Paternity of the social male was excluded if there were at least two mismatches between the genotypes of the putative father and offspring and the social mother matched the offspring for at least five of the six loci. Single mismatches between parents and 1176 offspring occurred at nine maternal and six paternal loci, most apparently as a result of single-repeat slippage mutations. Statistical Analyses Most analyses were conducted via hierarchical mixed modeling to account for the nonindependence and hierarchical structure of the data. These models all included a random component with clutch (or brood) identity at level two and egg (or chick) identity at level one. Binomial response models were fitted to examine the relationship between laying order (or hatching time) and the binary response variables of paternity and survival to day 15, whereas effects of hatching time on the day 15 chick measurements of body mass, tarsus length, and wing length were examined via normal response models. These morphometric measurements were converted to a relative value by subtraction from the brood mean (i.e., centering) because we were only interested in within-brood variation. The explanatory variable of laying order was transformed to a scale of 0 to 1 (division of actual laying position by clutch size) to standardize variation in clutch size, which ranged from 5 to 18 (mean = 11.47 6 2.06 [SD], n = 188). This relative scale should be more biologically meaningful because only the last few eggs usually hatch appreciably later than earlier eggs (see Figure 2), regardless of clutch size. Nevertheless, we repeated these analyses with actual rather than relative laying order and also report these results. Laying order was strongly related to hatching order (see Figure 2), but we expected that hatching time relative to other chicks in the brood would have a more important influence on chick performance than laying order per se. Therefore, for each chick in the brood, we quantified hatch time as the number of hours after the hatching of the first chick (or chicks) in that brood and used this in all analyses of offspring performance. In the models where hatch time was the dependent variable, it was log-transformed to account for the strongly right-skewed distribution of these data. The other explanatory variable tested in the models that varied at the egg (or chick) level was offspring sex. We also tested clutch-level (or broodlevel) variables, including hatching asynchrony (interval in hr between when the first and the last egg in the clutch hatched), year (2006 or 2007), clutch size, and relative laying date (clutch initiation date centered about the median clutch initiation date for that year), in order to assess these variables possible interaction with laying order and/or hatch time. Potential nonlinear effects of laying order and hatch time were explored by inclusion of their squared terms. Examinations of potential differences in hatch time, survival, and morphometrics between EPO and their WP half-siblings were restricted to the subset of mixed-paternity clutches (or broods). These analyses were conducted in three stages. First, we constructed models including only paternity, offspring sex, and the proportion of EPO in the clutch (or brood), allowing us to estimate mean differences between EPO and their WP halfsiblings. Second, we included hatching asynchrony to assess whether it influenced the difference between EPO and their WP half-siblings (i.e., interaction between paternity and hatching asynchrony). Third, we introduced laying order or hatch time into the models (including their squared and interaction terms) to assess how they affected the observed difference between EPO and WPO. Because of our primary interest in within-brood comparisons, we also included mean hatch time of chicks in the brood to account for variation in hatching asynchrony between broods. The significance of all explanatory terms was determined via the Wald statistic, which approximates the c 2 distribution. Full and final model summaries are presented in Table S1. Sample sizes varied between analyses depending on the parameters included in the final model. All hierarchical modeling procedures were performed in MLwiN 2.02 [47]. Supplemental Data The Supplemental Data include one table and can be found with this article online at http://www.cell.com/current-biology/supplemental/s0960-9822(09)00975-0. Acknowledgments We thank Rob Gordijn, Evelien Jongepier, Carol van Kershaver, Daan Middendorp, Daphne Niehoff, Jelmer Samplonius, Peter Santema, Elske Schut, Jennifer Struikenkamp, Femke Tamminga, and Laura Vossen for indispensable help in the field and Simon Griffith, Bart Kempenaers, Peter Korsten, and Jan Lifjeld for helpful comments on the manuscript. We are also grateful to the Kraus-Groeneveld Stichting for permission to work on De Vosbergen estate. This research was financially supported by a Marie Curie International Incoming Fellowship to M.J.L.M. (CMIF1-CT-2005-008914) and grant number 028696 to J.K., both of which are components of the European Community s Sixth Framework Programme. Received: February 17, 2009 Revised: March 24, 2009 Accepted: March 24, 2009 Published online: April 30, 2009 References 1. Griffith, S.C., Owens, I.P.F., and Thuman, K.A. (2002). Extra-pair paternity in birds: A review of interspecific variation and adaptive function. Mol. Ecol. 11, 2195 2212. 2. Griffith, S.C. (2007). The evolution of infidelity in socially monogamous passerines: Neglected components of direct and indirect selection. Am. Nat. 169, 274 281. 3. Westneat, D.F., and Stewart, I.R.K. (2003). Extra-pair paternity in birds: Causes, correlations, and conflict. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 34, 365 396. 4. Arnqvist, G., and Kirkpatrick, M. (2005). The evolution of infidelity in socially monogamous passerines: The strength of direct and indirect selection on extra-pair copulation behavior in females. Am. Nat. 165, S26 S37. 5. Arnqvist, G., and Kirkpatrick, M. (2007). The evolution of infidelity in socially monogamous passerines revisited: A reply to Griffith. Am. Nat. 169, 282 283. 6. Akçay, E., and Roughgarden, J. (2007). Extra-pair paternity in birds: Review of the genetic benefits. Evol. Ecol. Res. 9, 855 868. 7. Eliassen, S., and Kokko, H. (2008). Current analyses do not resolve whether extra-pair paternity is male or female driven. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 1795 1804. 8. Jennions, M.D., and Petrie, M. (2000). Why do females mate multiply? A review of the genetic benefits. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 75, 21 64. 9. Neff, B.D., and Pitcher, T.E. (2005). Genetic quality and sexual selection: An integrated framework for good genes and compatible genes. Mol. Ecol. 14, 19 38. 10. Kempenaers, B., Verheyen, G.R., and Dhont, A. (1997). Extra-pair paternity in the blue tit Parus caeruleus: Female choice, male characteristics, and offspring quality. Behav. Ecol. 8, 481 492. 11. Sheldon, B.C., Merila, J., Qvarnstrom, A., Gustafsson, L., and Ellegren, H. (1997). Paternal genetic contribution to offspring condition predicted by size of male secondary sexual character. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 264, 297 302. 12. Garvin, J.C., Abroe, B., Pedersen, M.C., Dunn, P.O., and Whittingham, L.A. (2006). Immune response of nestling warblers varies with extra-pair paternity and temperature. Mol. Ecol. 15, 3833 3840. 13. O Brien, E.L., and Dawson, R.D. (2007). Context-dependent genetic benefits of extra-pair mate choice in a socially monogamous passerine. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 775 782. 14. Freeman-Gallant, C.R., Wheelwright, N.T., Meiklejohn, K.E., and Sollecito, S.V. (2006). Genetic similarity, extrapair paternity, and offspring quality in and Savannah sparrows Passerculus sandwichensis. Behav. Ecol. 17, 952 958.

Laying Order and Extra-Pair Paternity 797 15. Bouwman, K.M., Van Dijk, R.E., Wijmenga, J.J., and Komdeur, J. (2007). Older male reed buntings are more successful at gaining extrapair fertilizations. Anim. Behav. 73, 15 27. 16. Dreiss, A.N., Silva, N., Richard, M., Moyen, F., Thery, M., Møller, A.P., and Danchin, E. (2008). Condition-dependent genetic benefits of extrapair fertilization in female blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus. J. Evol. Biol. 21, 1814 1822. 17. Charmantier, A., Blondel, J., Perret, P., and Lambrechts, M.M. (2004). Do extra-pair paternities provide genetic benefits for female blue tits Parus caeruleus? J. Avian Biol. 35, 524 532. 18. Johnsen, A., Andersen, V., Sunding, C., and Lifjeld, J.T. (2000). Female bluethroats enhance offspring immunocompetence through extra-pair copulation. Nature 406, 296 299. 19. Fossøy, F., Johnsen, A., and Lifjeld, J.T. (2008). Multiple genetic benefits of female promiscuity in a socially monogamous passerine. Evolution 62, 145 156. 20. Hasselquist, D., Bensch, S., and von Schantz, T. (1996). Correlation between male song repertoire, extra-pair paternity and offspring survival in the great reed warbler. Nature 381, 229 232. 21. Schmoll, T., Dietrich, V., Winkel, W., Epplen, J.T., Schurr, F., and Lubjuhn, T. (2005). Paternal genetic effects on offspring fitness are context dependent within the extra-pair mating system of a socially monogamous passerine. Evolution 59, 645 657. 22. Cordero, P.J., Wetton, J.H., and Parkin, D.T. (1999). Within-clutch patterns of egg viability and paternity in the house sparrow. J. Avian Biol. 30, 103 107. 23. Krist, M., Nádvorník, P., Uvírová, L., and Bureš, S. (2005). Paternity covaries with laying and hatching order in the collared flycatcher Ficedula albicollis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59, 6 11. 24. Magrath, R.D. (1990). Hatching asynchrony in altricial birds. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 95, 587 622. 25. Stoleson, S.H., and Beissinger, S.R. (1995). Hatching asynchrony and the onset of incubation in birds: When is the critical period? Curr. Ornithol. 12, 191 270. 26. Gebhardt-Henrich, S., and Richner, H. (1998). Causes of growth variation and its consequences for fitness. In Avian Growth and Development: Evolution within the Altricial-Precocial Spectrum, J.M. Stark and R.E. Ricklefs, eds. (New York: Oxford University Press), pp. 324 339. 27. Krokene, C., Rigstad, K., Dale, M., and Lifjeld, J.T. (1998). The function of extra-pair paternity in blue tits and great tits: Good genes or fertility insurance? Behav. Ecol. 9, 649 656. 28. Leech, D.I., Hartley, I.R., Stewart, I.R.K., Griffith, S.C., and Burke, T. (2001). No effect of parental quality or extrapair paternity on brood sex ratio in the blue tit Parus caeruleus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 12, 674 680. 29. Brommer, J.E., Korsten, P., Bouwman, K.M., Berg, M.L., and Komdeur, J. (2007). Is extrapair mating random? On the probability distribution of extrapair young in avian broods. Behav. Ecol. 18, 895 904. 30. Cramp, S., and Perrins, C.M. (1993). Birds of the Western Palearctic, Volume 7, S. Cramp and C.M. Perrins, eds. (Oxford: Oxford University Press), pp. 225 248. 31. Slagsvold, T., Amundsen, T., and Dale, S. (1995). Costs and benefits of hatching asynchrony in blue tits Parus caeruleus. J. Anim. Ecol. 64, 563 578. 32. Roulin, A., Christe, P., Dijkstra, C., Ducrest, A., and Jungi, T.W. (2007). Origin-related, environmental, sex, and age determinants of immunocompetence, susceptibility to ectoparasites, and disease symptoms in the barn owl. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 90, 703 718. 33. Schew, W.A., and Ricklefs, R.E. (1998). Developmental plasticity. In Avian Growth and Development: Evolution within the Altricial-Precocial Spectrum, J.M. Stark and R.E. Ricklefs, eds. (New York: Oxford University Press), pp. 288 305. 34. Metcalfe, N.B., and Monaghan, P. (2001). Compensation for a bad start: Grow now, pay later? Trends Ecol. Evol. 16, 254 260. 35. Lochmiller, R.L., and Deerenberg, C. (2000). Trade-offs in evolutionary immunology: Just what is the cost of immunity? Oikos 88, 87 98. 36. Brommer, J.E. (2004). Immunocompetence and its costs during development: An experimental study in blue tit nestlings. Biol. Lett. 271, S110 S113. 37. Kunz, C., and Ekman, J. (2000). Genetic and environmental components of growth in nestling blue tits (Parus caeruleus). J. Evol. Biol. 13, 199 212. 38. Møller, A.P., and Alatalo, R.V. (1999). Good-genes effects in sexual selection. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 266, 85 91. 39. Birkhead, T.R., and Møller, A.P. (1992). Number and size of sperm storage tubules and the duration of sperm storage in birds: A comparative study. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 45, 363 372. 40. Walsh, P.S., Metzger, D.A., and Higuchi, R. (1991). Chelex-100 as a medium for simple extraction of DNA for PCR-based typing from forensic material. Biotechniques 10, 506 513. 41. Richardson, D.S., Jury, F.L., Blaakmeer, K., Komdeur, J., and Burke, T. (2001). Parentage assignment and extra-group paternity in a cooperative breeder: The Seychelles warbler Acrocephalus sechellensis. Mol. Ecol. 10, 2263 2273. 42. Griffiths, R., Double, M.C., Orr, K., and Dawson, R.J. (1998). A DNA test to sex most birds. Mol. Ecol. 7, 1071 1075. 43. Dawson, D.A., Hanotte, O., Greig, C., Stewart, I.R.K., and Burke, T. (2000). Polymorphic microsatellites in the blue tit Parus caeruleus and their cross-species utility in 20 songbird families. Mol. Ecol. 9, 1941 1944. 44. Bensch, S., Price, T., and Kohn, J. (1997). Isolation and characterization of microsatellite loci in a Phylloscopus warbler. Mol. Ecol. 6, 91 92. 45. Griffith, S.C., Stewart, I.R.K., Dawson, D.A., Owens, I.P.F., and Burke, T. (1999). Contrasting levels of extra-pair paternity in mainland and island populations of the house sparrow Passer domesticus: Is there an island effect? Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 68, 303 316. 46. Kalinowski, S.T., Taper, M.L., and Marshall, T.C. (2007). Revising how the computer program CERVUS accommodates genotyping error increases success in paternity assignment. Mol. Ecol. 16, 1099 1106. 47. Rasbash, J., Steele, F., Browne, W., and Prosser, B. (2004). A User s Guide to MLwiN Version 2.0 (London: Institute of Education).