The response of guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis Familiaris) to cues of human referential communication (pointing and gaze)

Similar documents
Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Comp Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 August 01.

Inter-specific visual communication and cognition in the context of domestication

Evolutionary approach to communication between humans and dogs

Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) Are Sensitive to the Attentional State of Humans

Explaining Dog Wolf Differences in Utilizing Human Pointing Gestures: Selection for Synergistic Shifts in the Development of Some Social Skills

Breed Differences in Domestic Dogs' (Canis familiaris) Comprehension of Human Communicative Signals

Domestic Dogs Use Contextual Information and Tone of Voice when following a Human Pointing Gesture

Behavioural Processes

Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans attentional focus

Animal Behaviour xxx (2011) 1e8. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Behavioural Processes

Do domestic dogs interpret pointing as a command?

My owner, right or wrong: the effect of familiarity on the domestic dog s behavior in a food-choice task

PERSPECTIVE TAKING AND KNOWLEDGE ATTRIBUTION IN THE. DOMESTIC DOG (Canis familiaris): A CANINE THEORY OF MIND?

Institutionen för fysik, kemi och biologi. Examensarbete 16 hp. The effect of breed selection on interpreting human directed cues in the domestic dog

Applied Animal Behaviour Science

Sensing sociality in dogs: what may make an interactive robot social?

The integration of dogs into collaborative humanrobot. - An applied ethological approach - PhD Thesis. Linda Gerencsér Supervisor: Ádám Miklósi

Clever hounds: social cognition in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Applied Animal Behaviour Science

What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of

Do Dogs (Canis familiaris) Seek Help in an Emergency?

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Dogs (Canis familiaris) adjust their social behaviour to the differential role of inanimate interactive agents

Applied Animal Behaviour Science

Influence of delayed timing of owners actions on the behaviors of their dogs, Canis familiaris

Journal of Comparative Psychology

What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs sensitivity to human actions

Communication between domestic dogs and humans: effects of shelter housing upon the gaze to the human

Social Referencing in Domestic Dogs: The Effects of Human Affective Behavior on Canines Point Following

Do Tamed Domesticated Dogs (Canis familiaris) Ignore Deceptive Human Cues When the Actual Food Location is Visible?

Ethologically inspired robot design

Modelling behavioural evolution and cognition in canines: Some problematic issues

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2016, 105, NUMBER 1 (JANUARY)

LIVING WITH WOLVES. They are creatures of legend,

Behavioural Processes

Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs

Prospective object search in dogs: mixed evidence for knowledge of What and Where

Animal Behaviour. Do dogs distinguish rational from irrational acts?

Incentive Contrast in Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris)

Human-directed social behaviour in dogs shows significant heritability

Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) use of the solidity principle

Social mimetic behaviour and social anticipation in dogs: preliminary results

Man s other best friend: domestic cats (F. silvestris catus) and their discrimination of human emotion cues

Attachment and Sociability in Therapy Dogs. by Shelby Hiigel Wanser A PROJECT. submitted to. Oregon State University. University Honors College

Simple Mechanisms Can Explain Social Learning in Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris)

Research Article DOI: / Tauzin et al. Dogs Identify Agents Through Contingency

Behavior Modification Reinforcement and Rewards

SWGDOG SC 9 - HUMAN SCENT DOGS Avalanche Search

New research relevant to companion animal welfare

Tinbergen s four questions for investigating behavior. Mechanism Ontogeny Function Evolution. Topic for today

Human Uniqueness. Human Uniqueness. Why are we so different? 12/6/2017. Four Candidates

Remember! Life skills for puppies

R U S T Y D O G N I T I O N R E P O R T - A P R I L 1 2,

The Feeding Behavior of Dogs Correlates with their Responses to Commands

Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology. Master Thesis. Labrador and German shepherd breed differences in dog-human communication.

The Development of Behavior

This article is downloaded from.

Vision during head bobbing: are pigeons capable of shape discrimination during the thrust phase?

Clicker Training Guide

SUBNOVICE OBJECTIVES. Successful completion of this class means that the following objectives were obtained:

CANINE IQ TEST. Dogs tend to enjoy the tests since they don't know that they are being tested and merely think that you are playing with

Resistance to Impulsivity and Temporal Discounting in Canis lupus familiaris

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

TRAINING DOMESTIC DOGS (CANIS LUPUS FAMILIARIS) ON A NOVEL ODOR- DETECTION TASK IN DISCRETE TRIALS

Naughty But Nice. minute. 3gamechangers

Conflict-Related Aggression

Time, Number, Space, and the Domestic Dog

Golden Rule Training

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Anthrozoos. Author manuscript.

B U S T E R D O G N I T I O N R E P O R T - A P R I L 1 2,

Psy Advanced Laboratory in Operant Behavior Dognition Laboratory One. I. Let s see how well your dog can observe pointing signals.

NBN 3MIN GAME CHANGERS

1.4. Initial training shall include sufficient obedience training to ensure the canine will operate effectively based on mission requirements.

Five simple boosts, huge relationship. BOOST. relationship

BEGINNER I OBEDIENCE Week #1 Homework

SWGDOG SC9 HUMAN SCENT DOGS Searching for Human Remains in Disaster Environments Posted for Public Comment 4/24/12 6/22/12

Basic Commands and Training

Context specificity of inhibitory control in dogs

Nathaniel J. Hall. Curriculum Vitae NW 7 th Ave 931 Center Drive Gainesville, Fl Gainesville, Fl 32611

Human-directed social behaviour in dogs shows significant heritability

A comparison of rating and coding behavioural traits in dogs

The S Files Success with Maria: Sunshine: Biting Reported by S.G. Friedman, PhD and L. McGuire

1.3. Initial training shall include sufficient obedience training to perform an effective and controlled search.

A Thesis Presented to. The Faculty of Alfred University. Insecure Attachments in Dogs and Interpreting Human Social Signals.

Puppy Development. Part One

An Attachment Style Based Experimental Design to Maximize Dog Adoption Success

Establishing a routine

How to have a well behaved dog

C H A S E R D O G N I T I O N R E P O R T - A P R I L 0 5,

!"#$%&'()*&+,)-,)."#/')!,)0#/') 1/2)3&'45)."#+"/5%&6)7/,-,$,8)9::;:<;<=)>6+#-"?!

1.2. Handler training shall include human scent theory, relevant canine case law and legal preparation, including court testimony.

Free Bonus: Teach your Miniature Schnauzer 13 Amazing Tricks!

Mastering the water blind (aka the memory mark) by Jeff Martin

THE FIVE COMMANDS EVERY DOG SHOULD KNOW

1 This question is about the evolution, genetics, behaviour and physiology of cats.

FreeBonus: Teach your Cavalier King Charles Spaniel 13 Amazing Tricks!

Housetraining Drs. Foster & Smith Educational Staff

Transcription:

DOI 10.1007/s10071-008-0188-6 ORIGINAL PAPER The response of guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis Familiaris) to cues of human referential communication (pointing and gaze) Miriam Ittyerah Florence Gaunet Received: 7 November 2007 / Revised: 15 August 2008 / Accepted: 18 August 2008 Springer-Verlag 2008 Abstract The study raises the question of whether guide dogs and pet dogs are expected to diver in response to cues of referential communication given by their owners; especially since guide dogs grow up among sighted humans, and while living with their blind owners, they still have interactions with several sighted people. Guide dogs and pet dogs were required to respond to point, point and gaze, gaze and control cues of referential communication given by their owners. Results indicate that the two groups of dogs do not diver from each other, revealing that the visual status of the owner is not a factor in the use of cues of referential communication. Both groups of dogs have higher frequencies of performance and faster latencies for the point and the point and gaze cues as compared to gaze cue only. However, responses to control cues are below chance performance for the guide dogs, whereas the pet dogs perform at chance. The below chance performance of the guide dogs may be explained by a tendency among them to go and stand by the owner. The study indicates that both groups of dogs respond similarly in normal daily dyadic interaction with their owners and the lower comprehension of the human gaze may be a less salient cue among dogs in comparison to the pointing gesture. Keywords Referential communication Cues Dog-owner dyads Guide dogs Apprenticeship M. Ittyerah (&) Department of Psychology, Christ University, Bangalore 560029, India e-mail: miriam.ittyerah@christuniversity.in F. Gaunet Laboratoire Eco-Anthropologie et Ethnobiologie, Muséum National Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France Introduction Comparative research on responses to the pointing gesture and gaze at a desired target among animals has revealed a high variability in their comprehension of human-given cues (e.g. Povinelli et al. 1997). Recent experiments have shown that seals (Scheumann and Call 2004), dogs and cats (Miklósi et al. 2005), horses (Maros et al. 2008) and wolves (Virányi et al. 2008) are very skilful in trials with proximal dynamic pointing gestures in contrast to chimpanzees (Tomasello et al. 1997a, b; Bräuer et al. 2006). Over the past few years, it has been observed that dogs successfully use various nonverbal referential cues to localize an attractive object (favourite toy or food) previously hidden under one of two bowls (Hare and Tomasello 1999, 2005; Soproni et al. 2001, 2002; Miklósi and Soproni 2006). These studies show that dogs can represent and are able to decode the referential information provided by these cues. Various mechanisms have been deemed to be involved in the enactment of human like social skills in dogs (Miklósi et al. 2004; Hare et al. 2005; Hare and Tomasello 2005; Miklósi and Soproni 2006). In particular, living in a human environment may facilitate the development of such skills (Cooper et al. 2003). Since dogs learn to live, communicate and collaborate with humans their social skills may be subject to behavioural changes (Miklósi and Soproni 2006). Communicative behaviours can indeed be viewed as a kind of control system that allows groups to synchronize activities (Csányi 2000). For instance, Kerepesi et al. (2005) observed mutual dependency between dogs and humans during a tower building task as their behaviours become organized into highly complex interactive temporal patterns. Studies indicate that the main cues that are used above chance are the following: pointing at target (Hare et al. 1998; Miklósi et al. 1998; McKinley and Sambrook 2000;

Soproni et al. 2002), pointing and gaze (Hare and Tomasello 1999; Agnetta et al. 2000) and gaze with head and eye at target (Miklósi et al. 1998; Hare et al. 1998; McKinley and Sambrook 2000; Agnetta et al. 2000; Soproni et al. 2001). It is of interest to note that the use of most cues does not develop with age (Agnetta et al. 2000; Hare et al. 2002). Findings indicate inter-individual variability and suggest that speciwc instances of variability in the use of the eyes as a cue may need to be considered (McKinley and Sambrook 2000; Soproni et al. 2001). Furthermore, comparing dogs and apes in experiments where eye contact (accompanied by distal pointing) was obtained before pointing, dogs seem to be more eycient and there are suggestions that eye contact or especially gaze alternation increases the evect of the pointing gesture (Miklósi and Soproni 2006) and might facilitate the comprehension of deictic signals (Byrne 2003; Miklósi and Soproni 2006). Overall there are some indications that the eyes per se are used as cues while the exact conditions do not appear clearly. For this reason we choose to compare guide dogs of blind owners with pet dogs of sighted owners to determine whether dogs are able to respond to the eyeobject information of their owners (Heyes 1994). The aim of the present study is thus to determine if dogs are avected by the visual status of their owners, by comparing the responses of pet dogs of sighted owners and guide dogs of blind owners to human given cues. Evaluating the evect of enculturation in the domestic environment on social cognition among animals is diycult, however, some studies have been performed on single human reared apes (Bering 2004; Tomasello and Call 2004). Apprenticeship, a result of enculturation in apes is strongly linked to the rearing histories of the animals whether laboratory/zoo trained, nursery-raised or mother reared in captivity (Call and Tomasello 1996). Animals with close association with humans will have the opportunity to learn and respond to both intentional and unintentional behaviours, to predict future events and consequently develop complex social cognitive skills (Cooper et al. 2003). Because guide dogs do not live with their owners until they are 2 years of age, whereas pet dogs are exposed to their sighted owners since birth, the present study is designed to test for the evects of apprenticeship in both groups of dogs. Guide dogs appear to be the best cohorts to study the evects of apprenticeship among dogs (Gaunet 2008). Guide dogs and their blind owners have extensive interaction during free time navigation as they synchronize their actions with a change to-and-from the role of the initiator (Naderi et al. 2001). When dogs were prevented from accessing food that they could previously access, Gaunet (2008) observed that gazing at the food container and gazing at the owner, or gaze alternation between the container and the owner, or vocalisation and contact with the owner did not diver between the guide and pet groups, indicating no diverences between guide and pet dogs in the understanding of their owner s state of attention. However, guide dogs engaged in mouth licking more often than the pet dogs, indicating possibilities of incidental learning that may be deployed. Therefore, it is of interest in the present study to test the dyadic interaction between guide dogs of blind owners and pet dogs of sighted owners with referential cues of communication such as pointing at a target (arm and Wnger direction), gaze only (head and eye orientation) and pointing and gaze. The comparison of pet dogs and guide dogs tests whether human vision is a source of information during dyadic interaction. If, during the presentation of the gaze, the eyes or sight of the owner serve as a deictic cue, it may be hypothesized that the response to gaze will be worse in guide dogs than in pet dogs; pointing and gaze may be also avected in guide dogs. If the eyes or sight of the owner do not serve as a deictic cue during the presentation of the gestures, one would expect no diverences between the groups and the dogs may perform above chance for the pointing, the pointing and gaze and gaze cues. Method Subjects Two groups of 13 dogs matched for breed participated in the study along with their owners (see Tables 1, 2 for characteristics of the dogs): one was a group of guide dogs with their blind owners (mean age: 5.96 years/71.54 months; SD: 2.21 years/26.55 months), and the other was a group of pet dogs with their sighted owners (mean age: 6.33 years/ 76.0 months; SD: 1.76 years/21.1 months), t 24 = 0.48, P=0.63). The mean duration of co-habitation between pairs of dogs and owners is 4.27 years/51.23 months (SD: 2.32 years/27.78 months) for guide dogs and 5.24 years/ 62.92 months (SD: 2.01 years/24.33 months) for pet dogs, t 24 = 1.42, P=0.26. The guide dogs were raised in sighted families prior to their education and were trained at a school for 6 18 months. All the pet dogs spent, on average, 2 h a day interacting with their owners besides educational games. Both groups of dogs were fed a few hours before the experiment to ensure that they were not hungry. Experimental arrangements A carpeted room (8 m 5 m) was dedicated to the experiment. The room was novel to the dogs and their owners. The stimuli consisted of two toys (the favourite toy of the dog as indicated by the owner and a second, unfamiliar, object) and two identical cylindrical bowls (Xower pots) (30 30 30 cm), used to conceal the toys.

Table 1 Characteristics of the guide dogs Guide dogs Breed Age (in months) Gender Duration with owner (in months) 1 Labrador 84 M 66 2 Labrador 120 F 102 3 Golden retriever Labrador 120 F 96 4 Labrador 72 F 54 5 Golden retriever 48 M 24 6 Labrador 36 M 18 7 Golden retriever 42 M 12 8 Golden retriever 72 M 60 9 Hovawart 78 F 48 10 Flat coat 48 M 24 11 Golden retriever 66 M 48 12 White shepherd 60 F 48 13 Labrador 84 F 66 Table 2 Characteristics of the pet dogs Pet dogs Breed Age (in months) Gender Duration with owner (in months) 1 Labrador 78 M 76 2 Labrador 96 F 94 3 Golden retriever 72 F 69 4 Labrador 72 F 68 5 Golden retriever 84 F 82 6 Labrador 47 F 42 7 Labrador 96 M 93 8 Golden retriever 96 M 36 9 Labrador German shepherd 96 M 90 10 Labrador German shepherd 48 F 30 11 Golden Retriever 96 M 36 12 German Shepherd 72 M 69 13 Labrador 36 M 33 The instructor [experimenter (E1) or owner (O)] stood half a meter away from the centre of the two bowls. The bowls were 1.5 m apart. The dog stood at a distance of 2.5 m from the instructor, with the holder [either (O) or (E1)]. These distances were marked on the Xoor of the experimental room. A second experimenter (E2) helped with the dog. The same room was used for training the participants and conducting the experiment. However, to avoid conditioning, the experiment was performed in a diverent part of the room. A camera placed in a corner of the room, oriented toward the dog recorded the behaviour of the dog, the owner and the experimenter during all the trials. Procedure The experiment was conducted during daytime. The owners were informed about the two experimental phases (training and experiment) and were carefully trained just before each phase. Training phase The training phase was necessary to ensure that the dogs knew that the toy they had to retrieve was under one of the bowls. The training was performed with the instructor (E1) kneeling at the mid point between the two bowls. The instructor (E1) tried to make eye contact with the dog. If the dog did not pay attention within a few seconds, she called it by its name. While the dog was attentive, the instructor (E1) showed the dog the favourite toy and placed it under one of the bowls. The covering of the toy with the bowl was sounded so that the holder (O) (especially if blind) knew that she/he had to perform the next action: the O gently released the dog, and said search the ball or go to the

ball and let the dog approach and chose one of the bowls. If the dog chose the correct bowl, the favourite toy was given to her/him, and s/he received verbal praise or a pat by both the instructor and holder. If the dog made an incorrect choice, did not move, walked around or went to the instructor, the experimenter called the dog s attention and lifted the correct bowl and showed the dog the favourite toy. Toys were never hidden under the same bowl more than twice consecutively and the order varied with each dog. In total, each dog was required to perform at least eight errorless trials, four on each side, from a minimum of sixteen trials. Dogs that were not able to meet these criteria were excluded after a maximum number of twenty trials, ten on each side. Experimental phase When the training phase ended, the favourite toy and the unfamiliar object were both wrapped and rubbed together by the owner for a minute. The O took the role of the instructor and E1 took the role of the holder. Before the experiment was initiated, the dog remained with E1 in an adjacent room, while E2 carefully trained the instructor (O) with the procedure and the four gestures to be performed (see below). Then E1 returned to the experimental room while E2 remained with the dog outside the room in the corridor. E1 took the favourite toy and an unfamiliar toy and placed each of them under one of two bowls. The unfamiliar toy was immediately removed while the favourite toy remained under one of the two bowls. This was done to control for any noise or smell that may be generated by placing the favourite toy under only one of the bowls. At this time the O was informed about the gesture to be performed. When a ready signal was given, E1 brought the dog into the experimental room and instructed the dog to sit or stand in its place (2.5 m) before the instructor (O). The dog was held by E1, while E2 stood at the side behind E1 during the experimental trials. The instructor (O) had a relaxed standing posture facing the dog and kept her/his hands by her/his sides. S/he faced and looked at the dog and called it by its name. If the owner was blind, (E1) or (E2) would inform the instructor if the dog was inattentive. If the dog changed its line of view the owner called it again. While the dog was looking at her/ him, the instructor performed the predetermined gesture (see below). The time limit for the initiation of the response of the dog to the instructor s gesture was 30 s and the response time was noted by the second experimenter. If the dog failed to attend or if the instructor made an error, the trial was administered later. If the dog chose the correct bowl, it was given the toy and received a verbal reward. If the dog made an incorrect choice, did not move, walked around or went to the instructor, (E1) called the dog, lifted the correct bowl and showed the favourite toy to the dog. The predetermined gestures (cues) given by the instructor during the experiment were: Pointing: The common human pointing gesture was given with the arm and the index Wnger ipsilateral to the side of the baited bowl. The distance between the pointing Wnger and the bowl was approximately 60 cm. After the dog attended to the instructor, the instructor immediately pointed at the correct bowl and said search. The instructor remained facing the dog and continued to point until the dog performed the indicated task. Gaze: The instructor called the dog by its name. If the dog attended, the instructor immediately turned her/his head and eyes towards the desired bowl and said search. The head and eyes remained in this position until the dog responded. Pointing and gaze: After the dog attended to the instructor the pointing gesture, as described above, was immediately performed accompanied by the instructor s turning of the head and eyes towards the correct bowl, with the search instruction. This gesture was maintained until the dog performed the required task. Control: Control trials were performed along with the experimental trials. The control served to test how the dogs behaved in the absence of any instructor given cues. It also tested whether the dog responded to the odour of the baited bowl. After the dog attended to the instructor, the instructor bowed her/his head with closed eyes and said search. Each cue was presented ten times at random, Wve times using the left bowl and Wve times using the right bowl. Each dog received 40 cues from its owner in a random manner. The order of gestures and toy placements was randomised with the restriction that one side could not be used more than twice consecutively and that each combination of a side and gesture was not repeated immediately. On average each experiment lasted an hour. Rest pauses were allowed between trials. Scoring and data analysis Two guide dogs and four pet dogs did not pass the training phase and were eliminated from the study. If the dog took a period of 30 s or less to respond, the action was considered to be correct, provided the nose of the dog was not further than 15 cm from the bowl. If the dog went to the wrong bowl, or did not move, or walked about, or went and stood by the owner the trials were treated as errors. The performance of each dog was recorded as being one of Wve types of responses: going to the right bowl, the wrong bowl, no movement, going to and standing by the owner and walking around the experimental

area. Since there are 13 dogs in a group, there are 520 expected responses, 130 for each cue. The correct place was given a score of 1 and the wrong place and all other actions were scored as 0 and indicated by the performed action. The correct responses of both groups of dogs were analyzed for frequency and latency for each of the four gestures. Latencies in seconds for correct responses were recorded as the time period between the verbal instruction search given by the instructor and the dog s arrival at the correct bowl. The frequencies of the four incorrect responses were also analyzed. The experimenters analyzed the tapes with the correct and incorrect actions (100% behaviours) of all the 26 dogs for frequencies and latencies. Each experimenter analyzed 13 dogs independently. A third person that did not participate in the experiment independently checked the tapes and analyzed the responses (100% behaviours) of all the 26 dogs. The inter-rater reliability between the experimenters and the external rater was very high for frequencies, latencies and incorrect actions for both guide dogs (κ =1) and pet dogs (κ = 1) (Siegel and Castellan 1988). The criterion for the alpha level of signiwcance in the study is 0.05. Results Frequencies of correct responses The percentages of correct responses to point, point and gaze, gaze and control cues are presented in Fig. 1. Wilcoxon one sample tests for chance performance indicate that guide dogs perform above chance for point (n =13, T =0, P = 0.001), for point and gaze (n =13, T =0, P = 0.001), and for gaze (n =13, T =7, P = 0.007) and below chance for control trials (n =13, T =4, P = 0.009). Wilcoxon one sample tests indicate that pet dogs perform above chance for point (n =13, T =0, P = 0.001), for point and gaze (n =13, T =0, P = 0.001), Percentage of correct responses (%) 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Guide dogs Pet dogs Point Point & Gaze Gaze Control Fig. 1 Percentage of correct responses for the four gestures in the guide and pet dogs and for gaze (n =13, T =0, P = 0.002) and at chance for control trials (n =13, T =17.5, P =0.091). χ 2 tests calculated to test for performance diverences between the guide dogs and pet dogs in response to the gestures of point, point and gaze, gaze and control, indicate no diverences between the two groups of dogs (χ 2 = 0.304, df =3, P=0.95). There are no diverences in performance for cues presented to the bowls on the left or right side of the owner, for either the guide dogs (χ 2 =1.05, df =3, P=0.75) or the pet dogs (χ 2 =1.61, df =3, P=0.75). Friedman s ANOVA, computed to test for diverences between the cues, indicates that the responses to the cues diver from each other for the guide dogs (χ 2 = 29.43, df =3, P = 0.0001) as well as for the pet dogs (χ 2 = 33.52, df =3, P = 0.0001). Dunn s post hoc test (Dunn 1964) indicates that for guide dogs, the point and point and gaze cues do not diver, the point cue is better than the gaze cue (P < 0.05) and the control cue (P < 0.05). The point and gaze cue is also better than the gaze cue (P <0.05) followed by the control cue (P < 0.05). The gaze cue is more accurate than the control cue (P < 0.05). Among the pet dogs the four cues diver from each other. The point and gaze cue is more accurate than the point cue (P < 0.05), and both the point and point and gaze cues are more accurate than the gaze cue (P < 0.05), followed by the control cue (P < 0.05). Furthermore, the gaze cue is more accurate than the control cue (P < 0.05) among the pet dogs. Latencies for correct responses The latencies of correct responses to the gestures of point, point and gaze and gaze were analysed using Friedman s ANOVA. Since the guide dogs performed below chance for the control trials and the pet dogs are at chance performance, the latencies for the control trials of both groups of dogs were excluded from the analysis. Friedman s ANOVA indicates that the latencies of the guide and pet dogs do not diver for the point (χ 2 =1, df =1, P = 0.32), point and gaze (χ 2 = 1.5, df =1, P =0.22) and gaze (χ 2 = 0.615, df =1, P = 0.43) cues. However, the cues diver from each other (χ 2 = 10.71, df =2, P = 0.005) with longer latencies for gaze (mean = 4.27, SD = 2.07) when compared to point (mean = 2.96, SD = 1.72) and point and gaze (mean = 2.12, SD = 1.45). Dunn s post hoc test shows that the latency for gaze is longer (P < 0.05) than that of point and point and gaze. The latencies are presented in Fig. 2. Comparison of correct responses to the control gesture with incorrect response behaviours Since the guide dogs performed below chance to the control gesture of their owners, it is of interest to investigate the

Mean latencies (sec.) Fig. 2 Mean latencies for the three gestures in the guide and pet dogs possibility that the guide dogs were less inclined to choose one of the bowls in the control condition. For this the distinctions between the guide and pet dogs for the correct responses of the control gesture are compared with the incorrect response behaviours of no movement, go to owner and walk in the experimental room. This comparison is of interest because there is an absence of response to cues of referential communication in the control gesture as well as in the no response actions. Figure 3 presents these evects. Figure 3 compares the percentages of correct control responses and the grouped incorrect response actions in the guide and pet dogs. The frequencies of correct responses for the guide dogs in the control condition are 47 and the combination of no movement, go to owner and walk is 49. For the pet dogs, the frequencies of correct responses in the control condition are 52 and the combination of no movement, go to owner and walk are 31. Wilcoxon tests indicate that these diverences between the control conditions and the incorrect response actions are not signiwcant for either the guide (n = 13, T = 36.5, P = 0.84) or the pet dogs (n =13, T = 21.5, P = 0.09). The grouped incorrect response actions of no movement, go to owner and walk were also compared with the correct Percentage of responses 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 45 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 Guide dogs Pet dogs Point Point & Gaze Gaze Guide Pet Control Incorrect Fig. 3 Comparison of percentage of correct control responses and incorrect response actions (no movement, go to owner and walk) in the guide and pet dogs responses to point, point and gaze and gaze. A Wilcoxon test comparing the incorrect response actions and the correct responses in the experimental conditions indicate that the point cue is signiwcantly diverent from the incorrect actions in the guide dogs (n =13, T =9, P =0.01) and in the pet dogs (n = 13, T =0, P = 0.003). The incorrect response actions and the correct responses to the point and gaze cue also diver in the guide dogs (n =13, T =7, P = 0.01) and in the pet dogs (n =13, T =1.5, P =0.002). However, the correct response to gaze and the incorrect actions do not diver in the guide dogs (n = 13, T = 18.5, P = 0.06) whereas they diver for the pet dogs (n = 13, T =1.5, P = 0.003). Comparisons of the incorrect responses and the correct actions indicate that the main within group diverences between the guide and the pet dogs are in response to the gaze cue and the control gesture of the owner. The incorrect response actions and the response to gaze do not diver for the guide dogs as compared to the pet dogs indicating that the gaze gesture of the blind owner is relatively less comprehended than that of the other gestures. Likewise, guide dogs go to the owner more often in response to the control gesture (χ 2 =10.7, d.f. =3, P = 0.01) as compared to the cues of point, point and gaze and gaze. Discussion The study compared the responses of guide dogs of blind owners and pet dogs of sighted owners to cues of human referential communication to test for evects of apprenticeship in guide dogs with their blind owners. The results, in general, indicate that the two groups of dogs do not diver from each other in response to gestures of point, point and gaze, gaze and control cues. Although both groups of dogs performed above chance for point, point and gaze and gaze, guide dogs performed below chance to control gestures of the bowed head of the owner. The pet dogs were at chance performance for control trials. The results of the control condition for both groups suggest that dogs were not lead by cues of odour to the correct bowl. EVect of the visual status of the owner The Wnding that both groups of dogs do not diver for gestures of point, point and gaze and gaze indicates that there are no diverences in apprenticeship according to the visual status of the owners. This is in agreement with Gaunet s (2008) Wndings of no diverences between guide and pet dogs in the understanding of their owner s state of visual attention. Overall, the groups do not diver because guide dogs like pet dogs may be inxuenced by the visibility of sighted people about them, indicating that the comprehension

of deictic cues is not avected by the visual status of the owner. Rather, the ability to use referential cues is attuned with common human behaviour. The absence of diverences between the groups for the gaze cue may also suggest that dogs are more sensitive to the head direction rather than to the sight of the owner. Nevertheless, the lower success rates of the use of eyes as cues for referential communication in dogs cannot be ignored (Miklósi et al. 1998; McKinley and Sambrook 2000). These evects will be discussed with reference to the responses to gaze in the present study. Response to human gaze The response to gaze was comparatively lower than the point and the point and gaze cues in both groups of dogs indicating lesser comprehension of the gaze gesture produced by the owner. It is of relevance to note the signiwcantly lower success rates for cues that involve eye movement or gaze. For example in Miklósi et al. (1998) study, only one dog out of eleven learnt to use directional glancing. McKinley and Sambrook (2000) observed that only two dogs out of sixteen succeeded in the use of eye direction, however, Soproni et al. (2001) found that the ability to use eye direction as a cue developed across sessions. Although dogs are able to respond correctly to human gaze (Hare and Tomasello 1999), there are only a few instances of the use of eye direction for localizing a target (Miklósi et al. 1998; McKinley and Sambrook 2000; Soproni et al. 2001). In the present study we observed that the dogs could be avected by the head direction of the owner. Furthermore, in within group comparisons among guide dogs, responses to gaze as a cue are fewer when compared to point and point and gaze. However, this does not indicate that the blind owners do not gesture evectively, because blind people, even if early blind, gesture as they speak with both sighted and blind individuals (Goldin Meadow 1999). Bräuer et al. (2006) suggest that the relatively lower performance of dogs to the looking cue in comparison to pointing is probably because looking as a cue is not always used by humans for intentional communication, therefore making it diycult to diverentiate between looking (gaze) as a cue and arbitrary looking. Other studies indicate that of the two methods to test for the use of gaze, non human primates are able to follow gaze by looking in the same direction as the model or experimenter but Wnd it diycult to use gaze as a cue to a hidden object (Call and Tomasello 2004; Call et al. 2000). In contrast to primates, domestic dogs perform well in object choice paradigms (Miklósi et al. 1998; Soproni et al. 2002, 2001; Agnetta et al. 2000). According to Gomez (2005) the superior performance of the dogs could be attributed to their rearing by humans. Bräuer et al. (2006) have also indicated that domestication in dogs has inxuenced responsiveness to human given social cues as compared to chimpanzees. However, we are unaware of any studies that have compared the latency for the comprehension of the gaze gesture with that of pointing or pointing and gaze. The present Wndings indicate that not only the frequency of response to gaze is lower than that of pointing and point and gaze in both groups of dogs, but the latency of response to the gaze gesture of the owner is relatively longer in both groups of dogs, indicating uncertainty in the comprehension of the referential nature of gaze. However, studies that tested the sensitivity of dogs to the visual states of attention in humans (Cooper et al. 2003; Gácsi et al. 2004; Viranyi et al. 2004) have found that dogs are able to diverentiate between visual and non visual states of the experimenter. Overall there are some indications that the eyes per se are used as cues for sources of information. Furthermore, when comparing dogs with non human primates and children in tasks that use gaze as a cue to Wnd an object, dogs perform better than primates and are almost on a par with children (Miklósi et al. 2004), although children show spontaneous gaze following in early infancy (Itakura 2004). However, the present Wndings of both groups of dogs to the gaze cue suggest that dogs are less aware of the referential nature of the eyes of their owners as sources of communication. The results also suggest that, due to the lower comprehension of the gaze cue, gaze may be a less salient cue among dogs as compared to the pointing gesture. The pointing gesture The success rates of both groups of dogs were remarkably high for the point and point and gaze gestures. It is also noteworthy that these success rates in the dogs have been achieved with dynamic distal pointing and support the existing evidence of successful use of human pointing cues in dogs (Miklósi and Soproni 2006). The latencies for the point and point and gaze gestures did not diver and were distinctly faster than those for gaze, indicating good comprehension of the point gesture in both groups of dogs and cooperation in dyadic interaction with their owners (Hare 2001; Naderi et al. 2001). The evects of high levels of pointing comprehension in dogs have also been attributed to domestication and socialization (Hare and Tomasello 2005; Miklósi and Soproni 2006) and this suggests that dogs have evolved socio-cognitive abilities for living in human settings. Bräuer et al. (2006) have clariwed that dogs out perform chimpanzees in response to the pointing gesture because dogs are able to comprehend the communicative evects of the pointing gesture, whereas chimpanzees are guided by causal inferences. According to Hare and Tomasello (2004) apes succeed in understanding the goal directed actions of others, but this is not the same as reading communicative cues in which one

individual attempts to direct another s attention to some third entity in order to provide them with needed information. Therefore, the comprehension of the pointing gesture in dogs cannot be attributed to simple forms of learning (Shapiro et al. 2003), but rather seems to be enhanced by the communicative context with humans (e.g. Agnetta et al. 2000) and/or is a predisposition to engage in gazing contact with humans (Miklósi et al. 2003). The results also indicate that among pet dogs a combination of point and gaze is more facilitative as a cue than point only. This suggests that head direction accompanied by the pointing gesture increases accuracy. Gaze alone as a cue was least salient in both groups of dogs. However, there are suggestions that eye contact or gaze alternation may increase the evect of the pointing gesture or facilitate the development of dog-human communicative interactions (Byrne 2003; Miklósi and Soproni 2006). This indicates that dogs could be sensitive to the eyes of humans for establishing interaction (Call et al. 2003; Bräuer et al. 2004). It seems more pertinent from the present Wndings to conclude that the dogs were more attentive to the head direction than to the gaze of their owners because both groups of dogs have fewer responses to the gaze of their owners, though performance was better when gaze was accompanied by the point gesture. Further comparisons of the incorrect response actions of the dogs with the correct responses in the control condition indicate that, although there is a tendency amongst the guide dogs to go toward their owners more often and therefore have higher incorrect response actions, both groups of dogs do not diver in sensitivity to the cues of their owners. In conclusion, although guide dogs and pet dogs alike behave as evectively in normal daily dyadic interaction, the tendency among guide dogs to go to their owners more often in response to the lack of cue (control condition), suggests that this action is an outcome of their training to cooperate with their blind owners. Acknowledgments This work was supported by INSERM NORD- SUD France and conducted at the Laboratoire Eco-Anthropologie et Ethnobiologie, Muséum National Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France in an Indo-French research collaboration. We are especially grateful to the guide and pet dog-owner dyads for their interest and cooperation and to D. Sulinski for her assistance in the experiment and both P. Piwowar and D. Sulinski for their help in checking the data. The experiments complied with the current French laws governing animal research. References Agnetta B, Hare B, Tomasello M (2000) Cues to food locations that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of diverent ages do and do not use. Anim Cogn 3:107 112 Bering JM (2004) A critical review of the enculturation hypothesis : the evects of human rearing on great ape social cognition. Anim Cogn 7:201 213 Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M (2004) Visual perspective-taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers. Appl Anim Behav Sci 88:299 317 Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Riedel J, Call J, Tomasello M (2006) Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog causal ape. J Comp Psychol 120:38 47 Byrne RW (2003) Animal communication. What makes a dog able to understand its masters. Curr Biol 13:R347 R348 Call J, Tomasello M (1996) The evect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In: Russon AE, Bard KA, Parker ST (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 371 403 Call J, Tomasello M (2004) Social cognition. In: Maestripieri D (ed) Primate psychology. Harvard University Press, USA, pp 234 253 Call J, Agnetta B, Tomasello M (2000) Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to Wnd hidden objects. Anim Cogn 3:23 34 Call J, Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Tomasello M (2003) Domestic dogs are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. J Comp Psychol 117:257 263 Cooper JJ, Aston C, Bishop S, West R, Mills DS, Young RJ (2003) Clever hounds: social cognition in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). App Anim Behav Sci 81:229 244 Csányi V (2000) The human behaviour complex and the compulsion of communication: key factors in human evolution. Semiotica 128:45 60 Dunn OJ (1964) Multiple comparisons using ranks. Technometrics 6:241 252 Gácsi M, Miklósi Á, Varga O, Topál J, Csányi V (2004) Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human s attention. Anim Cogn 7:144 153 Gaunet F (2008) How do guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis familiaris) ask their owners for food? Anim Cogn 11(3):475 483 Goldin Meadow S (1999) The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends Cogn Sci 3:419 429 Gomez DC (2005) Species comparative studies and cognitive development. Trends Cogn Sci 9:118 125 Hare B (2001) Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? Anim Cogn 4:269 280 Hare B, Tomasello M (1999) Domestic dogs (Canis Familiaris) use human and conspeciwc social cues to locate hidden food. J Comp Psychol 113:173 177 Hare B, Tomasello M (2004) Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than cooperative cognitive tasks. Anim Behav 68:571 581 Hare B, Tomasello M (2005) Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends Cogn Sci 9:439 444 Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M (1998) Communication and food location between human and dogs (Canis familiaris). Evol Commun 2:137 159 Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634 1636 Hare B, Plyusnina I, Ignacio N, Schepina O, Stepika A, Wrangham R, Trut L (2005) Social cognitive evolution in captive foxed is a correlated by product of experimental domestication. Curr Biol 15:226 230 Heyes CM (1994) Cues, convergence and curmudgeon: a reply to Povinelli. Anim Behav 48:242 244 Itakura S (2004) Gaze following and joint visual attention in nonhuman animals. Jpn Psychol Res 46:216 226 Kerepesi A, Jonsson GK, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V, Magnusson MS (2005) Detection of long-term temporal patterns in doghuman interaction. J Behav Processes 70:69 79

Maros K, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á (2008) Comprehension of human pointing gestures in horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 11:3 467 466 McKinley J, Sambrook TD (2000) Use of human given cues by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 3:13 22 Miklósi Á, Soproni K (2006) A comparative analysis of animals understanding of the human pointing gesture. Anim Cogn 9:81 93 Miklósi Á, Polgárdi R, Topál J, Csányi V (1998) Use of experimenter given cues in dogs. Anim Cogn 1:113 121 Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Gasci M, Viranyi Z, Csányi V (2003) A simple reason for a big diverence: wolves do not look back at humans but dogs do. Curr Biol 13:763 766 Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2004) Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? Anim Behav 67:995 1004 Miklósi Á, Pongrácz P, Lakatos G, Topál J, Csányi V (2005) A comparative study of dog-human and cat-human interactions in communicative contexts. J Comp Psychol 119:179 186 Naderi A, Miklósi Á, Dóka A, Csányi V (2001) Co-operative interactions between blind persons and their dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 74:59 80 Povinelli DJ, Reaux JE, Bierschwale DT, Allain AD, Simon BB (1997) Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children, but not adolescent chimpanzees. Cogn Dev 12:423 461 Scheumann M, Call J (2004) The use of experimenter given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus). Anim Cogn 7:224 231 Shapiro AD, Janik VM, Slater PJB (2003) Gray seal (Halichoerus grypus) pup responses to experimental-given pointing and directional cues. J Comp Psychol 117:355 362 Siegel S, Castellan JN Jr (1988) Non parametric statistics for the behavioural sciences, 2nd edn. McGraw-Hill, New York Soproni K, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2001) Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris). J Comp Psychol 115:122 126 Soproni K, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2002) Dogs responsiveness to human pointing gestures. J Comp Psychol 116:27 34 Tomasello M, Call J (2004) The role of humans in the cognitive development of apes revisted. Anim Cogn 7:213 215 Tomasello M, Call J, Gluckman A (1997a) Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Dev 68:1067 1080 Tomasello M, Call J, Hare B (1997b) Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspeciwcs. Anim Behav 55:1063 1069 Viranyi Z, Topál J, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á, Csányi V (2004) Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans attentional focus. Behav Processes 66:161 172 Virányi Z, Gácsi M, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Belényi B, Ujfalussy D, Miklósi Á (2008) Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn 11(3):373 387