Predation risk, cover type, and group size in European rabbits in Donana (SW Spain)

Similar documents
Cover is safe during the day but dangerous at night: the use of vegetation by European wild rabbits

Activity pattern of the red fox Vulpes vulpes in Donana, SW Spain

Wild rabbit restocking for predator conservation in Spain

A NOTE ON THE MOVEMENTS OF A FREE-RANGING MALE DOMESTIC CAT IN SOUTHWESTERN SPAIN

The large-scale environment and the rabbit's genetic diversity as factors to bear in mind in Iberian lynx Conservation

Age estimation in Iberian wildcats Felis by canine tooth sections

Pygmy Rabbit (Brachylagus idahoensis)

Coyote (Canis latrans)

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Ecography. Supplementary material

ESRM 350 The Decline (and Fall?) of the White-tailed Jackrabbit

Reproduction and pre-dispersal survival of Iberian lynx in a subpopulation of the Doñana National Park

BOBWHITE QUAIL HABITAT EVALUATION

12 The Pest Status and Biology of the Red-billed Quelea in the Bergville-Winterton Area of South Africa

Managing Uplands with Keystone Species. The Case of the Gopher tortoise (Gopherus polyphemus)

NOTES 143. Observations of feeding groups in the Spanish lynx (Felis pardina) in the Donana National Park, SW Spain. ALDAMA and M.

rodent species in Australia to the fecal odor of various predators. Rattus fuscipes (bush

Gambel s Quail Callipepla gambelii

COMPETITION FOR FOOD IN URBAN PIGEONS: THE COST OF BEING JUVENILE

Food Item Use by Coyote Pups at Crab Orchard National Wildlife Refuge, Illinois

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

SKELETONS: Museum of Osteology Tooth and Eye Dentification Teacher Resource

Vigilance Behaviour in Barnacle Geese

Trends in Fisher Predation in California A focus on the SNAMP fisher project

Genetic Effects of Post-Plague Re-colonization in Black-Tailed Prairie Dogs

FALL 2015 BLACK-FOOTED FERRET SURVEY LOGAN COUNTY, KANSAS DAN MULHERN; U.S. FISH AND WILDLIFE SERVICE

THE EFFECT OF MAGPIE BREEDING DENSITY AND SYNCHRONY ON BROOD PARASITISM BY GREAT SPOTTED CUCKOOS

Raptor Ecology in the Thunder Basin of Northeast Wyoming

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

Rufous hare-wallaby Lagorchestes hirsutus

By Hans Frey ¹ ² & Alex Llopis ²

PROGRESS REPORT for COOPERATIVE BOBCAT RESEARCH PROJECT. Period Covered: 1 April 30 June Prepared by

Avayalik. An average migration lasted 23 days and birds traveled 3,106 km. Hunting. Nesting

Marsupial Mole. Notoryctes species. Amy Mutton Zoologist Species and Communities Branch Science and Conservation Division

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Chicken Farmers of Canada animal Care Program. Implementation guide

THE FOOD OF THE RED FOX (VULPES VULPES L) AND THE MARTEN (MARTES FOINA, ERXL) IN THE SPRING-SUMMER PERIOD IN OSOGOVO MOUNTAIN

Basin Wildlife. Giant Garter Snake

Predator-prey interactions in the spinifex grasslands of central Australia

Striped Skunk Updated: April 8, 2018

A change of diet from rodents to rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Is the wildcat (Felis silvestris) a specialist predator?

GREATER SAGE-GROUSE BROOD-REARING HABITAT MANIPULATION IN MOUNTAIN BIG SAGEBRUSH, USE OF TREATMENTS, AND REPRODUCTIVE ECOLOGY ON PARKER MOUNTAIN, UTAH

Feral Animals in Australia. An environmental education and sustainability resource kit for educators

Figure 4.4. Opposite page: The red fox (Vulpes vulpes) can climb trees. (Foto: F. Labhardt)

May Dear Blunt-nosed Leopard Lizard Surveyor,

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

Nature Quiz British Birds Birds of Prey

2014 BOBCAT MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES

Grey Fox. Urocyon cinereoargenteus

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Seasonal and sex-specific differences in feeding site attendance by red foxes Vulpes

Geoffroy s Cat: Biodiversity Research Project

Bobcat. Lynx Rufus. Other common names. Introduction. Physical Description and Anatomy. None

Family Soricidae Masked shrew Southeastern shrew (long-tailed shrews)

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1996) provides a comprehensive account.

Rabbits and hares (Lagomorpha)

Y Use of adaptive management to mitigate risk of predation for woodland caribou in north-central British Columbia

Ecology and Management of Ruffed Grouse and American Woodcock

Estimadores numericos y conductuales de depredaci6n sobre conejos en matorrales de tipo meditemineo: Un caso parad6jico

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

The Long-term Effect of Precipitation on the Breeding Success of Golden Eagles Aquila chrysaetos homeyeri in the Judean and Negev Deserts, Israel

Water vole survey on Laughton Level via Mill Farm

Demography and breeding success of Falklands skua at Sea Lion Island, Falkland Islands

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

Scaled Quail (Callipepla squamata)

Amphibians & reptiles. Key points

AN APPLIED CASE STUDY of the complexity of ecological systems and process: Why has Lyme disease become an epidemic in the northeastern U.S.

Antipredation role of clumped nesting by marsh-nesting red-winged blackbirds

ABSTRACT. Ashmore Reef

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

Snowshoe Hare. Lepus americanus. Other common names. Introduction. Physical Description and Anatomy. Snowshoe rabbit, varying hare, white rabbit

TEMPORAL AND SPATIAL DISTRIBUTION OF THE BLACK-LEGGED TICK, IXODES SCAPULARIS, IN TEXAS AND ITS ASSOCIATION WITH CLIMATE VARIATION

ECOLOGY OF ISOLATED INHABITING THE WILDCAT KNOLLS AND HORN

Gull Predation on Waterbird Nests and Chicks in the South San Francisco Bay

TURTLES DEMONSTRATE THE IDEAL FREE DISTRIBUTION BY DISTRIBUTING TO MAXIMIZE FOOD CONSUMPTION

Wild Fur Identification. an identification aid for Lynx species fur

FIELD GUIDE TO NORTH AMERICAN MAMMALS Bailey's Pocket Mouse (Chaetodipus baileyi)

Animal Instincts. Modified from a lesson found at

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

Animal behaviour (2016, 2) THE SPOTTED HYENA

Status and Management of Amphibians on Montana Rangelands

American badgers selectively excavate burrows in areas used by blackfooted ferrets: implications for predator avoidance

Iberian wild cat Felis silvestris tartessia prédation on rabbit Oryctolagus cuniculus: functional response and age selection

PRESSING ISSUES ACTION PLAN. Completed by Pressing Issues Working Group for the Idaho Bird Conservation Partnership September 2013

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

PREDATION ON RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD EGGS AND NESTLINGS

Donkeys Controlling Dogs

Nat Geo Notes for: How do Living Things Survive and Change?

Proponent: Switzerland, as Depositary Government, at the request of the Animals Committee (prepared by New Zealand)

Avian Ecology: Life History, Breeding Seasons, & Territories

In the News. Feral Hogs (Sus scrofa) in Texas. From the Field. What is in a name? 11/15/2013

Miguel Ferrer a a Estación Biológica de Doñana, CSIC, Avd. María Luisa,

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

Ovulation Synchrony as an Adaptive Response to Egg Cannibalism in a Seabird Colony

8 Fall 2014

Fisher. Martes pennanti

Soraya Wijntuin, CI-SURINAME

The grey partridges of Nine Wells: A five-year study of a square kilometre of arable land south of Addenbrooke s Hospital in Cambridge

Effects of prey availability and climate across a decade for a desert-dwelling, ectothermic mesopredator. R. Anderson Western Washington University

Transcription:

Acta Theriologica 42 (2): 225-230, 1997. PL ISSN 0001-7051 Predation risk, cover type, and group size in European rabbits in Donana (SW Spain) Rafael VILLAFUERTE and Sacramento MORENO Villafuerte R. and Moreno S. 1997. Predation risk, cover type, and group size in European rabbits in Dofiana (SW Spain). Acta Theriologica 42: 225-230. As European rabbits Oryctolagus cuniculus (Linnaeus, 1758) in SW Spain face high predation pressure, some antipredator tactics are expected. We examined rabbit group size in relation to the safety of the cover (sparse or dense), and the proximity to shrub cover. Stem density influenced the distance rabbits ventured from shrub; rabbits ventured greater distances when stem density in cover was higher. Rabbit group size was related to the distance to cover; solitary animals remained closer to protective cover, whereas rabbits in groups (> 3 animals) foraged further from cover. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that the formation of rabbit groups is related to defense against predation, and that group size is influenced by risk of perception. Estación Biológica de Doñana, CSIC. Apdo. 1056, Sevilla 41080, Spain; e-mail: villafuerte@cica.es Key words: Oryctolagus cuniculus, predation risk, group size, habitat structure, log- -linear models Introduction The European rabbit Oryctolagus cuniculus (Linnaeus, 1758) is native to Spain (Zeuner 1963). In many regions where they occur, they use proximity to cover as an antipredator strategy (Jaksic et al. 1979, Moreno et al. 1996). However, diferences in predator avoidance tactics have been observed between European rabbits that have been introduced to new regions and endemic populations (Jaksic and Soriguer 1981). In regions where the rabbit is not native (eg Australia and Chile), they may reach high densities in open habitats because of a lack of effective predators (Jaksic et al. 1979). However, Simonetti and Fuentes (1982) have shown that juvenile rabbits in Chile tend to use areas closer to shrub, which is a behaviour similar to adult rabbits in Spain. Rabbits must forage away from cover to obtain food, which results in a high risk of predation. It has been shown that rabbits in Spain forage in the open at night, because they are less at risk, while rabbits stay in cover in daylight as it is safer (Moreno et al. 1996). However, by living in groups, rabbits may reduce this risk because a foraging group can detect an approaching predator sooner [225]

226 R. Villafuerte and S. Moreno (Bertram 1978), and each member of a group is less at risk of being singled out during an attack (Vine 1971). If the distance of an individual rabbit from a refuge (eg shrub cover) can be considered as an index of risk (Jaksic et al. 1979, Jaksic and Soriguer 1981, and if individuals experience lower predation risk when in a group, then we can predict that single rabbits would stay closer to cover whereas groups of rabbits would be able to forage farther from cover. Nevertheless, most authors have used cover as a broad term which includes many types of refuge. As the perception of risk for a prey would be very different depending on the degree of obstruction that the cover presents to approaching predators, and the safety of the refuge it provides (eg Lazarus and Symonds 1992), the relationship between cover and precation risk is more complicated than it appears and needs further investigation. In this paper we test both, the relationships between rabbit group size and distance to cover, and the rabbits perception of risk in two different shrub types (dense and sparse), in a rabbit population in southwestern Spain. Methods The study was conducted in Donana National Park (southwestern Spain, ca 37 10 N, 6 23 W) that covers 550 km2 on the west bank of the mouth of the Guadalquivir River. The climate is Mediterranean, with hot dry summers and mild rainy winters. Three main biotopes exist: marshes, shrubland, and dunes, and rabbits are especially abundant in the shrub-marshland ecotone where they dig warrens in the shrub, and feed in the marsh (Beltran 1991, Moreno and Villafuerti 1995, Moreno et al. 1996). The marshland includes open grasslands with higher quality and more abandant forage than the shrubland (Lazo 1992). Two shrub types are easily distinguishable by height and degree of visual obstruction: (a) sparse- -shrubland, composed mainly of Cistus spp. and Halimium spp. and (b) dense-shrubland, conposed mainly of Erica spp. (Allier et al. 1974). Mean cover percentages for each shrub type a:e very different: 91% for dense and 50% for sparse shrub (Kufner and Moreno 1989). No differences existed in biomass or composition of grasses in the proximity of either shrub types (Lazo 1992). In Spain, rabbits are a major prey of 29 predators (Jaksic and Soriguer 1981). At Donana, it least 16 species prey upon rabbits (Moreno et al. 1996). Four carnivores: Iberian lynx Lynx pardinus, wildcat Felis silvestris, red fox Vulpes vulpes, and mongoose Herpestes ichneumon; and five lirds of prey: red kite Milvus miluus, black kite M. migrans, imperial eagle Aquila adalberti, bootei eagle Hieraetus pennatus, and marsh harrier Circus aeroginosus, include rabbits as more than 15% if their diet (M. Delibes, pers. com.). Observations of single individuals and rabbits in groups were conducted from a vehicle llong a permanent census course (about 5 km long) in the grasslands just at the border of the shrihlands, where the two cover types intermingle but are clearly delimited, so rabbits were easily assigned to each type of shrub. Monthly data were collected for three consecutive days one hour before dusk from January 1989 to December 1990. Warren density is low in the shrub along this transect becaise they are situated in sandy areas within the shrub. As a result, nearly all rabbits censused were adults, because young rabbits do not travel long distances from warrens (Kolb 1991). Rabbits were considered paired or in a group when the distance between them did not exceed 10 m. If any animal was further away when first sighted but at less than 25 m from its learest neighbour, it was not included. Only individuals or groups at least 25 m from each other were considered independent. Thus, rabbits were assigned to three group sizes (GS): solitary, piirs, or

Risk of predation in European rabbits 227 groups (> 3 rabbits). Distance of a rabbit from cover (DC) and cover type (CT), ie dense or sparse, were also recorded. Distance to shrub for a group was assigned as the mean distance of all the individuals in the group, assigned to 3 distance categories: short (0-2 m), medium (2-5 m), and long (> 5 m). Using the frequency of sighting of rabbit groups, a three dimensional table (GS x CT x DC) was built. Data analysis was done using BMDP-4F (Dixon 1983). Partial and marginal associations among these variables were tested using G-tests. A log-linear model was built considering only those interactions showing both types of association as being significant, and the expected values generated by the model (lambda) were compared with the table of observed values by a G-test (Heisey 1985). Standardized values of the log-linear parameters of the model were used to test the deviations of predicted frequencies from randomness (Edwards 1989). Results In total, 2 331 rabbit groups were observed (Table 1): 1895 solitary individuals, 337 pairs and 99 rabbit groups (with mean size 4, range 3-20). We found significant associations between group size and the distance to cover (partial association: G2 = 11.26, df = 2, p < 0.01; marginal association: G2 = 11.77, df = 2, o p < 0.01), and between shrub type and distance (partial association: G = 24.37, df = 4, p < 0.001; marginal association: G2 = 24.88, df = 4, p < 0.001). No relation was found between the size of a rabbit group and the shrub type (partial association: G2 = 4.55, df = 2, p > 0.05; marginal association: G2 = 5.07, df = 2, p > 0.05). Second order interactions among variables were not significant. The log-linear model, taking into consideration only those significant interactions (GS x DC, CT x DC), predicted a table of values that did not differ from the observed distribution (G2 = 10.72, df = 6, p > 0.05). The analysis of the log-linear parameters showed that solitary rabbits were found more frequently closer to and less frequently further from shrub than Table 1. Number of rabbit groups observed, classified according to their distance to cover and shrub type. Group size Cover type Distance to cover Short (0-2 m) Medium (2-5 m) Long (> 5 m) Solitary Sparse 611 407 174 Dense 330 284 89 Total 941 691 263 Pairs Sparse 94 85 32 Dense 35 66 25 Total 129 151 57 Groups Sparse 18 22 11 Dense 15 21 12 Total 33 43 23 Total 1103 885 343

228 R. Villafuerte and S. Moreno P airs Distance Distance Distance Fig. 1. Estimated values of the log-linear parameters (Lambda) and their standard errors, for the interaction between group size and rabbit-to-cover distance. Sign of the parameters indicates a greater (positive) or lower (negative) tendency of occurrence than random. (*** - p < 0.001, ** - p < 0.01). 0.3 0.2 Sparse shrub CD 0.1 "C3 -o S o CD -J - 0.1-0.2-0.3 Dense shrub Short Medium Long Distance Fig. 2. Estimated values of the log-linear parameters (Lambda) and their standard errors, for the interaction between rabbit-to-cover distance and cover-type. The direction o f the bar in dicates if the tendency is favourable to sparse (positive) or dense (negative) cover. (*** - p < 0.001, ** - p < 0.01). expected at random, and that rabbits in groups were found further from shrub than expected (Fig. 1). No tendencies were found in space use by rabbits observed in pairs. Rabbits remained nearer cover when the closer shrub type was sparse than when cover was dense, when they favoured the middle distances (Fig. 2). There were no significant effects of cover type on the frequency of long-distance observations. Discussion Grouping and use of space by rabbits is strongly influenced by sex, age, social rank, time of the day, and their role as prey (Cowan 1987, Jaksic et al. 1979, Jaksic and Soriguer 1981, Simonetti and Fuentes 1982, Moreno et al. 1996). Our data show that European rabbits respond to predation risk by remaining closer to cover when alone than when in groups. Caraco et al. (1980a), also showed that

Risk of predation in European rabbits 229 yellow-eyed juncos Junco phaeonotus formed larger groups when further away from cover, and these same authors (Caraco et al. 1980b) also demonstrated this grouping behaviour to be affected by the presence of a predator. By living in groups, rabbits accrue substantial benefits, such as avoiding heat loss (through huddling in the warren), cooperative maintenance, and the construction of safety protection (eg Cowan 1987). For many species, two clear benefits of grouping are the risk dilution effect (eg Vine 1971) and the reduction in individual vigilance in its component members (eg Hoogland 1979, Lagory 1986). As indicated by Lagory (1986), alertness is positively related to vulnerability because natural selection is expected to favor an optimal (minimum necessary) level of vigilance. In finches, escape tactics depend on the perception of the safety value of the space used (Lima 1990, Lima et al. 1987). In our study of rabbits, refuge type does not seem to affect grouping behaviour, but does influence how far the animal can venture into the open. So, when the refuge provides high protection, as happens in the dense-shrubland, rabbits take greater risks by venturing further away from cover. In contrast, in poor refuge areas, like sparse-shrubland, rabbits stay close to cover. But shrub may also provide danger for rabbits. In dense cover it would be more difficult for rabbits to detect a carnivore coming, thus rabbits would move away from thiker cover to have more time to scape. Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that in southern Spain, at least, the formation of rabbit groups is related to defense against predation. Although our data do not indicate that group size is influenced by cover type, they demonstrate that group size is influenced by perception of risk and enables animals to exploit food further from cover. This behaviour can be considered as an adaptation in rabbits to maximise the rate of energy acquisition while minimizing predation risk. Aknowledgements: We thank J. E. Fa, F. Alvarez, D. F. Smith and specially to J. A. Litvaitis, C. FitzGibbon, A. Lazo, J. F. Beltran, and G. Jordán for their helpful comments. This paper is a contribution to the projects PB94-0480 (DGICYT). References Allier C., González-Bernáldez F., Ramírez-Díaz L. 1974. Mapa ecológico. Reserva Biológica de Doñana. División de Ciencias del CSIC Estación Biológica de Doñana, Sevilla: 1-11. Beltrán J. F. 1991. Temporal abundance pattern of the wild rabbit in Doñana, SW Spain. Mammalia 55: 591-599. Bertram B. 1978. Living in groups: predators and prey. [In: Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies, eds]. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford: 64-97. Caraco T., Martindale S. and Pulliam H. R. 1980a. Avian time budgets and the distance to cover. Auk 97: 872-875. Caraco T., Martindale S. and Pulliam H. R. 1980b. Avian flocking in the presence of a predator. Nature (London) 285: 400-401. Cowan D. P. 1987. Group living in the European rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus): mutual benefit or resource localization? Journal of Animal Ecology 56: 779-795. Dixon W. J. 1983. B.M.D.P. Statistical Software. University of California Press, Berkeley: 1-733.

230 R. Villafuerte and S. Moreno Edwards T. C. 1989. The ontogeny of diet selection in feeding ospreys. Ecology 70: 881-896. Heisey D. M. 1985. Analyzing selection experiments with log-linear models. Ecology 66: 1774-1748. Hoogland J. L. 1979. The effect of colony size on individual alertness of prairie dogs (Sciuridae: Cynomys spp.). Animal Behaviour 27: 394-407. Jaksic F. M., Fuentes E. R. and Yáñez J. L. 1979. Spatial distribution of the old world rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) in central Chile. Journal of Mammalogy 60: 207-209. Jaksic F. M. and Soriguer R. C. 1981. Predation upon European rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) in Mediterranean habitats of Chile and Spain: a comparative analysis. Journal of Animal Ecology 50: 269-281. Kolb H. H. 1991. Use of burrows and movements of wild rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) in an area of hill grazing and forestry. Journal of Applied Ecology 28: 892-905. Kufner M. B. and Moreno S. 1989. Abundancia y amplitud de los desplazamientos de Apodemus sylvatius en cuatro biotopos de Doñana que difieren en cobertura vegetal. Doñana Acta Vertebrata 16: 179-181. Lagory K. E. 1986. Habitat, group size, and the behaviour of white-tailed deer. Behaviour 98: 168-179. Lazarus J. and Symonds M. 1992. Constrasting effects of protective and obstructive cover on avian vigilance. Animal Behaviour 43: 519-521. Lazo A. 1992. Socioecologia del ganado bovino asilvestrado de la Reserva Biológica de Doñana. Unpublished Ph D thesis, University of Sevilla, Sevilla: 1-371. Lima S. L. 1990. Protective cover and the use of space: different strategies in finches. Oikos 58: 151-158. Lima S. L., Wiebe K. L. and Dill L. M. 1987. Protective cover and the use of space by finches: is closer better? Oikos 50: 225-230. Moreno S. and Villafuerte R. 1995. Traditional management of scrubland for the conservation of rabbits Oryctolagus cuniculus and their predators in Doñana National Park, Spain. Biological Conservation 73: 81-85. Moreno S., Villafuerte R. and Delibes M. 1996. Cover is safe during the day but dangerous at night: the use of vegetation by European wild rabbits. Canadian Journal of Zoology 74: 1656-1660. Simonetti J. A. and Fuentes E. R. 1982. Microhabitat use by European rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) in central Chile: are adult and juvenile patterns the same? Oecologia (Berlin) 54: 55-57. Vine I. 1971. Risk of visual detection and pursuit by a predator and the selective advantage of flocking behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology 30: 405-422. Zeuner F. E. 1963. A history of domesticated animals. Hutchinson, London: 1-560. Received 16 August 1996, accepted 13 January 1997.