Greenland halibut observed by video in front of survey trawl: behaviour, escapement, and spatial pattern

Similar documents
FEATURES OF DISTRIBUTION OF LOADING IN COD-END OF TRAWL OF A VARIOUS DESIGN

Serial No. N5461 NAFO SCR Doc. 07/75 NAFO/ICES WGPAND MEETING OCTOBER/NOVEMBER 2007

INTERACTIONS BETWEEN COD (Gadus morhua) AND DEEP SEA SHRIMP (Pancialus borealis) IN THE BARENTS SEA. Knut Korsbrekke, Sigbj0rn Mehl and Arvid Hylen

Performance of the Campelen 1800 Shrimp Trawl During the 1995 Northwest Atlantic Fisheries Centre Autumn Groundfish Survey

Serial No. N6570 NAFO SCR Doc. 16/027 SCIENTIFIC COUNCIL MEETING JUNE 2016

2008/048 Reducing Dolphin Bycatch in the Pilbara Finfish Trawl Fishery

Shrimp (Pandalus borealis) populations of Isfjorden and Kongsfjorden:

Efficiency of the Korean Bottom Survey Trawl for Snow Crab Chionoecetes opilio

DEEP SEA TD/RES 113 SOUTHEAST ASIAN

Pelagic Fish Behaviour During Trawl Sampling Off Angola

CM 2004/ R:23. Can subjective evaluation of echograms improve correlation between bottom trawl and acoustic densities?

from an experimental bag net SHIODE, DAISUKE; TAKAHASHI, MUTSUKI Proceedings of the 6th Internationa SEASTAR2000 workshop) (2011): 31-34

Simrad ITI Trawl monitoring system

Trawls - Design, Construction and Methods

Guidelines to Reduce Sea Turtle Mortality in Fishing Operations

GRID DEVICES TO SELECT SHRIMP SIZE IN TRAWLS

Introduction. Trawl Gear description (fish & shrimp) Introduction. Introduction 4/4/2011. Fish & invertebrates

Are my trawl wires marked correctly? Is my trawl spread optimally? Is the trawl on bottom?

Trawl Gear description (fish & shrimp)

REGULATIONS RELATED TO TRAWL GEAR CONFIGURATION, GEARS ALLOWED ON BOARD, AND AREA OF USE

ICES Journal of Marine Science

Flume Tank Testing of a 1:10 Scale Model of the Fjardanet/Jackson T90 Trawl

Design and Test of a Kite-assisted Shrimp Codend to Reduce Small Shrimps and Juvenile Fish Bycatch

DIGITUS Network Cabinet Unique Series, 600, 800 mm width - 600, 800, 1000, 1200 mm depth

BBRG-5. SCTB15 Working Paper. Jeffrey J. Polovina 1, Evan Howell 2, Denise M. Parker 2, and George H. Balazs 2

Response to SERO sea turtle density analysis from 2007 aerial surveys of the eastern Gulf of Mexico: June 9, 2009

Dive-depth distribution of. coriacea), loggerhead (Carretta carretta), olive ridley (Lepidochelys olivacea), and

Modeling and Control of Trawl Systems

Southern Shrimp Alliance, Inc P.O. Box 1577 Tarpon Springs, FL Ph Fx

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

Portside Sampling and River Herring Bycatch Avoidance in the Atlantic Herring and Mackerel Fishery

Representation, Visualization and Querying of Sea Turtle Migrations Using the MLPQ Constraint Database System

Developing a Salmon Excluder for the Pollock Fishery

Southeast U.S. Fisheries Bycatch Reduction Technology. John Mitchell NOAA Fisheries Southeast Fisheries Science Center Harvesting Systems Unit

FIFTH REGULAR SESSION 8-12 December 2008 Busan, Korea CONSERVATION AND MANAGEMENT OF SEA TURTLES Conservation and Management Measure

Mississippi Shrimp Summary Action Plan Marine Advancement Plan (MAP)

Exceptions to prohibitions relating to sea turtles.

Yonat Swimmer, Richard Brill, Lianne Mailloux University of Hawaii VIMS-NMFS

Alabama Shrimp Summary Action Plan Marine Advancement Plan (MAP)

Legal Supplement Part B Vol. 53, No th March, NOTICE THE ENVIRONMENTALLY SENSITIVE SPECIES (OLIVE RIDLEY TURTLE) NOTICE, 2014

Recognizing that the government of Mexico lists the loggerhead as in danger of extinction ; and

Annual Pink Shrimp Review

Reduction of sea turtle mortality in the professional fishing

PLL vs Sea Turtle. ACTIVITIES Fishing Trials. ACTIVITIES Promotion/WS

The Effect of Phase Shifts in the Day-Night Cycle on Pigeon Homing at Distances of Less than One Mile

STATE OF CALIFORNIA FISH AND GAME COMMISSION FINAL STATEMENT OF REASONS FOR REGULATORY ACTION

Size structure, distribution and interaction characteristics of dominant jellyfish from surface trawls in the Eastern Bering Sea

Dugong movements Current knowledge and tracking tools

Endangered Species Origami

ENGINEERING TEST SPECIFICATION

2011 Winner: Yamazaki Double-Weight Branchline

Field report to Belize Marine Program, Wildlife Conservation Society

Nautilus Behavior in Aquaria

Legal Supplement Part B Vol. 53, No th March, NOTICE THE ENVIRONMENTALLY SENSITIVE SPECIES (GREEN TURTLE) NOTICE, 2014

YOKOTA, KOSUKE; MINAMI, HIROSHI; NO TAKAHIRO. Proceedings of the 3rd Internationa. SEASTAR2000 workshop) (2006):

SCIENTIFIC COMMITTEE FIFTH REGULAR SESSION August 2009 Port Vila, Vanuatu

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

NARWHALS. The decrease of the Monodon monoceros population. By Caitlin Seppi

Snapshot Day 10/7/08 Data (Salt Front RM 63.4) RIVER MILE 25W Piermont Pier Lamont-Doherty Earth Observatory

The role of catch shares in Pacific halibut bycatch reduction in the U.S. West Coast bottom trawl fishery

People around the world should be striving to preserve a healthy environment for both humans and

Assessment of cryptic seabird mortality due to trawl warps and longlines Final Report: INT Johanna Pierre Yvan Richard Edward Abraham

Writing Simple Procedures Drawing a Pentagon Copying a Procedure Commanding PenUp and PenDown Drawing a Broken Line...

Grade: 8. Author: Hope Phillips

Title Temperature among Juvenile Green Se.

Estimation of pelagic trawl efficiency in a combined acoustictrawl. survey, with reference to demersal fish spatial distribution.

Sustainable management of bycatch in Latin America and Caribbean trawl fisheries REBYC-II LAC. Revised edition

IUCN Red List. Industry guidance note. March 2010

PERCEPTION OF OCEAN WAVE DIRECTION BY SEA TURTLES

Proceedings of the International Sy. SEASTAR2000 Workshop) (2004):

Plating the PANAMAs of the Fourth Panama Carmine Narrow-Bar Stamps of the C.Z. Third Series

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Basket Stars. Copyright 2008 LessonSnips

Harry s Science Investigation 2014

Short Report Key-site monitoring on Hornøya in Rob Barrett & Kjell Einar Erikstad

Optoacoustic imaging of an animal model of prostate cancer

Dr Kathy Slater, Operation Wallacea

Trapped in a Sea Turtle Nest

SEDAR31-DW30: Shrimp Fishery Bycatch Estimates for Gulf of Mexico Red Snapper, Brian Linton SEDAR-PW6-RD17. 1 May 2014

Volusia County Lighting Ordinance

National Geographic. Young Explorer. September issue 2014

Reversing Category Exclusivities in Infant Perceptual Categorization: Simulations and Data

Spatial distribution and larval biology of Spirobranchus giganteus

ROUGH TERRAIN CRANE GR-120NL GR-120N

Let s begin by learning a little more about rays, in general. First, an anatomy lesson!

Serial No. N5748 NAFO SCR Doc. 10/2 SCIENTIFIC COUNCIL MEETING JUNE 2010

Development of a GIS as a Management Tool to Reduce Sea Turtle Bycatch in U.S. Atlantic Ocean and Gulf of Mexico Fisheries

RELATIVE ABUNDANCE OF JUVENILE SMALL TOOTH SAWFISH

BEHAVIOUR OF DOGS DURING OLFACTORY TRACKING

Evaluation of large-scale baiting programs more surprises from Central West Queensland

Shrimp Trawl Bycatch Reduction. Dan Foster NOAA Fisheries Service Harvesting Systems and Engineering Division

New SS4 Door Distance sensors

A CITIZEN S GUIDE TO IDENTIFYING AND CORRECTING PROBLEM LIGHTS ADJACENT TO SEA TURTLE NESTING BEACHES

Global Perspectives on Fisheries Bycatch: The Legacy of Lee Alverson

1) Calculate the percentages of shrimp infected with black gill for each month in 2004 and Round to the nearest whole number (15 pts total).

Bear Awareness Training

Marine Debris and its effects on Sea Turtles

Call of the Wild. Investigating Predator/Prey Relationships

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International Advanced Subsidiary and Advanced Level

SOAR Research Proposal Summer How do sand boas capture prey they can t see?

Transcription:

In Collaboration with the Netherlands Institute for Sea Research ELSEVIER Journal of Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 JOURNAL OF SEARESEARCH www.elsevier.com/locate/seares Greenland halibut observed by video in front of survey trawl: behaviour, escapement, and spatial pattern O.T. Alberta *, A. Harbitza, Á.S. H0inesb a Institute o f Marine Research, Tromso branch, Tromso, Norway b Institute o f Marine Research, Bergen, Norway Received 15 October 2002; accepted 15 February 2003 Abstract Video recordings of Greenland halibut (Reinhardtius hippoglossoides) were made at eight trawl stations in Svalbard waters in August 2002. The recordings were made down to 600 m depth using artificial light. A method for calculating actual fish length from the video image was established and the recordings were analysed with respect to length-dependent behaviour, escapement and spatial pattem. All Greenland halibut observed were either lying on the bottom or swimming in a horizontal position close to the bottom, and there was no tendency to schooling. Individual fish reacted in an ordered way to the approaching trawl and were herded along the ends of the ground-gear. Escapement under the ground-gear was higher for smaller fish, while some larger individuals were apparently able to escape the trawl ahead of the observed region. 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Greenland halibut; Trawl video; Behaviour; Escapement; Catchability 1. Introduction Fish behaviour in relation to sampling gear has long been anticipated as an important factor for inferences in resource biology and stock assessments (Fréon et al., 1993). For Greenland halibut (Reinhardtius hippoglossoides) inferences about population structure (length and age composition), biological rates (growth and maturation) and spatial dynamics (distribution and migration) all rely heavily on data from research surveys using some sort of sampling trawl. The behaviour of fish towards an approaching * Corresponding author. E-mail address: ole.thomas.albert@imr.no (O.T. Albert). trawl has attracted much research during the last few decades (Wardle and Hollingworth, 1993; Femö and Olsen, 1994; Fréon and Misund, 1999). However, most of this research has been on roundfish, both demersal and pelagic. Studies on flatfish behaviour in front of the trawl have mainly been done in shallow water and mostly during day (Main and Sangster, 1981; Walsh and Hickey, 1993; Bublitz, 1996). These results may not be directly relevant for Greenland halibut living at low light levels below 500 m depth. This paper is part of a comprehensive effort to study distribution and migration of Greenland halibut in the Northeast Arctic region, i.e. the Norwegian Sea, Barents Sea and Svalbard area, throughout the year and during ontogeny. The objective of this paper is to describe behaviour in front of the trawl, with empha 1385-1101/$ - see front matter 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved, doi: 10.1016/S 1385-1101(03)00063-7

118 O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 sis on the extent of schooling behaviour, herding effects and escapement under the ground-gear with respect to fish size. 2. Material and methods 2.1. Recording and sampling at sea Video recordings were made in the trawl opening during a bottom trawl survey with RV Jan Mayen west of Svalbard in August 2002. The hauls were made using Campelen 1800 with Rockhopper ground-gear (Engás and Godo, 1989), a modified shrimp trawl used as the standard Norwegian survey trawl. Towing speed was 1.5ms 1 (3 knots). The video recorder was mounted on the headline and directed towards the mid-section of the ground-gear. Artificial light was mounted in the net roof, 2 m behind the camera. Eight recordings were made with identical settings. Table 1 lists position, depth and catch from the eight stations and Fig. 1 (upper panel) gives a schematic description of the set-up. The camera used was a SIMRAD osprey oel324 s.i.t. camera with a ± 30 degrees horizontal opening angle in water. The light source was 9W fluorescent lamp of type Ralux /E with average luminance 2,8 cd cm ~2. Although Greenland halibut abundance in these areas peaks at depths between 600 and 700 m, the recordings were limited by the video equipment, which was not certified for depth greater than 600 m. The observed behaviour patterns may thus not be Table 1 Position, distance trawled and catch of Greenland halibut in numbers, for each trawl station where video recordings were made Station no Latitude Longitude Depth (m) Distance (m) Catch (n) 1 80 03' 17 16' 378 2700 21 S 2 78 36' 8 59' 600 2500 23 S 3 78 38' 9»20' 575 3200 36-4 80 27' 16 05' 435 3000 Catch - lost 5 80 12' 10 04' 578 2500 13 S 6 78 32' 9 10' 580 2200 5 7 78 11' 9 18' 589 2000 13 S 8 79 53' 8 15' 595 2600 14 Length measured.g 1 X 2,3) (3,3 Fig. 1. Upper panel: Rigging of the gear. Schematic drawing of position and observation window of the video recorder and light source. Lower panel: Example of video image with (x,y) coordinate system for manual recording of image positions. representative for areas with higher abundance. In addition to video recordings, each trawl catch of Greenland halibut was also retained and analysed for length composition. The mean length was 40 cm, which is slightly (5 cm) below that of trawl catches from a broader depth range in the same area (Albert, 2003). 2.2. Quan tification and classification o f behaviour The videos were projected on a screen and the behaviour of Greenland halibut in front of the trawl was analysed. The fish observed were classified as certain Greenland halibut, certain flatfish and uncertain flatfish. Only fish of the first category were used further in the analyses. Time of first detection, when crossing the ground-gear and when out of sight were recorded for each individual. Behavioiu was classified,2) 2,2) (3,2 ( - a t ^ (-1.1) (0.1) 2,1) ^ (3,1 (-3,0) (-2,0) (-1,0) (-3,-1) (-2,-1) (-1.-1U (0,-1) (1,-1) (2,-1) (3,-1) (-3,-2) (-2,-2) (-1,-2) (0,-2) (1,-2) (2,-2) (3,-2) (-3,-3) (-2,-3) (-1,-3) (0,-3) (1,-3) (2,-3) (3,-3) 3

O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 119 within four phases: At first detection, between first detection and crossing of the ground-gear, when crossing the ground-gear, and after having crossed the ground-gear. Behaviour at first detection of a specimen was classified with respect to vertical position in the water, transverse (left-right) position on the screen, and horizontal orientation. Foiu different categories were used for vertical position in the water: (1) Resting on the bottom substrate, (2) Swimming close to the bottom (not above 35 cm off bottom, i.e. within the height of the rubber-discs of the Rockhopper ground-gear), (3) Swimming between 0.35 and approx. 1 m off the bottom and (4) Swimming more than 1 m off the bottom. The transverse position on the screen was recorded by dividing the screen from left to right in 7 adjacent vertical bins with equal width, coded by integer numbers from 3 to 3, with 0 corresponding to the central vertical bin (see Fig. 1, lower panel). These figures were in the later analysis divided by 3.5 in order to obtain standardised measiues ranging from 1 to 1. In addition, the orientation of the fish on the screen was recorded using a reference compass card with North pointing in the vmi-dircction, corresponding to the direction into the trawl opening (see Fig. 1). For fish that took off from the bottom, orientation at take-off was used instead of orientation at first detection. After first detection the fish was traced and classified into eight different behavioiu categories (Table 2) with possibility to give more than one category to each observation. For fish that left the bottom, the along-trawl distance from the groundgear at take-off was measiued on the screen and the position of the fish at take-off was recorded using a scale from 3 to + 3 in the x- and y- directions (Fig. 1, lower panel). When the fish crossed the ground-gear, registrations of the screen position and orientation were recorded the same way as when first detected. Contact with the ground-gear or trawl opening was classified into seven different categories (Table 2). After crossing, the fate of the fish was classified to caught, lost or uncertain. If possible, also the length of the fish was measiued on the screen. This was only done for individuals that were seen in a horizontal position at or close Table 2 Codes used to classify type of behaviour after first detection, and contact with the ground-gear or trawl opening when entering trawl Code Behaviour observed Contact with after first detection ground-gear of the fish or trawl opening 1 Take-off from bottom Moving in above the ground-gear and further back in trawl 2 Swimming in front Moving in above the of the ground-gear ground-gear, keep at the bottom position and coming back 3 Swimming with the Hit by the ground-gear ground-gear, between 0.35 and 1 m above the bottom 4 Swimming higher up Overrun by the in the water ground-gear 5 Swimming towards Swimming actively the trawl opening towards the ground-gear (between the discs or under the ground-gear) 6 Swimming across Moving in through the trawl direction ground-gear and coming back through the ground-gear 7 Disappearing from Bend towards the roof video image at the of the trawl (turning, sides upward orientation) 8 Disappearing from video image ahead of the ground-gear to the bottom. Also the screen position and orientation of the fish when measiued were recorded in order to calculate the real length from the length measiued on the screen (see below). For each fish observed, the distance across the foiu central rubber-discs of the Rockhopper ground-gear was also measiued as a reference length. The spatial pattem of Greenland halibut along the trawl path was analysed using distance between succeeding individuals on the video recording. This inter-fish distance is not only a function of the spatial pattern per se (i.e. regular, random, or clumped distribution), but also by the fish density on the locality (i.e. number caught per nautical mile). For each trawl haul the inter-fish distances were therefore standardised by dividing with the mean inter-fish distance.

120 O.T. Albert et al. / Journal of Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 2.3. Estimation o f fish length from video Let.Tim and i'm, denote the normalised co-ordinates of the horizontal and vertical positions in the video frame, i.e. ranging from 1 to 1. The correspondence between these image co-ordinates and the co-ordinates X, y and z in the water (see Fig. 1) can then be deduced by translation and rotation of co-ordinate systems (Harbitz, 1990) Y = 0-8 -Yi,n tan (eft) ' sin(0) cos(0) yim tan((/>) K ; = cos(d) + sin(o) yim tan(</>) ^ sin(0) cos(0) y im tan((ƒ>) where r j 30 degrees is (half) the horizontal opening angle of the camera, corresponding to technical specifications, and 0 70 degrees is the tilt angle between the horizontal plane and the main camera direction. The factor 0.8 in the x-formula is due to the difference between the width and the height of the screen. A more general and rigorous work on perspective transformations is given by Haralick (1980). The vertical distance, /?, between the camera (lens) and the seabed was determined directly from measurements by a Scanmar sensor mounted close to the camera when these measiuements were available. Based on 5 such measiuements, a reference length defined by the distance across the foiu central rubber-discs of the Rockhopper groundgear was estimated to 1.05 m. Unfortunately the reference length could not be verified, because the entire trawl along with the video equipment were lost in a later haul. When the reference length was known, this was used to estimate h for each fish measiuement by manipulation of Eq. (1) using the image coordinates (left and right points) of the reference length. Finally, Eqs. (1) and (2) were applied to measiue fish length. Because only fish very close to the bottom were measiued, it is a good approximation to set z = 0. The fish length was calculated as the distance between the endpoints of a straight line with centre at the fish position and the appropriate length and orientation corresponding to the video frame measiuements. 3. Results 3.1. Length composition from catch and video In total, 80 Greenland halibut were classified as caught based on the video recordings from the seven trawl hauls where catch was retained. Additionally, for nine individuals it was not possible to determine whether they were caught or lost under the groundgear. This corresponded to 63% and 70%, respectively, of the actual catch from these seven trawl hauls. The lengths estimated from the video recordings were generally in good agreement with the length composition from the catch (Fig. 2). The difference between the mean length of those individuals that were classified as caught and the mean length from the real catch was less than eight percent in any of the six trawl hauls where Greenland halibut were measiued from the catch. The frequency distributions based on video and catch were of similar range and with similar modes. However, for individual trawls the video-mode was either similar or one 5 cm lengthgroup below the catch-mode. From all eight videos, 127 Greenland halibut were recorded as either caught or lost under the 4 0 - "Video Catch" o o o Trawl catch o 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 Length group midpoint (cm) Fig. 2. Length distribution of Greenland halibut caught by the trawl compared with estimated length distribution from video recordings. Data from six trawl stations where length was recorded from catch.

O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 121 * Caught e & Lost ------------Total 25 Ê 20 15 IQ- 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 Length group midpoints (cm) Fig. 3. Estimated length distributions of Greenland halibut from video recordings. Individuals observed going into the trawl compared with fish lost under the ground-gear. 3.2. Behaviour patterns In total 138 Greenland halibut were identified from the video recordings, 126 of which could be followed until crossing the ground-gear, either beneath or above. All individuals were swimming with the blind side facing down. At first detection, 53% of the fish started swimming from a resting position on the bottom substrate, 11% were swimming along with the trawl, while 36% were swimming towards the trawl opening. The two largest groups did not differ substantially in terms of length composition or in proportion lost under the groundgear (Fig. 5). Those swimming along with the trawl at first detection included only individuals at or ground-gear. The percentage of fish denoted as caught was 72. In addition, 11 individuals were registered as uncertain catch. Fig. 3 shows the frequency distributions of video-lengths of the caught and lost individuals respectively. For 30 cm and larger individuals the proportion caught (catchability) was 70% and independent of length (Fig. 4). For smaller individuals catchability was significantly lower (x2, df= 1, p<0.05), estimated to 40%. Caught I I Lost i i Uncertain 1.0 0.8 È" 0.6 O 04 0.2 0.0 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 Length group midpoint (cm) Fig. 4. Catchability by size of Greenland halibut in trawl estimated from video recordings. Symbol size indicates number of observations; the leftmost and the two rightmost values were based on less than ten observations. The first and last groups are plusgroups and the line is fitted using a cubic function. 55 20-1 5-10- 5-0 - b i l l. - - 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 Length group midpoints (cm) Fig. 5. Estimated length distributions of Greenland halibut at first detection when entering the picture frame, a: fish resting on the substrate, b: fish swimming in the trawl direction in front of the ground-gear, and c: fish swimming towards the trawl opening.

122 O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 above the mean length of the other two groups. Descriptions of behaviour for each of the three groups follow. The individuals lying on the bottom were first detected when they started to swim. They started off with a burst, usually generating a mud cloud that was seen in the water for a few seconds. Often subsequent tail bursts also curled up mud, thus generating a track of clouds. Most individuals remained on the bottom until the ground-gear was less than 1.5 m away. At take-off the fish were usually well within the observation window and mud-clouds from a take-off ahead of the camera field of view were only occasionally seen. Fig. 6 shows the distance from the gear at take-off. All individuals waiting with take-off until the groundgear was less than 30 cm away were caught. This is a significantly higher catching proportion (x 2, d f= l, p<0.02) than for those that took off at greater distance from the ground-gear. Otherwise, there was no clear indication that the probability of being captured decreased with increasing distance at take-off. For fish that start to swim from a resting position on the bottom, Fig. 7 shows the transverse position on the screen at take-off (lower panel) and when crossing the ground-gear (upper panel). The probability of being caught did not appear to be dependent on transverse position neither at take-off nor crossing. The individuals were mostly detected for the first time in front of the mid-section of the Caught I I Lost C I Uncertain -1 0 1 Transverse position Fig. 7. Greenland halibut resting on the bottom when first detected. Transverse position in the picture frame at first detection (b) and when fish crossing the ground-gear (a). ground-gear, while the transverse position when crossing the ground-gear was more uniformly distributed. This corresponds with the bearing of the fish changing markedly in this process (Fig. 8). Nearly all individuals took off in a direction away from the trawl, while their direction was predomi- a Starboard -3 and -2 Middle -1 to +1 +2 and +3 12-1 I Caught Lost Uncertain Crossing the gear Entering the picture Distance from gear at take - off (cm) Fig. 6. Greenland halibut resting on the bottom when first detected: Frequency distribution of distance from the ground-gear at take-off, related to whether they were eventually going into the trawl or under the ground-gear. Fig. 8. Greenland halibut resting on the bottom when first detected. The orientation at take-off (bottom) and when crossing the groundgear (top) relative to the transverse position in the picture frame when first detected. Trawl direction is downwards. The radius represents 14 observations.

O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 123 nantly transverse to the trawl path when they crossed the ground-gear. Most swam close to the seabed, 65% were below approx. 35 cm (the diameter of the rubber discs of the Rockhopper ground-gear) and only 6% swam more than ca. 1 m above the bottom. About half of the observed individuals swam in front of the trawl for some time before turning to the sides and approaching the ground-gear. These had the longest observation time, with a mean and maximum of 6 and 18 s, respectively. The other half turned directly after take-off and the mean observation time was less than 2 s (Table 3). The Greenland halibut that were first observed swimming along with the trawl had a rather uniformly distributed transverse position in the video image at first detection as well as when crossing the ground-gear (Fig. 9). After detection they behaved similarly to those that were observed starting off from the bottom. They crossed the ground-gear heading sideways, and were tracked for 2-14 s with a mean of 4. Table 3 summarises the different types of behaviour from detection to crossing of the ground-gear. E o 2 0-15- 1 0 - o Q 20H Z 15-1 0 - Caught I I Lost I I Uncertain 3-2 1 0 1 2 3 Transverse position Fig. 9. Greenland halibut swimming along with the trawl when first detected. Transverse position in die picture trame at first detection (b) and when crossing die ground-gear (a). The individuals that were heading towards the trawl opening after first detection were first observed at the lateral sides of the screen (Fig. 10). They were Table 3 Different behaviour patterns observed from first detection of individual Greenland halibut to the time it crossed the ground-gear Behaviour fiom detection to crossing of tile ground-gear Up from bottom, swimming in front, turning to the side Up from bottom, turning to the side Swimming in front, turning to the side Swimming along the ground-gear ends towards the trawl Swimming along the ground-gear ends towards the trawl, swimming in front n P t L SE C 32 29 5,9 35,2 1,3 68 29 26 1,9 38,3 2,5 65 10 9 4,4 42,0 2,2 73 36 33 1,4 38,2 1,7 78 3 3 9,1 38,6 4,4 100 n: Number of observations, p: Percentage of all observations classified, t: Mean time in sec. from detection to crossing of the ground-gear, L and SE: Mean length with standard error, C: Proportion being caught. o 2 0 - Caught I I Lost - 3-2 - 1 0 1 2 3 Transverse position Fig. 10. Greenland halibut swimming towards the trawl opening when first detected. Transverse position in the picture frame at first detection (b) and when crossing the ground-gear (a).

124 O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 Starboard Middle 3 and 1 to +1 +2 and +3 Crossing the gear Entering the picture Fig. 11. Greenland halibut swimming towards the trawl opening when first detected. Orientation when first detected (b) and when crossing the ground-gear (a) relative to the transverse position in the picture frame when first detected. Trawl direction is downwards. The radius represents 14 observations. The catch rate of Greenland halibut was highest during the first 100-200 m after the ground-gear hit the bottom. Fig. 12 (upper panel) shows the number observed per 100 m for all trawls combined, for the first 2000 m, which was the length of the shortest haul. The mean length of the individuals observed within the first 200 m was significantly larger than for later encounters (p< 0.001) and included the two largest individuals (>70 cm) observed. Neither catchability (percentage going above the ground-gear) nor previous behaviour (percentage within the different behaviour types recognised) of these first few individuals of each haul was different from that of those that were caught later in the hauls (x2, p>0.1 ). The frequency distribution of relative distance between successive observations of Greenland halibut (Fig. 12, lower panel) was not significantly different from the fitted exponential distribution (Kolmogorov-Smimov D, p>0.1). Thus, there is no reason to reject the null hypothesis that the spatial pattem conforms to a random Poisson process. 12 o 1 0 I following either side of the ground-gear in the same direction as the sweeps (Fig. 11) and crossed the ground-gear in its mid-section. They were mainly swimming close to the seabed: 80% were less than approx. 35 cm above the bottom and only 4% were above approx. 1 m. These individuals were usually seen for less than two seconds, except for three individuals that turned in front of the mid-section of the ground-gear and swam along with the trawl for up to 17 s before crossing the ground-gear (Table 3 ). When crossing the ground-gear, most fish were either swimming above or were hit and/or overrun by the ground-gear. A few individuals seemed to actively position themselves heading towards the ground-gear and moving between the rubber discs. The mean length and the previous behaviour patterns were not significantly different for these three types of contact with the ground-gear (p>0.1). w n E 6 0 - 'co 50 - Ê 14 0-2 3 0 - i 1------1------1------1------ 1------1------1------1------1------1------1------r 500 1000 1500 Distance from start of haul (m) 2000 3.3. Spatial pattern throughout the haul 1 0-0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Relative distance between successive specimens Fig. 12. Encounters of Greenland halibut along the trawl path during the hauls. All eight hauls combined. Upper panel: Number of fish observed relative to towed distance after the ground ground-gear hit the bottom. Data for the first 2000 m of each haul, as this was the length of the shortest haul. Lower panel: Distance between successive observations relative to the mean distance between observations within each trawl. Dots represent observed frequencies and the line represents the fitted exponential distribution.

O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 125 4. Discussion 4.1. Influence o f methods used The reaction of fish to an approaching trawl varies according to light levels (Blaxter and Parrish, 1965; Glass and Wardle, 1989). Therefore, it seems feasible that the use of artificial light in the otherwise dark environment of Greenland halibut might impact the observed behaviour. Previous studies of flatfish behaviour in front of trawls have not found clear differences attributable to neither ambient light (Beamish, 1969; Walsh and Hickey, 1993) nor artificial light (Weinberg and Munro, 1999). However, such effects cannot be ruled out. Therefore artificial light was used only in lack of alternative methods for direct observation at these depths of a bottom living flatfish with low acoustic contrast. Supplementing with flash photos and video recordings using time intervals on the light soiuce might indicate the realism of the constant light recordings. About 30% of the Greenland halibut caught were not observed on the video recordings. Since the recordings were of high quality, most of these other individuals must have entered the trawl outside the camera field of view. This could either be at the same swimming height as those observed, but close to the net wall, or higher up in the water where the width of the visual field was very limited. However, the upper limit of vertical distribution seemed well defined in oiu study, and Greenland halibut entering towards the roof of the trawl would have represented a behaviour pattern distinctly different from those observed. On the other hand, for Greenland halibut swimming towards the trawl, the frequency of transverse position increased towards the lateral edge of the observation field. It seems probable that those not observed were individuals swimming along the net towards the trawl opening. Increasing the visual angle or adjusting the tilt may thus decrease the discrepancy between observations and catch. Taking into account the lack of precise reference measures (i.e. width of Rockhopper disks), the length frequency distributions based on estimated and measiued lengths were reasonably similar. The discrepancy increased from zero to one 5-cm length interval for individuals from 20 to 50 cm. However, error in the estimated reference measiue will give increasing absolute error in estimated length for larger fish. Since there was little evidence of strong length-dependent behavioiu, it is reasonable to assume that the discrepancy was mainly due to imprecise estimate of the reference length. 4.2. Greenland halibut behaviour in front o f trawl Most Greenland halibut observed could be classified in two distinct main types of behavioiu: either leaving the bottom and swimming with the groundgear or swimming towards the trawl along the groundgear ends. These two main types may not be attributable to different phases of the same general pattem of reaction, as very few individuals were observed changing from one to the other. Also very few were swimming out of sight in such a way that they would be able to make such a change. If those that were caught without being observed on the video represent the latter behavioiu type, then the two types were equally represented. Both main behavioiu patterns indicate horizontal herding. Those swimming along the ground-gear ends towards the opening were herded to the mid-section of the ground-gear before crossing, and those that took off from the bottom were mainly located in front of the mid-section of the ground-gear and may thus have been herded before being detected by the video. The take-off behavioiu was similar to the way other flatfishes react in front of a trawl. Main and Sangster (1981) found that several flatfish species were herded by the sweeps towards the centre of the ground-gear. They reacted and swam away at right angles when the sweeps were close, and lay down again after 0.5-5 m. This behavioiu was repeated until they were swimming in front of the ground-gear. This is consistent with oiu observations, where fish that left the bottom were already concentrated to the mid-section of the ground-gear section when first detected in the video image. The observations by Main and Sangster ( 1981 ) were made in shallow water during daylight, but Walsh and Hickey (1993) found little difference in flatfish reaction to the trawl during day and night. If oiu observations were not affected by the use of artificial light, they suggest that Greenland halibut at 600 m depth are able to react to the approaching trawl in a way similar to that observed by Main and Sangster (op cit.). In that case, herding may

126 O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 be effective also along the sweeps and the associated sand clouds from the doors. The low swimming height of Greenland halibut would also facilitate herding along the sweeps. Even though Greenland halibut that took off from the bottom were heading away from the trawl, they may not all have reacted to the trawl before lying down. Flatfish may change horizontal heading when lying on the bottom and when leaving the bottom they tend to head away from disturbing objects (Stickney et al., 1973). When facing an approaching danger flatfish may also seek to the bottom to change heading before take-off in the new direction. This facilitates fast start in the new direction (Webb, 1981). Thus, it is not possible from our observations to estimate how many of the observed individuals had previously been herded towards the trawl path. The width and efficiency of the herding process remains to be evaluated. If individual Greenland halibut were herded by the action of doors and sweeps, one would expect to observe more and more individuals during the first few minutes of the hauls. Our results, however, showed the opposite effect, with significantly higher encounter rates at the very start than further on during in the hauls. This result may be related to some sort of surprise effect (Godo et al., 1990) on individuals encountered just after the ground-gear hit the bottom. Such effects will probably be related to signals that change abruptly rather than gradually. The visual stimuli from the artificial light soiuce will increase gradually also for fish encountered early in the haul, as the trawl approaches obliquely from above. The sound patterns, on the other hand, will change abruptly as the ground-gear hits the bottom. It is also probable that Greenland halibut can detect the sounds generated from the ground-gear, as can other flatfishes (Karlsen, 1992; Lagardère and Vilotte, 1990). However, also some visual cues will increase abruptly at bottom contact, e.g. the lighted dust-cloud from behind the ground-gear, and it is not possible from the present study to infer which sensory systems are most important in determining the reaction to the trawl. Oiu observations indicate that for smaller fish catchability increases with fish length due to reduced escapement beneath the ground-gear. Huse et al. (1999) found a bell-shaped selection curve for Greenland halibut caught by sampling trawl, including reduced catchability with increased size for the largest individuals. This seems consistent with oiu finding of reduced catch rates, especially of the largest individuals, during the main part of the hauls compared with the probable confusion area at the very start of the hauls. The larger Greenland halibut were probably to some degree able to avoid the trawl at distances ahead of the visual range of the camera. This is also consistent with the observation that the individuals where the tail part was the first part entering the video-frame (i.e. those that were observed reacting at greater distance from the ground-gear) constituted the upper part of the length composition in this study. A bell-shaped selection curve will seriously reduce estimates of maturation at age and the female portion of the spawning stock (Albert, 2003). The present results indicate that reducing haul duration might improve sampling of larger Greenland halibut. 4.3. General discussion and further research Though previous direct observations of Greenland halibut are virtually non-existent, the species has often been described as a vigorous swimmer, possibly swimming in a vertical position and probably behaving more like a roundfish (Smidt, 1969; De Groot, 1970; Jorgensen, 1997; Huse et al., 1999). Our observations do not support this general view of the species. Although we do not know for how long the individuals had used energy to avoid the trawl, it is noteworthy that none of those entering oiu observation range were able to keep up with the trawl for more than 20 s. The swimming position was exclusively horizontal, also at more than 1 m above the bottom. Most individuals were lying on the bottom and they were apparently, to a large degree, relying on the bottom for changing direction in the horizontal plane. This behavioiu is in accordance with some previous circumstantial observations of Greenland halibut held in experimental tanks (Albert et al., 1999), passing over a sorting grid in demersal trawl (R. Larsen, University of Tromso, pers. comm., 2003 ), and some Russian observations by video and flash camera (M. Zaferman, PINRO, Murmansk, pers. comm., 2003). Further, the spatial distribution was in accordance with a random Poisson distribution, and thus no evidence was found for schooling or shoaling behavioiu. It seems that Greenland halibut is more flatfish-like than previously anticipated.

O.T. Albert et al. / Journal o f Sea Research 50 (2003) 117-127 127 However, our observations were restricted with respect to coverage of the natural environment of Greenland halibut. First, the bottom depths were shallower than where the highest catch rates are found (Albert et al., 1998). Secondly, only the demersal region was covered, while several studies indicate that the species also may occur pelagically (Jorgensen, 1997; Bowering and Lilly, 1992; Hovde et al., 2002 ). Future research on availability of Greenland halibut to survey trawls should focus on behavioiu at higher abundance levels and diuing other time periods (e.g. spawning season), and also on the extent of pelagic occurrence. In addition, the effects of artificial light on catching performance of the trawl with respect to Greenland halibut need to be addressed. References Albert, O.T., 2003. Migration from nursery to spawning area in relation to growth and maturation of Greenland halibut (Reinhardtius hippoglossoides) in the Northeast Arctic. J. Northwest Atl. Fish. Sei. 31, 1-13. Albert, O.T., Nilssen, E.M., Stene, A., Gundersen, A.C., Nedreaas, K.H., 1998. Spawning of the Barents Sea/Norwegian Sea Greenland halibut (Reinhardtius hippoglossoides). ICES CM 1998/0:22. 19 pp. Albert, O.T., Midling, K.0., Gundersen, A.C., Rasmussen, T., Siikavuopio, S., 1999. Blâkveite som forsoksdyr: Erfaringer fra innledende forsok og perspektiver for bestandsvurderinger [Greenland halibut used for tank experiments: Experience from a pilot study and perspectives for stock evaluations (in Norwegian)]. Norwegian Institute of Fisheries and Aquaculture, Tromso, Norway. Report no 14/1999. 9 pp. Beamish, F.W.H., 1969. Photographic observations on reactions of fish ahead of otter trawls. FAO Fisheiy Rep. 62, 511-521. Blaxter, J.H.S., Parrish, B.B., 1965. The reaction of marine fish to moving netting and other devices in tanks. Mar. Res. 1966 (1) (15 pp.). Bowering, W.R., Lilly, G.R., 1992. Greenland halibut (Reinhardtius hippoglossoides) off southern Labrador and northeastern Newfoundland (Northwest Atlantic) feed primarily on capelin (Mallotus villosus). Neth. J. Sea Res. 29, 211-222. Bublitz, C.G., 1996. Quantitative evaluation of flatfish behaviour during capture by trawl gear. Fish. Res. 25, 293-304. De Groot, S. J., 1970. Some notes on an ambivalent behaviour of the Greenland halibut Reinhardtius hippoglossoides (Walb.) Pisces: Pleuronectiformes. J. Fish Biol. 2, 275-279. Engäs, A., Godo, O.R., 1989. Escape of fish under the fishing line of a Norwegian sampling trawl and its influence on survey results. J. Cons. Int. Exp lor. Mer 45, 269-276. Femö, A., Olsen, S. (Eds.), 1994. Marine fish behaviour in capture and abundance estimation. Fishing News Books, Blackwell Science, Oxford. 221 pp. Fréon, P., Misund, O.A., 1999. Dynamics of pelagic fish: Distribution and behaviour: Effects on fisheries and stock assessments. Fishing News Books, Blackwell Science, Oxford. 348 pp. Fréon, P., Gerlotto, F., Misund, O.A., 1993. Consequences of fish behaviour for stock assessment. ICES Mar. Sei. Symp. 196, 190-195. Glass, C.W., Wardle, C.S., 1989. Comparison of the reaction of fish to a trawl gear, at high and low light intensities. Fish. Res. 7, 249-266. Godo, O.R., Pennington, M., Volstad, J.H., 1990. Effects of tow duration on length composition of trawl catches. Fish. Res. 9, 165-179. Harbitz, A., 1990. Video based ocean wave analysis. Ph.D. thesis, University of Tromso, Norway. Haralick, R.M., 1980. Using perspective transformations in scene analysis. Computer Graphics and Image Processing 13, 191-221. Hovde, S.C., Albert, O.T., Nilssen, E.M., 2002. Spatial, seasonal and ontogenetical variation in diet of Northeast Arctic Greenland halibut (Reinhardtius hippoglossoides). ICES J. Mar. Sei. 59, 421-437. Huse, I., Gundersen, A.C., Nedreaas, K.H., 1999. Relative selectivity of Greenland halibut (Reinhardtius hippoglossoides, Walbaum) by trawls, longlines and gillnets. Fish. Res. 44, 75-93. Jorgensen, O.A., 1997. Pelagic occurence of Greenland halibut (Reinhardtius hippoglossoides, Walbaum), in West Greenland waters. J. Northwest Atl. Fish. Sei. 21, 39-50. Karlsen, H.E., 1992. Infrasound sensitivity in the plaice (Pleuronectes platessa). J. Exp. Biol. 171, 173-187. Lagardère, J.P., Vilotte, J.P., 1990. Performance particulière d un poisson plat (Solea solea) en écoute basse fréquence. In: Filippi, P., Zakharia, M. (Eds.), Les Ulis Editions de Physique. First French Conference on Acoustics Colloque de Physique c2, Suppl. 51, pp. 631-634. Main, J., Sangster, G.I., 1981. A study of the fish capture process in a bottom trawl by direct observations from a towed underwater vehicle. Scottish Fish. Res. Rep. 23 (23 pp.). Smidt, E.L.B., 1969. The Greenland halibut Reinhardtius hippoglossoides (Walb.), biology and exploitation in Greenland waters. Meddr Danm. Fisk.- og Havunders 6, 79-148. Stickney, R.R., White, D.B., Miller, D., 1973. Observations of fin use in relation to feeding and resting behaviour in flatfishes (Pleuronectiformes). Copeia 197, 154-156. Walsh, S.J., Hickey, W.M., 1993. Behavioural reactions of demersal fish to bottom trawls at various light conditions. ICES Mar. Sei. Symp. 196, 68-76. Wardle, C.S., Hollingworth, C.E. (Eds.), 1993. Fish behaviour in relation to fishing operations. Selected papers from a symposium held in Bergen, 11-13 June 1992. ICES Mar. Sei. Symp., vol. 196. 215 pp. Webb, P.W., 1981. The effect of the bottom on the fast start of flatfish (Citharichthys stigmaeus). Fish. Bull. 79, 271-276. Weinberg, K.L., Munro, P.T., 1999. The effect of artificial light on escapement beneath a survey trawl. ICES J. Mar. Sei. 56, 266-274.