Published online: 05 Dec 2007.

Similar documents
The Value of Data Gary Patronek & Stephen Zawistowski Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Hsin-Yi Weng a & Lynette A. Hart b a Department of Pathobiology, College of Veterinary

for Assistance Elise R. Shore a, Charles Burdsal a & Deanna K. Douglas b a Psychology Department, Wichita State University

Distressed Animal Behaviors and Some Recommendations for Improvements at the Kuala Lumpur Zoo, Malaysia Amber Haque Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Long-Term Outcome After Treatment of Feline Inappropriate Elimination Amy R. Marder & Joan M. Engel Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Departments, Iowa State University, Ames b Department of Population Medicine, Ontario Veterinary College, University of Guelph,

Nicole Cottam a, Nicholas H. Dodman a, Alice A. Moon-Fanelli a & Gary J. Patronek b a Department of Clinical Science, Tufts Cummings

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

TITLE: The effects of video footage versus photographs on perception of dog behavioral

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Amelia J. Cook a & Emily McCobb a a Center for Animals and Public Policy, Cummings

Behaviour of dogs adopted from an animal shelter. Svatava Vitulová, Eva Voslářová, Vladimír Večerek, Iveta Bedáňová

SHELTER DOGS AND THEIR DESTINY A RETROSPECTIVE ANALYSIS TO IDENTIFY PREDICTIVE FACTORS: A PILOT STUDY

Carin Wittnich a b & Michael Belanger b a Canadian Veterinary Medical Association, Ottawa,

To cite this article: Emily Weiss & Sandra Wilson (2003) The Use of Classical and

Sex, age and size as factors affecting the length of stay of dogs in Czech shelters. Jiří Žák, Eva Voslářová, Vladimír Večerek, Iveta Bedáňová

Christina Siettou a, Iain M. Fraser a & Rob W. Fraser a a School of Economics, Keynes College, University of Kent,

The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs

Sterilization of Companion Animals: Exploring the Attitudes and Behaviors of Latino Students in South Texas

BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT TOOLS FOR DOGS IN A SHELTER SETTING. Sara L. Bennett, DVM, MS, DACVB

Outcome of Cats Adopted From a Biomedical Research Program Brian A. DiGangi, P. Cynda Crawford & Julie K. Levy Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

What's in a Name: Effect of Breed Perceptions & Labeling on. Attractiveness, Adoptions & Length of Stay for Pit-Bull-Type Dogs.

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

John Reilly. Canisius College. March 17, 2018

Dog Breeding in New Providence, The Bahamas, and Its Potential Impact on the Roaming Dog Population II: The Fate of Puppies

Birth and Death Rate Estimates of Cats and Dogs in U.S. Households and Related Factors

Test. Assessment. Putting. to the. Inside Features. Features

30/04/2014. Why people keep pets and why we need to change how we breed them. Who I am. What are pets? What I plan to talk about

Reducing Surrenders. Dayna Kennedy Shelter Manager Upper Peninsula Animal Welfare Shelter

An Evaluation of Respondent Conditioning Procedures to Decrease Barking in an Animal Shelter

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Shawn Jen-Lung Peng a, Lisa Yu-Ting Lee b & Andrew Chang-Young Fei a a Institute of Animal Welfare, School of Veterinary

A survey of the management of inter-dog aggression by animal shelters in Canada

Scientifically evaluating welfare in commercial breeding kennels: does high volume preclude good welfare?

Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by owners of dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter

Is dog aggression a problem in Aboriginal communities?

Long-term Effects of Early Environments on the Behavior and Welfare of Dogs

To cite this article: Merry Lepper, Philip H. Kass & Lynette A. Hart (2002)

Indigo Sapphire Bear. Newfoundland. Indigo Sapphire Bear. January. Dog's name: DR. NEALE FRETWELL. R&D Director

Factors Impacting Public Perceptions of Animal Welfare & Animal Rights Candace C. Croney Purdue University

Comparative Evaluation of Online and Paper & Pencil Forms for the Iowa Assessments ITP Research Series

Chapter 13 First Year Student Recruitment Survey

An Argument against Breed Specific Legislation

ORGANIZATIONS THAT DO NOT ENDORSE BREED SPECIFIC LEGISLATION

Judging a Dog by Its Cover: Morphology but Not Training Influences Visitor Behavior toward Kenneled Dogs at Animal Shelters

Management of bold wolves

Dog Breeding in New Providence, The Bahamas, and Its Potential Impact on the Roaming Dog Population I: Planned and Accidental

Published online: 24 Sep 2008.

The Journal of Veterinary Medical Science

Published online: 28 Apr 2015.

Westminster Adoption Group and Services Bulldog Adoption Application

Daycare Application Form

The Force Concept Inventory (FCI) is currently

Daphne Green Temperament Assessment D.O.B: Weight:4wks-5.13lbs 5wks-6.91lbs 6wks-lbs 7wks-5.90lbs

The Kennel Club has long campaigned for a ban on the use and sale of electric shock collars in Scotland.

Dog Behavior and Training - Moving with Your Dog

Opal Pink Dot Temperament Assessment D.O.B: Weight:6wks 5.42lbs 7wks 6.20lbs

Effects of Differing Traits in Dogs on Perceived Adoptability. Dogs are a mainstream part of American life. While many pet dogs are mutts, 35% of

Longevity of the Australian Cattle Dog: Results of a 100-Dog Survey

Foster Application. Facebook.com/furrytailendingscaninerescue us at Susan Daniele, President

INTRODUCTION & MEASURING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR

SPAY / NEUTER: IT S NOT JUST ABOUT KITTENS AND PUPPIES

An Evaluation of a Shelter Dog Training Class: Outcomes for Volunteer Trainers and for Dogs

German Pinscher Club of America Rescue. (GPCA Rescue)

Lilac Green Temperament Assessment D.O.B: Weight: 5wks-3.31lbs 6wks-3.66lbs 7wks-4.77lbs

GERMAN SHEPHERD RESCUE of SOUTHEASTERN PENNSYLVANIA 9012 Cargill Lane Philadelphia, PA ADOPTION AGREEMENT

Conflict-Related Aggression

Max WHAT BREEDS MAKE UP MAX? German Shepherd Dog Mix crossed with Cocker Spaniel / Maltese Cross

ADOPTION QUESTIONNAIRE FOR A GSD RESCUE

Creating an Effective Shelter Intake Form to Reduce Owner Surrender

Selecting Shelter Dogs for Service Dog Training Emily Weiss Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Austria Published online: 28 Apr 2015.

Reversing Category Exclusivities in Infant Perceptual Categorization: Simulations and Data

Genetic analysis of a temperament test as a tool to select against everyday life fearfulness in Rough Collie 1

Published online: 23 Dec 2008.

Rottweiler Hearts Rescue

Life for Dogs Living in Shelters

Housing Density and Growth in Juvenile Red- Eared Turtles Scott P. McRobert Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Human-Animal Interactions in the Turkey Industry

The human-animal bond is well recognized in the

Fergie Blue Stripe Temperament Assessment D.O.B: Weight:5wks 5.07lbs 6wks 6.16lbs 7wks 7.06lbs

R U S T Y D O G N I T I O N R E P O R T - A P R I L 1 2,

BEHAVIOUR PROBLEMS IN DOGS: THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN REPORTED BEHAVIOUR PROBLEMS, CAUSAL ATTRIBUTIONS, AND INEFFECTIVE DISCIPLINE

Responses of shelter and pet dogs to an unknown human

B. Witkind Davis a, Kelvin Alie a, William J. Fielding b, Michelle Morters c & Francisco Galindo d

Dog Behavior and Training - Teaching Calm Settle and Relaxation Training

Daycare & Boarding Application

Public Perceptions of Dog Welfare, Sourcing and Breeding Regulation

Shelter Guidelines Project. Shelter Guidelines - Content

KCAI Scheme Online Assessments: Criteria

PIGEON DISCRIMINATION OF PAINTINGS 1

Backstromhus Puppy Questionnaire

What to look for in a breeder, checklist.

Separation Anxiety Syndrome

Muse Teacher Guide: February 2018

Transcription:

This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 07:13 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/haaw20 Dog Breed Stereotype and Exposure to Negative Behavior: Effects on Perceptions of Adoptability John C. Wright a, Alison Smith a, Katie Daniel a & Karen Adkins a a Psychology Department, Mercer University, Published online: 05 Dec 2007. To cite this article: John C. Wright, Alison Smith, Katie Daniel & Karen Adkins (2007) Dog Breed Stereotype and Exposure to Negative Behavior: Effects on Perceptions of Adoptability, Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 10:3, 255-265 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888700701353956 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL WELFARE SCIENCE, 10(3), 255 265 Copyright 2007, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Dog Breed Stereotype and Exposure to Negative Behavior: Effects on Perceptions of Adoptability John C. Wright, Alison Smith, Katie Daniel, and Karen Adkins Psychology Department Mercer University The purpose of the study was to determine if brief exposure to a dog behaving badly or in a friendly manner affects subsequent perceptions of the target dog s and other dogs adoptability. Participants viewed a videotape of an adoptable German shepherd behaving either aggressively or prosocially and were then asked to rate the characteristics and adoptability of the same and different dogs. The results showed that people who saw the aggressive behavioral schema perceived only the target dog and a dog of the same breed to be significantly less adoptable than dogs of other breeds (p <.01). Results of a principal components analysis showed participants perceived the adoptability of dogs to be related to sociability : Adoptable dogs were more approachable, friendly, intelligent, and less dangerous and aggressive (p <.01). Brief exposure to a misbehaving dog prior to making a decision to adopt may unfairly penalize other dogs perceived to be similar to the misbehaving dog. Recent research on shelter-dog adoptions has focused on identifying the factors that contribute to unsuccessful and successful placements. Some studies have focused on identifying the risk factors associated with relinquishment (Mondelli et al., 2004; New et. al., 2000; Patronek, Glickman, Beck, McCabe, & Ecker, 1996; Salman et al., 1998; Scarlett, Salman, New, & Kass, 1999; Shore, 2005) and ways to identify behaviorally risky dogs (Hennessy et al., 2001; Segurson, Serpell, & Hart, 2005). Other studies have identified the factors that contribute to successful adoptions or to a reduction in relinquishments after adoption Correspondence should be sent to John C. Wright, Psychology Department, 1400 Coleman Avenue, Mercer University, Mercer, GA 31207. Email: wright_jc@mercer.edu

256 WRIGHT, SMITH, DANIEL, ADKINS (Coppola, Grandin, & Enns, 2006; Marston, Bennett, & Coleman, 2005; Posage, Bartlett, & Thomas, 1998). Relatively absent in the shelter-dog adoption literature, however, is the extent to which people s perceptions of different dogs characteristics and behaviors contribute to their decision to adopt one dog over another or one breed of dog over another (Posage et al., 1998). Dog characteristics used to identify one nonhuman animal from another include physical attributes such as size, coat color, breed, sex, and reproductive status (intact or gonadectomized). Behavioral characteristics include a number of different, measurable dimensions including aggression and demand for affection. Although breed-specific perceptions of behavior have been described by veterinarians and other dog-care professionals (Bradshaw & Geodwin, 1999; Hart & Hart, 1985; Hart & Miller, 1985), the same kind of information has not been reported for potential adopters. Do people enter the shelter with preconceptions about the adoptability of dogs who are physically and behaviorally different? We assumed that the judgments people make about which dog to adopt from a shelter begin with preconceived notions about the kind of dog who would make an appropriate or an inappropriate companion animal. Initially, adoptability may be based on breed preferences (e.g., yellow lab, not boxer) and other physical characteristics (size, sex). If size were held constant, would people judge a sample of different breeds to be equally adoptable? More important, would people s initial exposure to a target dog s behavior (behaving badly or prosocially) influence their subsequent perceptions of the adoptability of only the target dog, of dogs of the same breed, or of any dog regardless of breed? It may be that the same cognitive-emotional processes involved in making judgments about the traits of a specific person are invoked when an individual makes judgments about the traits of a shelter dog. When people (actors) judge other people (targets), actors have a tendency to move rather quickly from observations of a target s general physical appearance and behaviors to inferences about personality traits judgments about what the target individual is like (Mae, Carlston, & Skowronski, 1999; Van Overwalle, Drenth, & Marsman, 1999). The process takes place rather spontaneously and automatically. As one observes a target s personal (dispositional) demeanor and behaviors, the characteristics attributed to the target in turn serve as the bases for predicting the target s future behavior (Newman, 1996). When the original, dispositional judgment is evaluative (either positive or negative), subsequent judgments of the target may be prejudicially biased without the actor s awareness (Winter & Uleman, 1984). If the positive or negative traits are generalized to a category of individuals, a category-based stereotype results (Corneille & Judd, 1999). Once formed, stereotypes activate implicit perceptions and expectations about the characteristics and behaviors of any individual perceived to be a member of the target s group, regardless of the specific target s actual traits and behaviors. We wondered if potential adopters have a tendency to make dispositional attributions about dogs, too, based on initial, scant information. Similar to the forma-

DOG BREED STEREOTYPE AND NEGATIVE BEHAVIOR 257 tion of other category-based stereotypes, stereotypes based on dogs physical characteristics may be formed as a result of evaluative judgments of a single dog s appearance, behaviors, and gestures (displayed signals). If so, an initial positive evaluation of an individual dog (a friendly golden retriever) could lead to perceptions of positive category-specific traits (all big dogs are friendly), and the resulting stereotypes could influence an actor s subsequent perceptions of large dogs and their adoptability. Although stereotypes can serve an adaptive function in people s lives, there are times when exceptions to stereotypes are noted (Bardach & Park, 1996). If someone is presented with information that contradicts a present stereotype, it may be processed differently from stereotype-consistent information and thought of as an exception to the rule, rather than the norm (Bardach & Park, 1996; Stangor & McMillan, 1992). The resulting schema (exception) may affect the way one processes future information and lead to the formation of a subgroup of the stereotyped category German shepherds, rather than all large dogs (Richards & Hewstone, 2001). Subgrouping is more likely to occur if the discordant schema is experienced in the immediate context (the shelter) within which the exception is experienced (Hamil, Wilson, & Nisbett, 1980). In this study we assessed actors perceptions of target dogs traits and adoptability as a function of breed and a manipulated factor behavioral schema, friendly or aggressive. Schematic information relating to the behavior of a specific target dog, a German shepherd, was presented immediately prior to viewing dogs of similar and different breeds. We hypothesized an interaction between schematic behavior and breed, such that the negative behavioral schema would reduce the subsequent adoptability ratings of the target German shepherd dog and a second German shepherd (same subcategory) but not the adoptability of other breeds. Participants METHOD Ninety-nine participants (62 women and 37 men), recruited from introductory psychology classes at Mercer University, received course credit in exchange for their anonymous participation. There were 58 participants in the schema-aggressive condition and 41 participants in the schema-friendly condition. Materials Videotaped dog: Construction of behavioral schemas. Initially, three videotaped segments 30-sec each of the same dog, a variegated German shepherd, and five photographs of dogs representing four different breeds were pre-

258 WRIGHT, SMITH, DANIEL, ADKINS pared for use in the study. The fifth photograph was the German shepherd who appeared in the videotapes. The videotapes showed the German shepherd behaving in a friendly manner, a neutral manner (sitting calmly), and an aggressive manner. Each videotaped segment represented a level of the between-subjects variable behavioral schema. To determine if the videotaped segments accurately portrayed the three behavioral schemas, a group of 29 psychology majors enrolled in a research methods class independently scored the three video segments on the dimensions friendliness and aggressiveness. The two ratings of each of the three schematic videotapes were recorded on a 7-point scale that ranged from 1 (not at all) to7 (extremely). Two one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted to determine if the German shepherd pictured in the three videotapes was perceived to be behaviorally different on the dependent measures of friendliness and aggressiveness. Alpha was set at.05 for all tests. For the friendly measure, a statistically significant difference was found, F(2, 56) = 19.10, p <.001. Pairwise comparisons revealed that all three video segments represented schemas that were statistically different from one another, in the predicted direction: The German shepherd representing the aggressive schema was rated relatively low on friendliness (M = 3.45, SD = 1.50), the neutral schema was rated friendlier (M = 4.66, SD = 1.37), and the friendly schema was rated most friendly (M = 5.66, SD = 1.26). Results of the ANOVA conducted on the aggressiveness measure also revealed a significant difference, F(2, 56) = 144.21, p <.001. Pairwise comparisons revealed a significant difference between the aggressive and neutral video segments and between the aggressive and friendly segments in the predicted direction. However, raters did not perceive a difference in aggressiveness between the friendly segment (M = 1.62, SD = 0.98) and the neutral segment (M = 1.79, SD = 1.11); both the latter video streams were perceived to be equally nonaggressive and significantly less aggressive than the shepherd portraying the aggressive schema (M = 5.21, SD = 1.15). Because the German shepherd segments representing the friendly and neutral schemas were perceived to not differ on aggressiveness, the neutral video was not used in this study. Instead, we used the two video segments that accurately represented the two dichotomous behavioral schemas: the German shepherd exhibiting friendly behavior (rated high on friendliness, low on aggression) and the same German shepherd exhibiting aggressive behavior (rated low on friendliness, high on aggression). Photographs of dogs. Eleven dog pictures representing different breeds were initially selected as stimulus photographs based on our best attempts to match them on similarity of appearance. The dogs appeared to be about the same size, occupied approximately the same percentage of photographic space, and

DOG BREED STEREOTYPE AND NEGATIVE BEHAVIOR 259 sat in a relaxed manner facing the camera. The 11 dogs were independently rated by eight undergraduate psychology majors on a favorableness scale that ranged from 1 (not at all favorable) to 7(extremely favorable); the scores were analyzed within a one-way ANOVA. Results showed the dogs were perceived to differ on favorableness, F(10, 70) = 3.85, p =.001. However, multiple pairwise comparisons revealed that not all dogs were statistically different from one another (p >.05). The five dog photographs we selected as stimuli did not differ on favorableness, and their means approximated the scale midpoint. The dog photographs included four different breeds, including a pointer (M = 4.63, SD = 0.93), a black German shepherd (M = 4.63, SD = 1.41), a bloodhound (M = 4.63, SD = 1.06), and a collie (M = 5.00, SD = 0.93; American Kennel Club, 2003). A still frame of the variegated German shepherd (GSV) from the friendly videotape was used as the fifth photograph. The questionnaire. The questionnaire consisted of five pages, with each page displaying only one dog photograph and six traits (dimensions) on which the dog was rated. Each dog photo was centered at the top of a rating sheet with the dog s breed written above the picture. The picture of the GSV was always placed last in the data pack after the other four stimulus dogs were viewed. The order of the presentation of the dogs, with the exception of the GSV, was counterbalanced. Design A 2 (behavioral schema) 5 (dog breed), mixed factorial design was used to determine the effects of schematic information on perceptions of dogs characteristics and adoptability. The between-subject variable was behavioral schema (friendly and aggressive); the within-subject variable was dog breed (German shepherd, collie, bloodhound, pointer, and GSV). Participants perceptions of each dog were measured on six dimensions: (a) approachability, (b) aggressiveness, (c) dangerousness, (d) intelligence, (e) friendliness, and (f) adoptability. Each item was rated on an 8-point Likert scale that ranged from 1 (not at all) to 8(extremely). Procedure Participants were randomly assigned to either the aggressive or friendly dog behavioral schema. Participants were told they were going to see a short videotape of a typical dog who could be seen in a shelter for adoption and that we wanted them to provide their own rating of the adoptability of several

260 WRIGHT, SMITH, DANIEL, ADKINS dogs after viewing the tape. Participants viewed and considered each dog individually and were told not to turn back to a previous dog s rating once they had gone on to the next dog. Participants were then debriefed and thanked for their participation. RESULTS The purpose of this study was to determine if viewing a dog behaving in a friendly or aggressive manner would affect the perceived traits and adoptability of dogs of the same or different breeds. For all analyses, alpha was set at.05. To determine if there was a relationship among any of the five traits and adoptability, a principle components analysis (PCA) with oblique (oblimin) rotation was performed on the scores (SPSS 15.0). A one-factor solution was extracted that accounted for approximately 67% of the variance in the six item scores; all items had salient loadings (>.30), and no other component was extracted. The single factor consisted primarily of items intended to assess the positive characteristics of dogs and was labeled sociability. Inspection of the Pearson correlation matrix (Table 1) shows that a dog high in sociability was perceived to be high on approachability, friendliness, intelligence, and adoptability and low on aggressiveness and dangerousness. A two-way mixed model ANOVA was conducted to determine if the perceived adoptability scores differed as a function of behavioral schema, breed, or their interaction. There was a significant interaction effect for Behavioral Schema Breed, F(4, 388) = 8.71, p <.01. Five subsequent independent groups t tests with Bonferroni correction were conducted (one for each dog photograph) to determine which dogs adoptability scores differed as a function of behavioral schema. As expected, the GSV showed the greatest schema-dependent difference in adoptability ratings, t(97) = 5.01, p <.01, η 2 =.21: Schema-aggressive viewers found the GSV to be less adoptable (M = 3.74, confidence interval [CI] = 3.28 4.20) than did schema-friendly viewers (M = 5.54, CI = 5.00 6.08). More important, there was also a significant difference in the predicted direction for the black German shepherd, t(97) = 3.69, p <.01, η 2 =.10: Schema-aggressive viewers perceived the second German shepherd to be less adoptable (M = 5.45, CI = 5.09 5.81) than the schema-friendly viewers did (M = 6.49, CI = 6.06 6.92), but the effect was not as strong. None of the other three breed adoptability ratings were significantly affected by the schema manipulation (Figure 1). The ANOVA also revealed two significant main effects, one for breed, F(4, 388) = 44.20, p <.01, η 2 =.31, and the other for behavioral schema, F(1, 97) = 10.80, p <.01, η 2 =.10. Both main effects were qualified by the more important Behavioral Schema Breed interaction.

TABLE 1 Correlation Matrix of Perceived Dog Traits and Adoptability Adoptability Approachability Friendliness Intelligence Dangerousness Aggressiveness Adoptability.80*.74*.56*.64*.59* Approachability.80*.49*.61*.64* Friendliness.50*.55*.56* Intelligence.26*.30* Dangerousness.85* Aggressiveness Note. N = 99. *p <.01. 261

262 WRIGHT, SMITH, DANIEL, ADKINS FIGURE 1 The mean adoptability scores (+ 1 SEM) for five dogs rated by participants who initially saw either a friendly or aggressive German shepherd. Inspection of the significant Behavioral Schema Breed interaction shows that those participants who viewed the aggressive, variegated German shepherd (GSV) subsequently rated both the GSV and a black German shepherd as significantly less adoptable (p <.01). DISCUSSION The results of the ANOVA performed on the adoptability ratings provide support for the hypothesized interaction between schematic behaviors and breed. The behavioral schema affected participants perceptions of both the target German shepherd (GSV) and the black German shepherd but not the adoptability of less similar dogs. Further, the adoptability of both German shepherds seemed to be more polarized by the negative schema (aggressiveness) than by the positive schema (friendliness; see Figure 1), although only the aggressive GSV dog was perceived to be relatively unadoptable. It was the only dog whose mean adoptability (3.74) fell below the 4.5 midpoint of the adoptability scale. These results appear to be consistent with other findings showing that people s evaluative judgments about individuals are susceptible to significant change by brief exposure to biasing information (Hamil et al., 1980) and are influenced more heavily by negative than by positive information (Ito, Larsen, Smith, & Cacioppo, 1998). It is perhaps not surprising that the processes people use in making quick judgments from scant information about subcategories of people (e.g., race or gender; Ito et al., 1998) may extend to organizing perceptions about subcategories of dogs (breeds). An alternative interpretation of the Schematic Behavior Breed interaction, however, is that actors already had a negative stereotype of German shepherds and

DOG BREED STEREOTYPE AND NEGATIVE BEHAVIOR 263 that exposure to the aggressive German shepherd merely activated the preexisting stereotype. Further, despite the initial neutral favorability ratings of the stimulus photographs, one wonders whether exposure to an aggressive collie, for example, would have penalized subsequent adoptability ratings of the collie to the same extent as the GSV. Perhaps activating an existing positive stereotype (all collies are Lassie) would dampen the effects of the negative behavioral schema or increase the effects of the positive schema. There are other limitations regarding the interpretation of Schema Breed effect that our study does not address. If reliable, how narrow or broad is the generalized effect of positive and negative schemas on breed evaluations? What is the duration of effect? For those people who actually adopt a pet, what is the average amount of time for the adoption decision to be made from the time the adopter enters the shelter facility? Does the average adopter put more thought into selecting an actual dog than our actors did, and are thoughtful adopters more resistant to stereotype-inconsistent information (Richards & Hewstone, 2001)? The preliminary nature of these results needs to be emphasized before generalizations to other breeds and certainly to real world shelter adoptions are considered. With respect to the rating of dog characteristics by potential adopters, Gosling, Kwan, and John (2003) have demonstrated that personality traits in dogs can be reliably measured. Our sociability factor consisted of adoptability ratings that significantly correlated with five additional traits (Table 1). Taken together, the intercorrelations seem to indicate that people perceived the adoptability of dogs holistically, with the common underlying thread related to the dogs positive social attributes. One goal of this study was to broaden the scope of inquiry regarding shelter placements of dogs to include the potential adopter. We have attempted to shed some light on the person-perception characteristics that may contribute to the adoption process and hope these results serve as a heuristic for future research. REFERENCES American Kennel Club. (2003). AKC recognized breeds. Retrieved October 2, 2003, from http://www.akc.org/breeds/recbreeds/index.cfm Bardach, L., & Park, B. (1996). The effect of in-group/out-group status on memory for consistent and inconsistent behavior of an individual. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 169 178. Bradshaw, J. W., & Goodwin, D. (1999). Determination of behavioral traits of pure-bred dogs using factor analysis and cluster analysis: A comparison of studies in the USA and UK. Research in Veterinary Science, 66, 73 76. Coppola, C. L., Grandin, T., & Enos, R. M. (2006). Human interaction and cartisol: Can human contact reduce stress for shelter dogs? Physiology & Behavior, 87, 537 541. Corneille, O., & Judd, C, M. (1999). Accentuation and sensitization effects in the categorization of multifaceted stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 927 941.

264 WRIGHT, SMITH, DANIEL, ADKINS Gosling, S., Kwan, V., & John, O. (2003). A dog s got personality: A cross-species comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1161 1169. Hamil, R., Wilson, T. D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1980). Insensitivity to sample bias: Generalizing from atypical instances. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 578 589. Hart, B. L., & Hart, L. A. (1985) Selecting pet dogs on the basis of cluster analysis of breed behavior profiles and gender. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Associations, 186, 1181 1185. Hart, B. L., & Miller, M. F. (1985). Behavioral profiles of dog breeds. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Associations, 186, 1175 1180. Hennessy, M. B., Voith, V. L., Mazzei, S. J., Buttram, J., Miller, D. D., & Linden, F. (2001). Behavior and cortisol levels of dogs in a public animal shelter, and an exploration of the ability of these measures to predict problem behavior after adoption. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 73, 217 233. Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887 900. Mae, L., Carlston, D. E., & Skowronski, J. J. (1999). Spontaneous trait transference to familiar communicators: Is a little knowledge a dangerous thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 233 246. Martson, L. C., Bennett, P. C., & Coleman, G. J. (2005). What happens to shelter dogs? Pt. 2. Comparing three Melbourne welfare shelters for nonhuman animals. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 8, 25 45. Mondelli, F., Prato Previde, E., Verga, M., Levi, D., Magistrelli, S., & Valsecchi, P. (2004). The bond that never developed: Adoption and relinquishment of dogs in a rescue shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 7, 253 266. New, J. C., Salman, M. D., Scarlett, J. M., Kass, P. H., King, M., & Hutchinson, J. M. (2000). Shelter relinquishment: Characteristics of shelter-relinquished animals and their owners compared with animals and their owners in U.S. pet-owning households, Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 3, 179 201. Newman, L. S. (1996). Trait impressions as heuristics for predicting future behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 395 411. Patronek, G. J., Glickman, L. T., Beck, A. M., McCabe, G. P., & Ecker, C. (1996). Risk factors for relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Associations, 209, 572 581. Posage, G. J., Bartlett, P. C., & Thomas, D. K. (1998). Determining factors for successful adoption of dogs from an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Associations, 213, 478 482. Richards, Z., & Hewstone, M. (2001). Subtyping and subgrouping: Processes for the prevention and promotion of stereotype change. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 52 73. Salman, M. D., New, J. G., Scarlett, J. M., Kass, P. H., Ruch-Gallie, R., & Hetts, S. (1998). Human and animal factors related to relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 1, 207 226. Scarlett, J. M., Salman, M. D., New, J. C., Jr., & Kass, P. H. (1999). Reasons for relinquishment of companion animals in U.S. animal shelters: Selected health and personal issues. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 2, 41 57. Serguson, S. A., Serpell, J. A., & Hart, B. L. (2005). Evaluation of a behavioral assessment questionnaire for use in the characterization of behavioral problems of dogs relinquished to animal shelters. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Associations, 227, 1755 1761. Shore, E. R. (2005). Returning a recently adopted companion animal: Adopter s reasons for and reactions to the failed adoption experience. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 8, 187 198.

DOG BREED STEREOTYPE AND NEGATIVE BEHAVIOR 265 Stangor, C., & McMillan, D. (1992). Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: A review of the social developmental literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 42 61. Van Overwalle, F., Drenth, T., & Marsman, G. (1999). Spontaneous trait inferences: Are they linked to the actor or the action? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 450 462. Winter, L., & Uleman, J. S. (1984). When are social judgments made? Evidence for the spontaneousness of trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 237 252.