THE INFLUENCE OF LIGHT INTENSITY ON OVIPOSITION OF THE COTURNIX QUAIL

Similar documents
STATE FFA POULTRY EVALUATION KNOWLEDGE TEST

206 Adopted: 4 April 1984

Effect of EM on Growth, Egg Production and Waste Characteristics of Japanese Quail Abstract Introduction Experimental Procedures

IT HAS been well established that

Quail farming. Introduction to quail farming. Housing management of quails. Advantages of quail farming. 1. Deep litter system. 2.

ON COMMERCIAL poultry farms during

The Effect of Full-Spectrum Fluorescent Lighting on Reproductive Traits of Caged Turkey Hens 1 ' 2

Ecochicks Poultry Limited

Guide for Incubation of Eggs and Care of Newborn Livestock

INFO SHEET. Cull Eggs: What To Expect And How To Reduce The Incidence.

T not always autonomous (STURTEVANT 1932). By transplantation experiments

Estelar CHAPTER-6 RAISING AND PRODUCTION OF POULTRY BIRDS

STUDY BEHAVIOR OF CERTAIN PARAMETERS AFFECTING ASSESSMENT OF THE QUALITY OF QUAIL EGGS BY COMPUTER VISION SYSTEM

RURAL INDUSTRIES RESEARCH AND DEVELOPMENT CORPORATION FINAL REPORT. Improvement in egg shell quality at high temperatures

Avian Reproductive System Female

THE POULTRY ENTERPRISE ON KANSAS FARMS

Agricultural Extensi?n Se:;ice University of Californi County of Orange

SO YOU'RE GOING TO HAVE A BABY... CHICK!! Tips for Teachers Embarking on the Embryology Experience

THE production of turkey hatching

FFA Poultry Career Development Event 2004 NEO Aggie Day. 1. With regard to egg storage, which of the following statements is FALSE?

THE LAYING FLOCK VIRGINIA 4-H CLUB SERIES. AGIUCU LTUJiAL EXTENSION SERVICE OF V. P. I., BLACKSBURG, VA.

Formoguanamine-induced blindness and photoperiodic responses in the Japanese quail, Coturnix coturnix japonica

Section 6. Embryonic Development and Hatchery Management Notes

Optimizing lighting for precision broiler breeder feeding. Grégory Bédécarrats Department of Animal Biosciences University of Guelph

Selection for Egg Mass in the Domestic Fowl. 1. Response to Selection

EGG production of turkeys is not important

Recommended Resources: The following resources may be useful in teaching

Factors Influencing Egg Production

Effect of Calcium Level of the Developing and Laying Ration on Hatchability of Eggs and on Viability and Growth Rate of Progeny of Young Pullets 1

FEEDING CHINESE RINGNECK PHEASANTS FOR EFFICIENT REPRODUCTION. Summary *

Test of the Impact on Reproductive Potential and Future Generations of Mammals and Test of the Impact on Reproduction of Birds

Variation of Chicken Embryo Development by Temperature Influence. Anna Morgan Miller. Rockdale Magnet School for Science and Technology

How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation?

COMBINING ABILITY OF QUAIL LINES Photoacceleration of Embryonic Development in Depigmented Japanese Quail Eggs

Reports hom the Research Laboratories... of the Department of Psychiatry University of Minnesota

EDUCATION AND PRODUCTION. Layer Performance of Four Strains of Leghorn Pullets Subjected to Various Rearing Programs

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

OBSERVATIONS ON SWALLOWS AND HOUSE- MARTINS AT THE NEST. BY

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite

EDUCATION AND PRODUCTION

,omb White Leghorn Layers in Three Types of Houses in Oregon

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt

This article is downloaded from.

Reproductive physiology and eggs

LAYING BEHAVIOUR OF EGG AND MEAT TYPE CHICKEN AS INFLUENCED BY NEST TIER

Unit D: Egg Production. Lesson 4: Producing Layers

Human-Animal Interactions in the Turkey Industry

Simplified Rations for Farm Chickens

Infinite Eggs. Sarah Shull. 12 th Grade. Francis Joseph Reitz High School

A STUDY ON EGG QUALITY TRAITS IN JAPANESE QUAILS * (Coturnix coturnix japonica)

Lighting Practices for Successful Laying Hens

Temperature Gradient in the Egg-Laying Activities of the Queen Bee

Kentucky Academic Standards

Structure 4.0 Objective 4.1 Introduction 4.2 General Features

Dubbing Production--Bred Single--Comb White Leghorns

Hatching Chicks in the Classroom

2. There will be one egg production class of four birds to be judged on past production qualities. Perfect score is 50 points

A CITIZEN S GUIDE TO IDENTIFYING AND CORRECTING PROBLEM LIGHTS ADJACENT TO SEA TURTLE NESTING BEACHES

Wheat and Wheat By-Products for Laying Hens

188 WING, Size of Winter Flocks SIZE OF BIRD FLOCKS IN WINTER BY LEONARD WING

HaloGLS, HaloCandle and HaloSpherical lamps

Dry Incubation. By Bill Worrell

Photostimulation in poultry

Under One Roof. Beehive Management During the Swarming Season in a single hive. By: - Nick Withers

POULTRY Allen County 4-H

Exterior egg quality as affected by enrichment resources layout in furnished laying-hen cages

Evolution in Action: Graphing and Statistics

BOURBON COUNTY FISCAL COURT ORDINANCE NO AN ORDINANCE ESTABLISHING STANDARDS FOR DOG KENNELS IN BOURBON COUNTY, KY

Case 2:14-cv KJM-KJN Document 2-5 Filed 02/03/14 Page 1 of 6 EXHIBIT E

Production Basics How Do I Raise Poultry for Eggs?

Corn Snake Care Sheet

Field Herpetology Final Guide

Chameleons: Biology, Husbandry and Disease Prevention. Paul Stewart, DVM. Origin: Africa (40% of species) and Madagascar (40% of species)

Reptile UVB100. Tropical Terrarium Bulb

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

Long-Term Selection for Body Weight in Japanese Quail Under Different Environments

MARY F. WILLSON RESULTS

Teaching Assessment Lessons

Before and After: The Chicken and the Egg

R&J Enterprises Green Cove Springs, FL Rjaquatics.com

Module Egg. MODULE NO. 25: Internal Quality of Egg

HATCHING BEHAVIOR OF THE BOBWHITE

Temperature Relationships of Two Oklahoma Lizards

The Effect of Oviposition Time on Egg Quality Parameters in Brown Leghorn, Oravka and Brahma Hens

Low Temperature Effects on Embryonic Development and Hatch Time 1

Complete Solutions for BROILER BREEDERS

SUMMARY OF THESIS. Chapter VIII "The place of research, its purpose, the biological material and method"

Physical Description Meadow voles are small rodents with legs and tails, bodies, and ears.

JUDGING RABBITS 4-H LEADER MANUAL EM4502E WHY JUDGE? HOW TO JUDGE

INCUBATION AND VITAL MORPHOLOGICAL TRAITS IN EGGS FROM AGE-RELATED TURKEYS

CHAPTER3. Materials and methods

The genetic factors under consideration in the present study include black (+) vs. red (y), a sex-linked pair of alternatives manifesting

I the BUSSEY INSTITUTION of HARVARD UNIVERSITY, it was found that

Biology of Citrus Trunk Borer (Anoplophora versteegi Rits.) (Coleoptera : Cerambycidae) under Laboratory Conditions

FEATURES OF DISTRIBUTION OF LOADING IN COD-END OF TRAWL OF A VARIOUS DESIGN

PRACTICAL APPLICATION OF ARTIFICIAL INSEMINATION IN CONJUNCTION WITH SYNCHRONIZATION OF HEAT CYCLE IN THE EWE

EFFECTS OF SEASON AND RESTRICTED FEEDING DURING REARING AND LAYING ON PRODUCTIVE AND REPRODUCTIVE PERFORMANCE OF KOEKOEK CHICKENS IN LESOTHO

Allocating Feed to Female Broiler Breeders: Technical Bulletin #2

R A I S I N G Y O U R H O M E C H I C K E N F L O C K

General Leopard Gecko Care: Housing

Transcription:

THE NFLUENCE OF LGHT NTENSTY ON OVPOSTON OF THE COTURNX QUAL HOWARD L. HOSCK NTEREST in the Japanese Quail (Cotarnix coturnix japonica) as an avian laboratory species has been spreading rapidly. Because of the increasing need for birds and eggs for study, there is much work being done on factors affecting egg laying by the quail. One of the major influences is light (see Abplanalp, 1961; Reese and Reese, 1962; Wilson et al., 1962). As with other birds, notably chickens and ducks, most of these studies have involved the role of photoperiodism with respect to egg laying. We have been more interested in the effects of light intensity as such on the egg laying patterns of the quail. Where other studies have shown a definite correlation between photoperiodism and egg laying (Abplanalp et al., 1962; Abplanalp, 1961), this study shows that time and place of oviposition are influenced by the available light intensity. MATERALS AND METHODS Cages 142 cm (56 inches) by 55 cm (22 inches) by 30 cm (12 inches) deep of one-inch wire mesh were set up as shown in Figure 1. A variety of light sources was employed, one at a time, including a fifty watt light bulb, a fifty watt bulb heavily filtered with colored cellophane, and a ten watt bulb. The ten watt source placed in one end of the cage gave a light gradient along the cage length ranging from forty foot-candles near the source to less than one-half foot-candle at the opposite cage end. ntensities were measured as incident light. The cages were blocked from extraneous light by heavy, dull, black plasticized cloth. The cage bottoms of one-inch wire mesh effectively prevented rolling and kicking about of the eggs after they were laid. Food and water positions were changed frequently in relation to one another and the light source to eliminate their placement as a factor in position of egg laying, and to avoid a training effect, as discussed below. There were approximately twenty birds to a cage, of which 25 per cent were males. Ten to twelve eggs were collected daily. Food was commercial Purina game bird chow. Lights were kept on in the cages continuously. Room temperature was fairly constant at 22 degrees centigrade. RESULTS We first used incandescent white light sources, in an attempt to ascertain the precise intensities in which laying tended to occur. The results are summarized in Figure 2. A large proportion of the eggs (6O.S%) were laid be- 434

Howard L. Hosick OVPOSTON OF COTURNX ---. \ \.\\ 1 \ \ ---. \ \ \ 1 \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ O w \ LGHT \ SOURCE. ; / : / /, ; 35--- 1 : *_ it :\ 1 _x / ; S---_ /, /, : - 5 2 0.5 foot-candles 22 FG. 1. Floor-plan and a three-dimensional sketch of the cage setup used. Dotted lines in top figure indicate light intensities at various cage positions. Note light, food, and water, shown in a typical arrangement. tween two and 0.5 foot-candles intensity. Seventy per cent of the eggs in this range (or, 55.3% of the total eggs) were placed in light intensities between one and 0.5 foot-candles. This latter range of 0.5 foot-candles could be resolved no further due to limitations on the photometer employed in these determinations, and practical limitations involving the precise positioning of the eggs when dropped on the one-inch mesh floor. t should perhaps be noted that precisely one egg was gathered per day in the higher light intensity ranges. This fact tends to indicate that the same bird was doing the abberant laying in all cases; perhaps this bird had some malfunction (blindness) which caused her to lay her eggs in response to some other stimulus (for example, heat). This assumption has not been verified. One light source used was a fifty-watt incandescent bulb, which put out enough light so that the intensity even at the opposite end of the cage was greater than the two-foot-candle maximum of the preferred laying range. The food container was placed precisely in the middle of the cage; hence, it cast a shadow, and this shadow was the only cage area in which the light intensity was less than two foot-candles. Consequently, ninety per cent of the eggs laid under these conditions were laid in the shadow of the food can. When the

436 THE WLSON BULLETN 80 J 70 60 - /- 7 i--u44 30 28 26 24 22 20 18 16 14 2 O 8 6 4 : LGHT NTENSTY (FOOT CANDLES 1 FG. 2. Number of eggs collected versus collection intensities for white light. Note majority of eggs laid at less than two foot-candles, and most of these between one and 0.5 foot-candles. food can was moved against the rear of the cage, so that it cast no shadow, the eggs were still deposited precisely where they had been before-that is, where the food-can shadow had been, but where there was now an increased light intensity (ranging from four to twenty foot-candles). This training effect continued for about one week, during which time progressively fewer eggs were laid daily in the higher intensity area, and more were laid at the very rear of the cage. These results are summarized in Figure 3, which is a plot of number of eggs collected versus light intensity for the first week after the food container was moved. A greater number of eggs was laid in the higher light intensities than under normal conditions (Fig. 2).

Howard L. Hosick OVPOSTON OF COTURNX 437 J 25- cn 20- : _ W 0 15-5- 0,, 30 28 26 24 22 20 18 16 14 12 O 8 6 4 2 <0.5 LGHT NTENSTY (FOOT CANDLES) FG. 3. Demonstration of training effect, as explained in text. Plot of number of eggs versus light intensities, for first week after low intensity shadow eliminated cage. Higher intensities shown here found where shadow had been. from Having attained these results with a white light, similar studies were made using colored lights to ascertain whether the egg laying response was initiated by light of a particular wavelength, rather than by the sum of wavelengths given by a white light. Using the same experimental setup as before, but with colored cellophane filters over the light source, essentially the same results were obtained as for the white light. These results are summarized in Figure 4. The colored filters cut down the light intensities to a considerable extent; the cages were, in fact, quite dark throughout. Consequently, egg production dropped off markedly, and after several weeks essentially no eggs were being laid. These results agree with those of Abplanap (1961). The eggs which were gathered during this period were small, sterile, and had extremely thin

438 THE 27 m 24 w 2 LL 18 0 15 VOLET a % 2 9 9 =e 3 8 0 Lnl 4816 ) 12108 6 4 2 (0.5 6 4 LGHT NTENSTY (FOOT CANDLES) TRANSMSSON CHARACTERS December 1966 Vol. 78, No. 4 LGHT NTENSTY (FOOT CANDLES) FG. 4. Number of eggs collected versus light intensities for various colored light sources. Note similarity, in all cases, to Fig. 2. shells. The effects of darkness on Coturnix are well known and have been described (Abplanalp, 1961; Abplanalp et al., 1962; Wilson et al., 1962). After being subjected to the original light intensity for one week, egg production was back to normal. Reinitiation of egg layin, 0 was thus easily accomplished in Coturnix although this is not so easily done with some other birds (Farner, 1964). The darkness effects were least pronounced with red light; egg production in red light continued normally for almost one month. This may be because red light penetrates organic tissue best, and thus a lower intensity of red light will elicit the same response as a higher intensity light of another color (Farner, 1964)) or perhaps the visual oil filters responsible for color vision in birds (Wald, 1950) differ slightly. f the red droplets were either in less abundance than yellow or red-green droplets, allowing more red light to reach the actual photoreceptors of the cones, or were more readily penetrable than the other two, a greater visual response for a given light intensity would be elicited. t should be noted, however, that two conflicting responses were observed, in that egg laying continued for a longer time in the red light, indi-

Howard Hosick L OVPOSTON OF COTURNX 439 96 - go- 84-78- 72 - u 42 W g 36 2 30 24-18- 12-6- 0,, 1, 30 28 26 24 22 20 18 16 14 12 LGHT NTENSTY (FOOT lb a CANDLES) FG. 5. Number of eggs collected versus light intensities from cage having heat filter placed to eliminate differential temperature effects on oviposition. eating greater sensitivity to lon g wavelengths, yet eggs were placed in the same light intensities in which they were placed with all other light sources. According to Wald (1950), there are droplets in the visual cones of birds corresponding to each of the three primary colors, and a fourth type, which is essentially clear. Thus a part of white light is able to penetrate all droplets, and the response elicited by a given white light would naturally be greater than that from a colored light of comparable intensity. Thus laying is most readily maintained in white light. At this point in the experiments, it was impossible to ascertain with certainty whether the observed response was the result of a light or of a tempera-

THE WLSON BULLETN Deremhcr 1966 Vol. 78, No. 4 ture gradient. Whereas a light gradient was indeed observable, the incandescent light source also produced a partial temperature gradient through the cage length, which may have been the cause of the observed egg distribution, rather than the variations of light intensity within the cage. Temperature and light as factors were effectively separated by the use of a heat filter consisting of two sheets of one-fourth inch thick plate glass, mounted two inches apart in a wooden frame. Using this device in front of the light source, a temperature difference of no greater than 0.5 degrees centigrade was ever observed in simultaneous readings over the cage length. Under conditions with a light gradient but constant temperature, oviposition occurred in the Z-O.5 foot-candle range in 88.7 per cent of all cases. These results are shown in Figure 5. n these determinations, the small, ten-watt bulb was used. Because of reflection and absorption by the glass filters, the maximum light intensity within the cage was decreased from 40 to 34 foot-candles with this light source. The effect on the minimum intensity within the cage could not be determined due to equipment limitations. Th e result of the lowered intensity, however, was that the 2-0.5 foot-candle range occupied 70 per cent of the cage length, or 20 per cent more than previously. Egg distribution within the l-o.5 footcandle range was more easily observable in the increased area covered thereby; assortment was found to be random within that range. During this period, several of the female quails were becoming blind. The eyes of these birds were observed to be swelling shut. As this blindness progressed, more and more eggs were laid in the higher light intensities. During a period of one week, the individuals used in these experiments were subjected to sixteen hours of high intensity light, which was, however, constant over the entire cage length. Durin, u the other eight hours of the day, the birds were subjected to the usual light gradient. All eggs were laid during the eight hours of relative darkness, apparently, as they were gathered in the same pattern as before. These results substantiate the premise that long-period high intensity light may inhibit the actual laying response (Farner, 1964). DSCUSSON Egg laying in low light intensities may confer several adaptive advantages on the quail. These intensities correspond to the very early dawn hours, at which time the air and ground temperatures are still relatively low. Thus the eggs will be rapidly cooled upon laying. This cooling has been found to be necessary before development of the embryo can proceed (Padgett and vey, 1964). The light at dawn (and dusk) tends to be shifted toward the red end of the spectrum in comparison with the light during the rest of the day, due to

Hownd L. Hosick OVPOSTON OF COTURNX 441 refraction of the sunlight by a greater depth of atmosphere. As explained previously, the egg laying response is easily elicited by red light. Thus the propensity to lay eggs in low light intensities is reinforced by the fact that said light intensities tend to be red when found in the birds natural environment. When the results here described are considered along with the work of others on the effects of light on the reproductive behavior of Coturnix quail, it becomes apparent that both light intensity and photoperiodism are important in the integrated response observed. Wilson et al. (1962) have made studies indicating that rate of sexual maturation is independent of light intensity. The same paper indicates the importance of day length on reproductive maturation, with photoperiods of fourteen or more hours being required for quantity egg production. This mechanism results in the production of eggs primarily in the long days of spring, when other factors such as food supply and temperature are optimal. An even longer photoperiod, such as that found in later months (June, July) may result in a dropping off of egg production (Farner, 1964). This lowering of egg production was not observed in the white light experiments here presented, even though lights were on in the cages continuously. This may have been due to the exceedingly low light intensities in which the birds spent part of their time. As a bird moved from the rear of the cage (farthest from the light-low intensity) to the front of the cage (near the light-high intensity) and back again, it may be considered to have undergone a brief high intensity light exposure. Because light intensities greater than 0.5 foot-candles were found over a much larger cage area than were low intensities (less than 0.5 foot-candles), and because movement of the birds was random throughout the cages, it can be considered that each bird was regularly subjected to a high-intensity exposure of greater than twelve hours per twentyfour hour day. Farner (1964) has described a pre-reproductive preparatory period which he has observed in birds. This consists of a high-intensity, relatively long photoperiod, such as a spring day, which is actually responsible for maturation of the reproductive system in preparation for copulation and egg laying. Egg laying can then occur at a later time when conditions for it, in turn, are optimal. The high intensities of light available in our experimental setup fulfill this preparatory function. The odd effects observed in the red light may be due in part to this light color precipitatin, m reproductive maturation more readily. Abplanalp (1961) has also done studies involving preconditioning, which indicate that a period of darkness prevents egg production during its application but enhances the later response of the hens to light. On the other hand,

THE WLSON BULLETN Drrpmber 1966 Vol. 78, No. 4 he indicates that intermittent lighting has an adverse effect on egg production. Both of these conditions were present in the white light setups employed by us (as explained above) yet egg production remained high. The effect of darkness apparently outweighs the intermittent lighting effects on subsequent egg laying. The hens inherent physiological tendency to ovulate once every twentyfour hours was not interrupted by the setup employed, as laying remained high and steady (during the first series of white light experiments). Such interruptions are, however, common under conditions of simple unnatural day lengths ( Abplanap, 1962). Wilson and Huang (1962) indicate that, in the studies they have made, about eighty per cent of Coturnix eggs were laid in the afternoon. However, it should be noted that the attendants were in the area during the early morning hours when egg laying would normally take place, quite possibly frightening the birds enough to delay oviposition. Arrington (1962) concurs with this afternoon-laying observation. More work is needed on this subject, as conflicting observations are apparent (Arrington et al., 1962; Wilson and Huang, 1962; Abplanalp, 1961). SUMMARY Coturnix quail has been found to lay its eggs in low light intensities when a range of intensities is available. The intensities at which laying took place were found to be independent of light color, although other responses were modified in monochromatic lights. The experimental setup used allowed for both development of the reproductive systems of males and females and the finalization of this maturation, namely, the laying of fertile eggs. The response to the influences of light intensity is apparently evolutionarily significant, in that several advantages are therewith conferred upon the species. Results employing heat filters to eliminate temperature differences over the cage length indicate that temperature gradients play essentially no role in these results. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This research was supported in part by National Science Foundation grant number GE 2636, and the R. G. Gustavson Research Award from the Colorado-Wyoming Academy of Science. The author wishes to thank Dr. Joseph C. Daniel for his help and interest. LTERATURE CTED ABPLANALP, H., A. E. WOODWARD, AND W. 0. WLSON 1962 The effects of unnatural day lengths upon maturation and egg production of ABPLANALP, H. the Japanese Quail Coturniz coturnix japonica. Poultry Sci., 41:1963-1968. 1961 Response of Japanese Quail to restricted lighting. Nature, 189:942-943. AHHNGTON, L. C., H. ABPLANALP, AND W. 0. WLSON 1962 Experimental modification of the laying pattern in Japanese Quail. British Poultry Sci., 3:105-113.

Howar L. Hosick OVPOSTON OF COTURNX FARNER, D. S. 1964 The photoperiodic control of reproductive cycles in birds. Amer. Scientist, 52: 137-156. PADGETT, C. A., AND W. D. VEY 1959 Coturnix quail as a laboratory research animal. Science, 129:267-268. REESE, E. P., AND T. W. REESE 1962 The quail, Coturnix coturnix, as a laboratory animal... Experimental Analysis WALD, G. of Behavior, 5 :265-270. 1950 Eye and camera. Scientific American, 183:32-41. WLSON, W. O., AND R. H. HUANG 1962 A comparison of the time of oviposition for Coturniz and chicken. Poultry Sci., 41:1%3%1845. WLSON, W. O., H. ABPLANALP, AND L. ARRNCT~N 1%2 Sexual development of Coturnix as affected by changes in photoperiods. Poul- try Sci., 41:17-22. DEPARTMENT OF BOLOGY, UNVERSTY OF COLORADO, BOULDER, COLORADO (PRESENT ADDRESS: DEPARTMENT OF ZOOLOGY, UNVERSTY OF CALFORNA, BERKELEY). 26 MARCH 1965.