CONSPECIFIC SCENT TRAILING BY GARTER SNAKES ( Thamnophis sirtalis ) DURING AUTUMN. Further Evidence for Use of Pheromones in Den Location

Similar documents
INFLUENCES OF HIBERNACULUM MICROENVIRONMENT ON THE WINTER LIFE HISTORY OF THE GARTER SNAKE (THAMN0PH1S SIRTALIS) 1

USE OF COMMUNAL SHEDDING SITES BY THE NORTHERN PACIFIC RATTLESNAKE (CROTALUS OREGANUS OREGANUS) IN CENTRAL WASHINGTON STATE

Flood-Associated Activities of Some Reptiles and Amphibians at Carlyle Lake, Fayette County, Illinois

Bio4009 : Projet de recherche/research project

Reptilian Physiology

THE concept that reptiles have preferred

Notes on Road-Killed Snakes and Their Implications on Habitat Modification Due to Summer Flooding on the Mississippi River in West Central Illinois

reproductive life History and the effects of sex and season on morphology in CRoTALus oreganus (northern PaCifiC RATTLESNAKES)

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

WATER plays an important role in all stages

BODY size and temperature affect nearly every

The Effect of Aerial Exposure Temperature on Balanus balanoides Feeding Behavior

Like mother, like daughter: inheritance of nest-site

Effects of movement and eating on chemosensory tongue-flicking and on labial-licking in the leopard gecko (Eublepharis macularius)

Alberta Conservation Association 2016/17 Project Summary Report

Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris

Timber Rattlesnakes (Crotalus horridus) in Tennessee, USA

Early experience shapes the development of behavioral repertoires of hatchling snakes

Objectives: Outline: Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles. Characteristics of Amphibians. Types and Numbers of Amphibians

Squamates of Connecticut

RESPONSES OF CAMBARID CRAYFISH TO PREDATOR ODOR

Obituary A Monument to Natural History Henry S. Fitch ( )

Biota of the Lehigh Gap Wildlife Refuge Reptiles and Amphibians

PREY-HANDLING BEHAVIOR OF HATCHLING ELAPHE HELENA (COLUBRIDAE)

A Role for Aggression Pheromones in the Evolution of Mammal-like Reptile Lactation

Meal Size Effects on Antipredator Behavior of Hatchling Trinket Snakes, Elaphe helena

REPORT OF ACTIVITIES TURTLE ECOLOGY RESEARCH REPORT Crescent Lake National Wildlife Refuge 31 May to 4 July 2017

Snakes of Wisconsin by Wisconsin DNR

SAMUEL M. MCGINNIS, Department of Biology, California State University, Hayward, CA 94542

From ethology to sexual selection: trends in animal behavior research. Animal behavior then & now

GREAT BASIN GOPHER SNAKE

Short-term Water Potential Fluctuations and Eggs of the Red-eared Slider Turtle (Trachemys scripta elegans)

Influence of meal size on postprandial thermophily in cornsnakes (Elaphe guttata)

Pill Bug Environmental Preferences Based on Moisture

Author's personal copy. Social behavior and pheromonal communication in reptiles

Site Selection and Environmental Assessment for Terrestrial Invertebrates, Amphibians and Reptiles

The Development of Behavior

Hibernation Site Philopatry in Northern Pine Snakes (Pituophis melanoleucus) in New Jersey

MICROHABITAT AND PREY ODOR SELECTION IN THE FORAGING PIGMY RATTLESNAKE

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

FIDELITY OF NORTHERN PINE SNAKES (Pituophis m. melanoleucus) TO NATURAL AND ARTIFICIAL HIBERNATION SITES IN THE NEW JERSEY PINE BARRENS

Reproductive strategies in snakes

The effect of invasive plant species on the biodiversity of herpetofauna at the Cincinnati Nature Center

Social and Thermal Cues Influence Nest-site Selection in a Nocturnal Gecko, Oedura lesueurii

MASTER OF SCIENCE IN ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCE

EVALUATION OF A METHOD FOR ESTIMATING THE LAYING RATE OF BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS

Byall, C., H. M. Smith, and D. Chiszar Response of Brown Tree Snakes (Boiga

FOREIGN OBJECTS IN BIRD NESTS

High Mortality of a Population of Cowbirds Wintering at Columbus, Ohio

PRELIMINARY EVALUATION OF THE IMPACT OF ROADS AND ASSOCIATED VEHICULAR TRAFFIC ON SNAKE POPULATIONS IN EASTERN TEXAS

California Bighorn Sheep Population Inventory Management Units 3-17, 3-31 and March 20 & 27, 2006

Ecological Archives E A2

Food Item Use by Coyote Pups at Crab Orchard National Wildlife Refuge, Illinois

Status and Management of Amphibians on Montana Rangelands

Species Results From Database Search

CROAKS AND TRILLS Volume 11, Issue 1 June 2006

REPORT OF ACTIVITIES 2009 TURTLE ECOLOGY RESEARCH REPORT Crescent Lake National Wildlife Refuge 3 to 26 June 2009

Spatial Learning of Shelter Locations and Associative Learning of a Foraging Task in the Cottonmouth, (Agkistrodon Piscivorus)

Mother offspring recognition in two Australian lizards, Tiliqua rugosa and Egernia stokesii

A SURVEY FOR THREATENED AND ENDANGERED HERPETOFAUNA IN THE LOWER MARAIS DES CYGNES RIVER VALLEY

Rules of the Game. Lab Report - on a separate sheet

Title Madagascan Snake, Leioheterodon mad. Author(s) Mori, Akira; Randriamboavonjy, Tahi. Citation Current Herpetology (2010), 29(2):

IN species where tail autotomy can occur during

Chemosensory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by lizards: implications of field spatial relationships between males

Conflicts between Courtship and Thermoregulation: The Thermal Ecology of Amorous Male Garter Snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis, Colubridae)

Post-Release Success of Captive Bred Louisiana Pine Snakes

Unit 19.3: Amphibians

Animal Adaptations. Structure and Function

I. Introduction. Orientation and Navigation 3/8/2012. Most difficult problem Must know. How birds find their way. Two terms often misused

POSTNATAL DEVELOPMENT IN PEROMYSCUS MANICULATUS-POLIONOTUS HYBRIDS I. DEVELOPMENTAL LANDMARKS AND LITTER MORTALITY 12

Carphophis amoenus Family Colubridae Subfamily Xenodontidae

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

Amphibians and Reptiles in Your Woods. About Me

Field Herpetology Final Guide

(Received May 6, 1994; accepted June 27, 1994)

Great Horned Owl (Bubo virginianus) Productivity and Home Range Characteristics in a Shortgrass Prairie. Rosemary A. Frank and R.

Communication and Displays of Snakes

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

by Andrew L. Shiels, Leader, Nongame and Endangered Species Unit

Density, growth, and home range of the lizard Uta stansburiana stejnegeri in southern Dona Ana County, New Mexico

Alberta Conservation Association 2013/14 Project Summary Report

Habitat use by the South-American rattlesnake (Crotalus durissus) in south-eastern Brazil

BODY SIZE AND SEXUAL SIZE DIMORPHISM OF BULLSNAKES (PITUOPHIS CATENIFER SAYI) IN WISCONSIN, USA

PIGEON DISCRIMINATION OF PAINTINGS 1

S7L Algal blooms that pollute streams, rivers, and lakes are caused by the presence of

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Pheromone-Mediated Intrasexual Aggression in Male Lizards, Podarcis hispanicus

Wandering Garter Snake

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Elk (Cervus canadensis) Extra credit assignment. Sad Underwing (Catocala maestosa) 10/11/2017

2 nd Term Final. Revision Sheet. Students Name: Grade: 11 A/B. Subject: Biology. Teacher Signature. Page 1 of 11

NATURAL AND SEXUAL VARIATION

Spatial distribution and larval biology of Spirobranchus giganteus

Effective August 2007 All indicators in Standard / 11

T HE recent and interesting paper by Alexander F. Skutch (1962) stimulated

Mate protection in pre-nesting Canada Geese Branta canadensis

Squamates of Connecticut. May 11th 2017

John Thompson June 09, 2016 Thompson Holdings, LLC P.O. Box 775 Springhouse, Pa

BUMPUS IN THE SNAKE DEN: EFFECTS OF SEX, SIZE, AND BODY CONDITION ON MORTALITY OF RED-SIDED GARTER SNAKES

8/19/2013. What is a community? Topic 21: Communities. What is a community? What are some examples of a herp species assemblage? What is a community?

The Use of Cage Enrichment to Reduce Male Mouse Aggression Neil Ambrose & David B. Morton Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Transcription:

Journal of Chemical Ecology, Vol. 15, No. 11, 1989 CONSPECIFIC SCENT TRAILING BY GARTER SNAKES ( Thamnophis sirtalis ) DURING AUTUMN Further Evidence for Use of Pheromones in Den Location JON P. COSTANZO Department of Zoology Miami University Oxford, Ohio 45056 (Received August 15, 1988; accepted January 11, 1989) Abstract--Adult garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis), collected in October near a traditional, communal hibemaculum in central Wisconsin, were acclimated to autumnal conditions and subjected to laboratory tests to determine whether they could follow scent trails of a conspecific. Graded responses were obtained, but 75% of the sample showed at least some inclination to follow scent trails. The results suggest that pheromone cues may be used by male and female garter snakes to locate traditional dens during autumnal migrations. Evidence from this and other studies suggests that pheromones are probably used in conjunction with other homing mechanisms and that the role of pheromones in den location may be more important in younger snakes and in populations inhabiting northern latitudes. Key Words--Pheromone, scent trail, chemical cue, hibernation, denning, homing, garter snake, Thamnophis sirtalis, Colubridae. INTRODUCTION An increasing literature concerning the role of chemical signals in the sociobiology of snakes (see Ford, 1986, for a recent review) has shown that the vomeronasal sense of these organisms is important and highly developed. Most studies of snake pheromones have addressed reproductive behaviors; however, scent trailing of conspecifics has been implicated in the formation of nonsexual aggregations for the purpose of avoiding unfavorable environmental conditions and reducing predation (e.g., Dundee and Miller, 1968; Duvall et al., 1985; Graves et ai., 1986; Noble and Clausen, 1936). Additionally, chemical cues 2531 0098-0331/89/1100-2531506.00/0 9 1989 Plenum Publishing Corporation

2532 COSTANZO have long been suspected to play a part in the relocation of communal snake dens during autumn (Hirth, 1966; Klauber, 1956; Noble and Clausen, 1936), but few investigators have attempted rigorous tests of this hypothesis. In northern climates, strong selection pressures have likely played a major role in the evolutionary development of mechanisms by which snakes relocate traditional dens during autumnal migrations from (often distant) summer habitats. Hiberuacula with features that effectively protect snakes from severe winter conditions may be limited on a local basis; in fact, this may be a reason why snakes den communally (Gregory, 1984). Snakes unable to relocate "proven" dens are likely subject to greater risks of mortality during winter. Also, mating opportunities in northern snake populations are often restricted to the den site (Gregory, 1982, 1984); thus, any individual not in attendance may forfeit its chance to contribute to the genome. Not unexpectedly, long autumnal migrations (up to 17.7 km in T. sirtalis; Gregory, 1984) to communal dens are made by many species and, in numerous cases, individuals show remarkably high fidelity to particular hibernacula (Gregory, 1982, 1984). Evidence that snakes are able to follow scent trails of conspecifics has been documented for 24 species representing five families (Ford, 1986). Few studies, however, have addressed this behavior in the context of den relocation (Brown and MacLean, 1983; Graves et al., 1986; Lawson, 1985). Accordingly, this study was undertaken to assess the ability of adult garter snakes, from a communally denning population in Wisconsin, to follow conspecific scent trails under simulated autumnal conditions. METHODS AND MATERIALS Eastern garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis) were collected during October 1986 as they gathered near a traditional, communal den in central Wisconsin (described by Costanzo, 1986). Twenty-eight snakes were housed, irrespective of body size or sex, in small groups. One additional snake, a large (94 g) female designated for use in forming scent trails, was maintained separately. Snakes were kept in plastic boxes and were exposed to autumnal circadian thermal and light regimes (20~ 13 hr photophase; 5~ 11 hr scotophase) for 14 days prior to testing. Snakes were provided with water, but food was withheld. Trailing ability was tested (procedure modified after Ford, 1981) during the last week in October, under photophase conditions (overhead fluorescent lighting; 387 lux), in a rectangular, plywood arena (Figure 1). Seventeen nails spaced at 5.5-cm intervals protruded from the floor and formed a continuous series of gates through which a snake could pass. The nails were arranged in a slight are so that all gates were located 200 cm from an entrance port in the front wall of the arena. The arena surface was covered with corrugated paper

SCENT TRAILING BY GARTER SNAKE 2533 9 '4-'-- 92 cm ~ FIG. 1. Diagram of the testing arena used to measure the trail following ability of garter snakes during autumn. Scent trails were produced by guiding the movement of a stimulus snake, which was confined inside a bollomless, removable tunnel (shown), down the length of the arena and through a randomly chosen gate. upon which 1200 ml of unscented cat litter (clay particles) was dispersed to provide traction. Nails were covered with cocktail straws to protect them from scent contamination. All coverings were replaced after each trailing and control test to ensure that residual odors were removed. To promote stimulus uniformity, only one animal was used to create scent trails; fresh trails were produced within 30 min of testing each subject for trailing ability. This was done by guiding the movements of the large female snake, which was enclosed inside a 4.5-cm wide, bottomless tunnel, down the length of the arena and through a gate (Figure 1) The tunnel and snake were removed prior to introducing the experimental subject. Gates containing scent were chosen randomly, except that the three nearest each side wall were never used; this restriction was necessary since pilot studies and Ford (1986) indicated that nontrailing and control snakes had a strong affinity for arena walls. Prior to testing, each subject was habituated for 20 min inside a holding box at the front of the arena. A pulley system was used to remove a partition between the holding box and the entrance port, thus allowing the snake to enter

2534 COSTANZO the arena. Trials involving snakes not entering the arena within 15 min were terminated. Movements and gate choices of snakes were observed from behind a blind 2.5 m away. After a subject passed completely through a gate and reached the distal wall of the arena, it was replaced in its holding cage. Snakes were tested in control situations (no scent trail present) four to six days later and then sexed. RESULTS Twenty-four of 28 snakes completed the control trials. All but one individual (96%) passed through end gates (the two adjacent gates nearest each side wall), thus, "wall-seeking" was a prominent behavioral characteristic of nontrailing snakes. This necessitated statistical evaluation of the results using a binomial test (choice of four end gates vs. choice of 14 middle gates; Sokal and Rohlf, 1969). Frequencies recorded for control snakes were significantly different from probabilities based upon random gate choice (Table 1). Binomial probabilities calculated for experimental and control groups showed that the presence of conspecific scent in the arena significantly influenced snake behavior (Table 1). Of the 28 snakes completing experimental (trials present) trails, 75% showed at least some inclination to follow scent trails. Trailing snakes demonstrated a behavior similar to the trail contact response (TCR) (Brown and MacLean, 1983), which involved slow and methodical sampling of the substrate, over which the trail-forming snake had passed, with its tongue tips. This behavior, which did not occur in control tests, allowed the observer to estimate (to within 10 cm) the length of the trail followed. Garter snakes showed differential responses to the presence of conspecific TABLE 1. EXPECTED AND OBSERVED BINOMIAL PROBABILITIES FOR GATE CHOICES OF GARTER SNAKES TESTED IN LABORATORY ARENA FOR ABILITY TO FOLLOW CONSPECIFIC SCENT TRAILS DURING AUTUMN Probability of Probability of N Significance Test condition end gate choice middle gate choice (P) Expected 0.222 0.778 Control 0.958 0.042 24 < 0.005 ~ Experimental 0.607 0.393 28 < 0.005 b Probability that responses of snakes tested in the absence of scent trails do not differ from random b gate selection. Probability that responses of snakes tested in the presence of scent trails do not differ from those obtained in control tests.

SCENT TRAILING BY GARTER SNAKE 2535 scent in the test arena. Strong trailing inclination was demonstrated by seven snakes (25 %) that correctly chose the scented gate and four snakes (14 %) that chose a gate immediately adjacent to the scented one. Ten snakes (36%) trailed initially (mean distance + SEM = 74 + 15 cm), but eventually passed through end gates. Seven individuals (25 %) showed no inclination to trail and also chose end gates. Snakes were classified, based on their inclination to trail, as exact trailers, near-exact trailers, partial trailers, or nontrailers (Table 2). Relative to males, a greater proportion of females (75070) showed partial and nontraining responses, but contingency analysis (Sokal and Rohlf, 1969) indicated that this difference was nonsignificant (chi square = 1.79, df = 1, P > 0.20). DISCUSSION The present study showed that garter snakes of both sexes, acclimated and tested under autumnal conditions, can follow the scent trails of a conspecific. This result concurs with the field observation that T. sirtalis followed common and well-defined "travel lanes" in their approach to the den (Costanzo, 1986, 1988) and further supports the hypothesis that pheromones are used for den location by snakes in nature. The present study is the first to provide direct experimental evidence for trail following, in the context of den location, for any colubrid snake. However, Brown and MacLean (1983) convincingly demonstrated that neonate timber rattlesnakes ( Crotalus horridus) from New York followed the trails of litter mates and adults of both sexes during autumn. This behavior apparently was not demonstrated by congeneric (C. viridis) adults from Wyoming, although details of testing protocol and specific results were not reported (King et al., 1983). Graves et al. (1986) later showed that C. viridis neonates were attracted to scents depos- TABLE 2. PERFORMANCE, SEX RATIO, AND MASS DATA FOR GARTER SNAKES TESTED IN LABORATORY ARENA FOR ABILITY TO FOLLOW CONSPECIF1C SCENT TRAILS DURING AUTUMN Males Females Mass (.~ _+ SEM) Performance group ~ N (N) (N) Exact trailers 7 6 1 38.3 + 5.7 Near-exact trailers 4 2 2 26.3 + 6.6 Partial trailers 10 6 4 39.8 3.7 Nontrailers 7 2 5 62.9 ++_ 10.0 asnakes were assigned to four groups on the basis of their inclination to follow scent trails. See text for classification details.

2536 COSTANZO ited by adults and concluded that neonates follow knowledgeable conspecifics short distances to the den site. Among snakes, pheromones facilitate aggregation for both sexual and nonsexual purposes. Nonsexual aggregations probably form when snakes follow conspecifics of both sexes (Burghardt, 1980; Dundee and Miller, 1968; Graves et al.. 1986: Heller and Halpern. 1981); however, during the mating period, the trailing response is clearly sex-limited (i.e., males trail females but not other males, and females do not trail either sex; Ford, 1986). Thus, it is unclear whether separate chemical cues are involved. Although further study is needed to fully characterize these substances, the pheromone permitting C. viridis neonates to locate dens is likely a skin lipid (Graves et al., 1986). Snake locomotion is well suited to the deposition of a continuous scent trail (Ford, 1986), yet the hypothesis of pheromone use for den location is not without conceptual problems. One criticism is that snakes would have an even chance of following a scent trail in the wrong direction. Ford and Lowe (1984), however, cleverly demonstrated that snakes gain directionality information from the differential placement of scent on objects contacted by the trail-forming individual. Nevertheless. the first snake to arrive at a den would not have the benefit of these cues. It is probable that older, experienced snakes may relocate traditional dens using other means, e.g., solar and celestial orientational cues (Landreth. 1973: Newcomer et al., 1974), topographic landmarks (Parker and Brown, 1980), and polarized light (Lawson, 1985). Naive snakes, such as neonates and juveniles (in the case where snakes hibernate apart from adults their first winter; Gregory, 1982, 1984), may rely more heavily on pheromones for den location than do adults. This contention is consonant with the finding in the present study that nontrailing T. sirtalis were significantly (Kruskal-Wallis, P = 0.02) larger (older?) than trailing snakes; it is also supported by the relatively late arrival of smaller snakes at some communal dens (Costanzo, 1986; Gregory, 1982, 1984; Parker and Brown, 1980), and by Lawson's (1985) suggestion that solar orientational abilities are less well developed in younger T. radix. Since many hibernacula are inconspicuous landscape features, orientational cues alone seem insufficient to precisely locate den sites. A more parsimonious explanation is that a combination of orientational, landmark, and pheromonal cues are involved (Brown and MacLean, 1983; Gregory et al., 1987; Hirth, 1966; Lawson, 1985; Parker and Brown, 1980). It is not surprising that snakes inhabiting high latitudes show remarkable abilities to relocate traditional dens during lengthy autumnal migrations. The original n.orthward expansion of species' ranges doubtless depended upon the evolution of ecophysiological adaptations to colder environments and the use of winter refuges that enhanced survival. Although den fidelity typically is high, it is also imperfect (Gregory, 1984). Accordingly, individuals that located (perhaps during summer forays) and used suitable hibernacula likely prospered and

SCENT TRAILING BY GARTER SNAKE 2537 established new communal dens. The progressive northward expansion of snake populations probably was associated with increasing selection pressures for the evolution of effective mechanisms (including pheromones) for den relocation and possibly relied on traditional, communal dens as "stepping stones." This may partly explain why communal denning behavior is more prominent in snake populations near the northern extent of the species' range (Gregory, 1982, 1984). Garter snakes inhabiting milder climates (with presumably less stringent requirements for overwintering sites and consequently a greater abundance of suitable dens) show reduced inclination to trail conspecifics outside of the courtship period (Ford, 1986). The role of pheromones in den location may therefore be of greatest importance in populations inhabiting the harsh environment characteristic of northern latitudes. Most trail-following studies are performed under idealized conditions, thus their results are only suggestive of what may occur in nature. Precipitation decreases the arrival frequency of snakes at communal dens (Costanzo, 1986; Parker and Brown, 1980), perhaps by interfering with pheromone detection. Tongue flicking, a critical element in the pheromone detection process, is slowed dramatically in T. elegans at low body temperatures (Stevenson et al., 1985). Preliminary work in the present investigation showed that, at body temperatures of 5 and 12~ T. sirtalis were largely unresponsive to the scent trails of conspecifics. Future studies should address the influence of environmental factors (e.g., temperature, precipitation, humidity, substrate texture, wind velocity, etc.) on the trail-following ability of snakes, as well as the taxonomic and geographical limits of pheromone use for den location. Acknowledgments--I thank Dr. Dennis L. Claussen, Dr. Douglas H. Taylor, Debbie Holland, and two anonymous reviewers for commenting on this manuscript. This work is a portion of a Ph.D dissertation submitted to the faculty of Miami University, Oxford, Ohio. REFERENCES BROWN, W., and MACLEAN, F. 1983. Conspecific scent-trailing by newborn timber rattlesnakes ( Crotalus horridus). Herpetologica 39:430-435. BUR~HARDT, G.M, 1980. Behavioral and stimulus correlates of vomeronasal functioning in reptiles: Feeding, grouping, sex and tongue use, pp. 275-301, in D. Muller-Schwarze and R. Silverstein (eds.) Chemical Signals: Vertebrates and Aquatic Invertebrates. Plenum, New York. COSTANZO, J. 1986. Influence of hibernaculum microenvironment on the winter life history of the garter snake (Thamnophis sirtalis ). Ohio J. Sci. 86:199-204. COSTANZO, J. 1988. Eco-physiological adaptations to overwintering in the eastern garter snake, Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis. PhD dissertation. Miami University, Oxford, Ohio. DUNDEE, It., and M1LI.ER, M., III. 1968. Aggregative behavior and habitat conditioning by prairie ringneck snakes, Diadophis punctatus arnyi. Tulane Stud. Zool. 15:41-58. DUVALL, D., KIN~, M., and GUTZWtLLER, K. 1985. Behavioral ecology and ethology of the prairie rattlesnake. Nat. Geogr. Res. 1:80-112.

2538 COSTANZO FORD, N. 1981. Seasonality of pheromone trailing behavior in two species of garter snake, Thamnophis (Coluhridae). Southwest. Nat. 26:385-388. For~Ig, N. 1986. The role of pheromone trails in the sociobiology of snakes, pp. 261-278, in D. Duvall, D. Muller-Schwarze, and R. Silverstein (eds.). Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, IV. Plenum, New York. FORD, N., and LowE, J., Jr. 1984. Sex pheromone source location by garter snakes: a mechanism for detection of direction in nonvolatile trails. J. Chem. Ecol. 10:1193-1199. GRAVES, B., DUVALL, D., KING, M., LtNSTEDT, S., and GERN, W. 1986. Initial den location by neonatal prairie rattlesnakes: functions, causes, and natural history in chemical ecology, pp. 285-304, in D. Duvall, D. Muller-Schwarze, and R. Silverstein (eds.). Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, IV. Plenum, New York. GREGORY, P. 1982. Reptilian hibernation, pp. 53-154, in C. Gans and F. Pough (eds.). Biology of the Reptilia, XlII. Academic Press, New York. GREGORY, P. 1984. Communal denning in snakes, pp. 57-75, in R. Seigel, L. Hunt, J. Knight, L. Malaret, and N.Zuschlag (eds.). Vertebrate Ecology and Systematics--A Tribute to Henry S. Fitch. University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas. GREGORY, P., MACARTNEY, J., and LARSEN, K. 1987. Spatial patterns and movements, pp. 366-395, in R. Seigel, J. Collins, and S. Novak (eds.). Snakes: Ecology and Evolutionary Biology. Macmillan, New York. HELLER, S., and HALPERN, M. 1981. Laboratory observations on conspecific and congeneric scent trailing in garter snakes (Thamnophis). Behav. Neurol. Biol. 33:372-377. H1RTH, H. 1966. The ability of two species of snakes to return to a hibemaculum after displacemerit. Southwest. Nat. 1 t:49-53. KING, M., MCCARRON, D., DUVALL, D., BAXTER, G., and GERN, W. 1983. Group avoidance of conspecific but not interspecific chemical cues by prairie rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis). J. Herpetol. 17:196-198. KLAUBER~ L. 1956. Rattlesnakes. Their Habits, Life Histories, and Influence on Mankind, (2 vols.) University of California, Los Angeles. LANDRETH, H. 1973. Orientation and behavior of the rattlesnake, Crotalus atrox. Copeia 1973:26-31. LAWSON, P. 1985. Preliminary investigations into the roles of visual and pheromonal stimuli on aspects of behavior of the western plains garter snake, Thamnophis radix haydeni. MS thesis. University of Regina, Saskatchewan, Canada. NEWCOMER, R., TAYLOR, D., and GUTTMAN, S. 1974. Celestial orientation in two species of water snakes ( Natrix sipedon and Regina septemvittata ). Herpetologica 30:194-204. NOBLE, G., and CLAUSEN, H. 1936. The aggregation behavior of Storeria dekayi and other snakes, with especial reference to the sense organs involved. Ecol. Monogr. 6:271-316. PARKER, W., and BROWN, W. 1980. Comparative ecology of two colubrid snakes, Masticophis t. taeniatus and Pituophis melanoleucus deserticola, in northern Utah. Milwaukee Publ. Mus. Publ. Biol. Geol. No. 7:1-104. SOKAL, R., and ROHLF, F. 1969. Biometry. W.H. Freeman, San Francisco. STEVENSON, R., PETERSON, C., and TsuJI, J. 1985. The thermal dependence of locomotion, tongue flicking, digestion, and oxygen consumption in the wandering garter snake. Physiol. Zool. 58:46-57.