Development of hippocampal specialisation in a food-storing bird

Similar documents
BRIEF REPORT A Brain of Her Own: A Neural Correlate of Song Assessment in a Female Songbird

Gonads and Singing Play Separate, Additive Roles in New Neuron Recruitment in Adult Canary Brain

Genesis and Death of Vocal Control Neurons During Sexual Differentiation in the Zebra Finch

Motor-driven gene expression

state. Results presented here are from birds hatched during the spring of Eggs were marked on the day of laying,

stable (11). This work also reported that the size of HVC mechanisms behind this growth were not determined. Thus,

UTILITY OF THE NEUROLOGICAL EXAMINATION IN RATS

Distribution of Thalamic Projection Neurons to the Wulst in the Japanese Quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica)

DEVELOPMENTAL PLASTICITY IN NEURAL CIRCUITS FOR A LEARNED BEHAVIOR

Stephanie M. Dloniak* and Pierre Deviche*,1

Failure to Detect Seasonal Changes in the Song System Nuclei of the Black-Capped Chickadee (Poecile atricapillus)

Sexual dimorphism in song-induced ZENK expression in the medial striatum of juvenile zebra finches

An Analysis of the Neural Representation of Birdsong Memory

Neural Constraints on the Complexity of Avian Song

Disconnection of a Basal Ganglia Circuit in Juvenile Songbirds Attenuates the Spectral Differentiation of Song Syllables

Conspecific and Heterospecific Song Discrimination in Male Zebra Finches with Lesions in the Anterior Forebrain Pathway

Testosterone Regulates the Activity and Expression of Aromatase in the Canary Neostriatum

Putative adult neurogenesis in two domestic pigeon breeds (Columba livia domestica): racing homer versus utility carneau pigeons

Melanie F. Guigueno, 1,2 David F. Sherry, 1,2,3 Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton 1,2,3 ABSTRACT: INTRODUCTION

Song Learning, Early Nutrition and Sexual Selection in Songbirds 1

Male song quality affects circulating but not yolk steroid concentrations in female canaries (Serinus canaria)

For Whom The Bird Sings: Context-Dependent Gene Expression

Homing in Pigeons: The Role of the Hippocampal Formation in the Representation of Landmarks Used for Navigation

ZENK Protein Regulation by Song in the Brain of Songbirds

The effect of testosterone injections on aggression and begging behaviour of black headed gull chicks (Larus ridibundus)

The Brain and Senses. Birds perceive the world differently than humans. Avian intelligence. Novel feeding behaviors

Visual and song nuclei correlate with courtship skills in brown-headed cowbirds

A SINGLE VIBRISSAL COLUMN IN THE FIRST SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX OF THE MOUSE DEMONSTRATED WITH 2-DEOXYGLUCOSE

Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males*

Invertebrates. Brain. Brain 12/2/2017. The Invertebrate Brain. The Invertebrate Brain. Invertebrate brain general layouts some specific functions

Early Condition, Song Learning, and the Volume of Song Brain Nuclei in the Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata)

The effects of Corticosterone on Neuronal Migration in Zebra Finches

What causes lizards brains to change size?

The role of visual landmarks in the avian familiar area map

University of Groningen

Sexually Dimorphic Neurocalcin Expression in the Developing Zebra Finch Telencephalon

Sex Difference in the Size of the Neural Song Control Regions in a Dueting Songbird with Similar Song Repertoire Size of Males and Females

ANALYSIS OF GROWTH OF THE RED-TAILED HAWK 1

experimental studies of many workers (Hetherington, 1941; Hetherington & nucleus, resulted in obesity. The confusion introduced by the notion

Sexual Differentiation of the Zebra Finch Song System Parallels Genetic, Not Gonadal, Sex

The Role of Auditory Experience in the Formation of Neural Circuits Underlying Vocal Learning in Zebra Finches

Distribution of Substance P Reveals a Novel Subdivision in the Hippocampus of Parasitic South American Cowbirds

Reports hom the Research Laboratories... of the Department of Psychiatry University of Minnesota

Alternatives in Veterinary Anatomy Training

RESEARCH AND PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE

TERRITORIAL BEHAVIOR AND CORTICAL BRAIN PLASTICITY IN ADULT MALE SCELOPORUS OCCIDENTALIS. A Thesis. presented to

Cultures, genes, and neurons in the development of song and singing in brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater )

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

NEURAL RESPONSES TO AUDITORY RHYTHMS IN THE ZEBRA FINCH. Jennifer A. Lampen

Effects of monocular viewing on orientation in an arena at the release site and homing performance in pigeons

Localized Changes in Immediate-Early Gene Regulation during Sensory and Motor Learning in Zebra Finches

Pigeons with ablated pyriform cortex home from familiar but not from unfamiliar sites

Arrhythmic Song Exposure Increases ZENK Expression in Auditory Cortical Areas and Nucleus Taeniae of the Adult Zebra Finch

The Laminar and Size Distribution of Commissural Efferent Neurons in the Cat Visual Cortex*

Brain gene regulation by territorial singing behavior in freely ranging songbirds

A Contribution to the Study of the Afferents to the Pigeon Optic Tectum

Course Curriculum for Master Degree Theriogenology & Artificial Insemination/Faculty of Veterinary Medicine

Nestling Vocalization Development in the European Starling (Sturnus vulgaris) By Ceilidh Dorothea McCoombs

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

A comparison of placental tissue in the skinks Eulamprus tympanum and E. quoyii. Yates, Lauren A.

PROBABLE NON-BREEDERS AMONG FEMALE BLUE GROUSE

Are There Place Cells in the Avian Hippocampus?

Interactions Between Nerve Growth Factor Binding and Estradiol in Early Development of the Zebra Finch Telencephalon

Rudimentary substrates for vocal learning in a suboscine

VIRIDOR WASTE MANAGEMENT LIMITED. Parkwood Springs Landfill, Sheffield. Reptile Survey Report

NEUROENDOCRINE CONTROL OF SONG IN THE DARK-EYED JUNCO {JUNCO H YEM A LIS) Stephanie Marie Dloniak

A Monoclonal Antibody Specific to a Song System Nuclear Antigen in Estrildine Finches

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar

Does begging affect growth in nestling tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor?

Optoacoustic imaging of an animal model of prostate cancer

Title. CitationPLoS One, 7(8): e Issue Date Doc URL. Rights(URL) Type.

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

AGRICULTURE, LIVESTOCK AND FISHERIES

Course Curriculum for Master Degree in Poultry Diseases/Veterinary Medicine

Evolution of Mating system: A Game Theory

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALS AND METHODS

Sexual preferences for mate song in female canaries (Serinus canaria)

This article is downloaded from.

Behavioural Processes

F.L. Andr6s. Rua Tristao Vaz No Esq., 1400 Lisboa, Portugal

September Population analysis of the Spaniel (English Springer) breed

1.14 Infanticide by a male lion. Bad fathers in wild life

September Population analysis of the Boxer breed

September Population analysis of the Maltese breed

Ames, IA Ames, IA (515)

September Population analysis of the Poodle (Standard) breed

Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds

IDR : VOL. 10, NO. 1, ( JANUARY-JUNE, 2012) : ISSN :

September Population analysis of the Chesapeake Bay Retriever breed

THE discovery of male sterile individuals

Human-Animal Interactions in the Turkey Industry

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

Naturalised Goose 2000

Afferent Connections of the Ectostriatum and Visual Wulst in the Zebra Finch

September Population analysis of the Dalmatian breed

(199) THE HATCHING AND FLEDGING OF SOME COOT

September Population analysis of the Borzoi breed

AGGRESSIVE DISPLAY OF THE CORN-CRAKE.

THE INTERACTIONS BETWEEN GENOTYPES AND HOUSING ENVIRONMENTS IN THE DOMESTIC HEN

Transcription:

Behavioural Brahz Research, 53 (1993) 127-131 9 1993 lsevier Science Publishers B.V. All rights reserved. 166-4328/93/$6. 127 BBR 142 Development of hippocampal specialisation in a food-storing bird S.D. H e a l y a n d J.R. K r e b s Department of Zoology and Department of Pharmacology, Oxford University, Oxford (ILK) (Received 3 August 1992) (Revised version received 5 October 1992) (Accepted 2 October 1992) Key words: Food-storing; Ilippocampus; Avian; Development; Neuron number Previous studies demonstrated that amongst food-storing passerine birds tile hippocanlpal region (dorso-medial cortex) is enlarged relative to the rest of the telencephalon. It has been hypothesised that this hippocanlpal specialisation is related to the spatial memory requirements of retrieving large numbers of stored items. Here we compare the development of the hippocampus in a food-storing and a non-storing corvid, the s of which' differ in relative hippocampal volume. The volume, cell density and number of cells in the hippocampal region of (5-25 days post hatching) and (> 32 days old) magpies Pica pica (food-storing) and jackdaws Corvus monedula (non-storing) were measured. In both species the volume of the hippocampus increases with the volume of the rest of the telencephalon during the growth phase. The relative volume of the hippocampus in 5- to 25-day-old s of the two species does not differ significantly. In the food-storing magpie, the relative volume of the hippocampus is significantly larger than that of s, whilst in the jackdaw, s and s do not differ. The density of neurons declines with increasing age and this effect is more marked in jackdaws than in magpies. Neuron number did not change significantly with age, but is significantly greater in magpies than in jackdaws. These results are discussed in relation to the possibility that changes in hippocampal volume and cell number are related to the use of spatial naemory in retrieving stored food. INTRODUCTION Previous studies have shown that amongst foodstoring passerine birds the dorsomedial cortex or hippocampal region is enlarged relative to the rest of the telencephalon in comparison with non-food-storing species ~2'22. This has occurred in at least three independent evolutionary lines (the titmice Paridae, corvids Corvidae and nuthatches Sittidae), suggesting an anatomical specialisation of the brain associated with storing behaviour. The hippocampal region plays a role in forming memories for stored food ]4'2] and the enlargement of the hippocampus of food-storers may be an adaptation for processing the large amounts of information involved in storage and retrieval of food t ~,t3 In the present study we examine how the difference between a storer and a non-storer in relative hippocampal volume arises during development. Although the ontogeny of food-storing behaviour has been documented in detail in only a few species 5'8 (for review see ref. 24), it is known that young birds do not store until after leaving the nest. In titmice, where the most detailed work has been done, birds start to store soon Correspomlence: S.D. tlealy, Department of Zoology, Oxford University, South Parks Road, Oxford, OXI 3PS, UK. after independence from the parents, i.e. about 2 weeks after leaving the nest 5. Thus by examining the relative hippocampal volume of storing and non-storing birds before and after leaving the nest, it is possible to ascertain whether or not the enlargement of the hippocampal region in storing species precedes the onset of storing behaviour. We examined the relative hippocampal volume, cell density and cell number of storing and non-storing species of corvid. The magpie Pica pica is known to store throughout the year and probably retrieves its cached food within a few hours to days of storing z. Tile jackdaw Corvus monedula rarely if ever stores food in the wild (review in ref. 9). Adults of the former have a significantly larger hippocampus than do s of the latter 9. MATRIALS AND MTIIODS Preparation of material Twenty-eight jackdaws (mean weight of s 22 g) and 25 magpies (mean weight of s 185 g) were sacrificed with an intraperitoneal overdose of sodium pentobarbitone and perfused transcardiaily with.75 ~o saline followed by 1~ formal saline. Following perfusion the brains were dissected out and postfixed for

128 7 days in 1~o formal saline before being transferred to 3~/o sucrose formalin. The brains were subsequently cut as frozen sections of either 5 pm or 25 #m thickness in the coronal plane. very fifth section (5/~m) or every tenth section (25 itm) was stained with Cresyl violet. Fourteen of the jackdaws and 1 of the magpies were taken under licence from the nest during April and May 1988, 1989 and 199 and ranged in age from ca. 5 to ca. 25 days posthatching. The remaining 15 magpies and 14jackdaws were s, at least 32 days old, collected under licence at the same season and in the same years as the s. Vohmletric analysis Hippocampal and telencephalon (excluding the hippocampal region) volumes were measured by tracing outlines under 1 x magnification with a photographic enlarger and digitising the areas on each section to compute volumes using the formula for a truncated cone. The definition of the boundaries of the hippocampal region followed those described by refs. 7, 12, 15. The volumemeasurements were logarithmically transformed and: analysed by stepwise multiple regression with hippocampal volume as the dependent variable and three independent variables: telencephalon volume, storer/non-storer (coded as a dummy variate) and age (, ) also coded as a dummy variate. Neuron density and number Neuron densities for a random subsample of 24 birds (six s, aged 1-25 days posthatching, and six s of each species) were estimated by counting profiles in a square graticule measuring 1 i/tm in a single plane of focus in the Nissl-stained sections and applying the standard correction factors to obtain an estimate of neuron density ~'1~ Neurons were identified by their pale nucleus and more darkly stained nucleolus: only cells with a visible nucleolus in the sample frame were included. The hippocampal formation is a heterogeneous structure in terms of its cell types and cell density 7. We therefore sampled three different areas of the hippocampal region: the medial arm of the V-shaped structure of densely packed cells (area 2 of ref. 7); the dorsomedial region (area 4 of ref. 7) and the dorsolateral region (area 6 of ref. 7) (Fig. 1). These regions were sampled at three levels of the rostro-caudal axis of the hippocampal formation corresponding approximately to the following levels of the canary atlas 23. A 3.5, characterised by the first appearance of the septo-mesencephalic tract; A 1.6, corresponding to the appearance of the anterior commissure; and A.4, recognised by the first appearance of the cerebellum. In each region at each level on the rostral-caudal axis, H A i n - n / J ven,ral i dorsomiiiil...l... " i, K1V l~.s. I Fig. 1. A schematic of the hippocampal region in coronal section to illustrate the sampling regions used for cell density estimates. The set of three squares in a row (not to scale) indicate the sampling regions. HA, hyperstriatum accessorium; HV, hyperstriatum ventrale; V, lateral ventricle; S, septum. three adjacent fields of cells in the left hippocampus were counted at 4 x magnification. ach field contained approximately 1 cells, thus the total number of cells counted per birds was about 9 per section (3 3 regions) or 2,7 in total. In a preliminary analysis of the data, the 3 sampling regions and the three rostro-caudal sections were considered separately, but no differences in the patterns reported below were seen between these subsamples, so all data were combined for the main analysis. The cell counting was done by using an image processing system (Image 1.4) to display the field on the screen of a Macintosh II computer. The images were enhanced to sharpen the nucleolus. The average density estimated from nine sampling areas in each bird was multiplied by hippocampal volume to obtain an estimate of the total number of neurones in the hippocampal formation for each individual. The method we used is similar to that used in most previous studies of neuron density and number, but recent developments of more sophisticated methods such as the optical dissector, have shown that conventional techniques are biased 6"26'28. Thus the measurements provide only an approximate estimate oftotal cell number and therefore may not detect subtle differences. RSULTS Vohtmetric changes In the multiple regression of hippocampal volume on telencephalon, species and age, there was a signifi-

129 cant effect of telencephalon volume (F1,47 = 823.679, P =. I), age (cumulative, ll,4 7 = 3.614, P =.1), a significant species effect (Fi.47=43.65, P=.1), and a significant interaction between species and age (Ft,47 = 12.418, P =.1). The results are displayed in Fig. 2A,B, which shows that during the late stage the relative hippocampal volume of the storing and non-storing species is similar, whilst in the s there is a striking difference: in the non-storing jackdaw, hippocampal volume relative to telencephalon volume remains the same as in the late stage, whilst in the magpie, relative hippocampal volume increases (Fig. 2A). When the and birds were analysed separately the difference between species 12 1 8 "-6 6-. ~ 8 = 4-2" A f ~.2 o "~.1 -'1 8 o.o. ~..,...~. -.1 -.2 O o < r,lr-i 9 Q I~1 9 r'n I"1 o o o ~! I J 1 2 3 4 telencephalon volume (nun 3 ) jackdaws 1 magpies t! -.3 m, Fig. 2. A: a plot of hippocampal volume as a function of volume of the rest of the telencephalon (correlated with age) showing that in magpies but not in jackdaws there is an increase in relative hippocampal volume after 25 days. Solid squares: 5- to 25-day-old jackdaws; open squares: >32-day-old jackdaws, solid circles: 5- to 25-day-old magpies; open circles: > 32-day-old magpies. B: hippocanlpal volume relative to telencephalon volume in s and s. The values are deviations from the regression of a log-log plot. B ~ was not significant in the former, although there was a trend for magpies to have a larger relative hippocampus (F2.2~ = 3.318, P =.83) and highly significant in the latter (F2.25 = 133.67, P =.1) (Fig. 2B). Neuron density attd mtmber Fig. 3A,B shows the results of the estimates of density and number of neurons. Neuron density declined with age (F~.2o = 7.24, P=.14; two-way ANOVA), did not differ overall between species (F~,2o = 1.29, P =.269) and showed a weak, non-significant trend towards a greater decline with age in jackdaws than in m a g p i e s (Ft,2o = 2.36; P =.169) (Fig. 3A). When the two species were analysed separately, jackdaws showed a significant decline with age (Fl,tl = 7.845, P =.19), whilst magpies did not (F~,~ = 1.484, P =.251). Total cell number did not vary with age, but overall magpies had more cells than did jackdaws (Fig. 3B) (age: Fi.2o =.368, P =.551; species: FL.zo -- 11.732, P =.3; interaction: F~,2o =.171; P =.683). When e-- s e ',,...,.o e-, 6 7 6-5- 4-3 2 1 4-,--1 o 3~ " e - m tfi 2- e-- 1oo A 13 9 <: M l magpie [] jackdaw [] magpie [] jackdaw Fig. 3. A: cell density and (B) cell number in the hippocampal region of (1-25 days) and (> 32 days) magpies and jackdaws.

13 s and s were analysed separately, there was no significant difference between s (FI,I 1 = 3.39) but magpies had significantly more cells than did jackdaws (Fl,ll = 14.5, P =.3). DISCUSSION The main results of tiffs study are as follows: (i) The difference between s of the non-storing jackdaw and the food-storing magpie in relative hippocampal volume and neuron number arises partly between 5 and 25 days posthatching but largely as a result of divergence after ftedging (between 35 and 365 days); (ii)in jackdaws, but not magpies, neuron density declines with age; (iii) magpies have a larger total number of neurons in the hippocampal region than do jackdaws. Previous work has shown that differences between I species of passerine birds in relative volume of the hippocampal region is associated with food-storing behaviour 9'~2'22. Here we show that, at least in one comparison between a storer and a non-storer, the difference arises at a relatively late stage of development, after the birds are fully grown and have left the nest. This raises the possibility that development of the hippocampal region proceeds in parallel with behavioural development and the use of spatial memory in storing and retrieving food. Although early workers pointed to possible correlations between neuron number and learning ability in amphibians 25 and rats 3 (for review see ref. 27), these studies had many confounding variables. The most detailed more recent studies of correlations between learning and changes in vohtme of brain nuclei and associated recruitment and loss of neurons are those of the passerine song control system. The male zebra finch learns its song in a sensitive phase between the ages of 2 (immediately postfledging) and 65 days. During this period, the volume of two song control nuclei, HVC and Area X, both of which are thought to play a role in memorisation and/or production of songs, increases rapidly in males but not in females ~6. This increase in volume is associated with a substantial increase in neuron number ~6, with relatively little change in neuron density. The change in neuron number is presumed to reflect neurogenesis, which in one study accounted for about 2~o of neurons in H V C tt. In another song nucleus, LMAN, which from lesion studies plays a particular role in early memorisation of songs, the number of neurons and the size of the nucleus decrease in males (but not in females) during the sensitive phase 4.t6. The decrease ofneurons in the male is due to cell death, perhaps, by analogy with other systems, under hormonal control t8. Thus memorisation and development of motor production of songs in the young male zebra finch is accompanied by both neurogenesis and by cell death. It is still not clear how these changes are causally related t 9 memorisation, although in the swamp sparrow, changes in volume and cell number of the song control)nuclei occur during the memorisation phase (2-6 days of age) and not in the motor production phase, which in this species does not start until the age of 275 days 19. A further complication is that neurogenesis also occurs in zebra finches of both sexes, well after the sensitive phase for learning song for motor production, but perhaps associated with learning to recognise songs 2~ At the moment we do not have sufficiently detailed data on the dual ontogeny of food-storing behaviour and brain structure to make detailed comparisons with the findings on the passerine song system. However, in broad terms, our results suggest that the emergence of a structural difference between two species in the brain is related to the emergence of a behavioural difference. At the level of volumetric measurement, the difference between jackdaws and magpies is that the magpie hippocampus appears to grow relative to the rest of the brain between 35 and 32 days, whilst the jackdaw hippocampus does not. This difference is not mirrored by a greater increase in neuron number in the magpie than in the jackdaw, but rather by a greater cell loss in the latter. Further experimental work, in which the opportunity to store and retrieve food during development is manipulated, will be necessary to further evaluate the relationship between food storing memory and hippocampal development. ACKNOWLDGMNTS This work was supported by SRC (S.D.H. and J.R.K.), St John's College Oxford (S.D.H.) and the Royal Society (J.R.K.). We thank the following for helpful comments and advice: Hugh Perry, Kevin Martin, Jonathan Newman, Andrea Szrkely and Nicky Clayton. Tim Birkhead, Philip Heeb and Frank Cezilly helped to provide birds. RFRNCS 1 Abcrcrombie, M. stimation of nuclear populations from microtome sections, Anat. Rec., 94 (1946) 239-247. 2 Birkhead, T., The magpies, T. & A.D. Poyser, London, 199. 3 Block, J.B. and ssman, W.B., Growth hormone administration during pregnancy: a behavioural difference in offspring rats, Nature, 27 (1965) 1136-1137.

131 4 Bottjer, S.W. and Sengelaub, D.R., Cell death during development ofa forebrain nucleus involved with vocal learning in zebra finches, J. NeurobioL, 2 (1989) 69-618. 5 Clayton, N.S., Development of food-storing behaviour in marsh tits, Behaviour, 12, in press. 6 Coggeshall, R.., A consideration of neural counting methods, Trends Neurosci., 15 (1992) 9-13. 7 richsen, J.T., Bingrnan, V.P. and Krebs, J.R., The distribution of neuropeptides in the dorsomedial telenccphalon of the pigeon (Cohtmba livia): a basis for regional subdivision, J. Comp. NeuroL, 314 (1991) 478-492. 8 Haftorn, S., Ontogeny of food storing in titmice Pants spp. Ibis, 134 (1992) 69-71. 9 tlealy, S.D. and Krebs, J.R., Food storing and the hippocampus in corvids: anlount and volume are correlated, Proc. R. Soc. B., 248 (1992) 241-245. I Konigsmark, B.W., In W.J.H. Nauta and S.O.. bbeson (ds.) Contemporary Research 3Iethods in Neuroanatomy, Springer, Berlin, 197.! 1 Krebs, J.R., Fod-storing birds: adaptive specialisation in brain and behaviour?, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B., 329 (199) 153-16. 12 Krebs, J.R., Sherry, D.F., llealy, S.D., Perry, V.11. and Vaccarino, A.L., I lippocampal specialisation of food-storing birds, Proc. NatL Acad. Sci. USA, 86 (1989) 1388-1392. 13 Krebs, J.R., tlilton, S.C. and ttealy, S.D., Memory in foodstoring birds: adaptive specialisation in brain and behaviour? In G.M. delman, W.. Gall and W.M. Cowan (ds.) Signal and Sense: Local alid Global Order ht Perceptual Maps, Wiley-Liss, New York, 199, pp. 475-5. 14 Krushinskaya, N.L., Some complex fornls of feeding behaviour of nutcracker Nucifraga caryocatactes, after removal of old cortex, Zh. vol. Biochhn. FisioL, 11 (1966)563-568. 15 Montagnese, C.M., Krebs, J.R., Szrkely, A.D. and Csillag, A., A sub-population of large calbindin-like immunopositive neurones is present in the hippocampal formation in food-storing but not in non-storing species of bird, Brahz Res., in press. 16 Nordeen, K.W. and Nordeen,.J., Sex and regional differences in the incorporation of neurons born during song learning in zebra finches, J. Neurosci., 8 (1988) 2869-2874. 17 Nordeen, K.W. and Nordeen,.J., Projection neurons within a vocal motor pathway are born during song learning in zebra finches, Nature, 334 (1988) 149-151. 18 Nordcen,.J., Nordeen, K.W., Sengelaub, D.R. and Arnold, A.P., Androgens prevent normally occurring cell death in a sexually dimorphic spinal nucleus, Science, 229 (1988) 671-673. 19 Nordeen, K.W., Marler, P.R. and Nordeen,.J., Addition of song related neurons in swamp sparrows coincides with memorisation, not production, of learned song, J. NeurobioL, 2 (1989) 651-661. 2 Nottebohm, F., Alvarez Buylla, Cynx, J., Kirn, J., Ling, C.-Y., Nottebohm, M., Surer, R., ToIles, A. and Williams, H., Song learning in birds: the relation between perception and production, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B., 329 (199) 115-124. 21 Sherry, D.F. and Vaccarino, A.L., ttippocampus and memory for food caches in black-capped chickadees, Behav. Neurosck, 13 (1989) 31-318. 22 Sherry, D.F., Vaccarino, A.L., Buckcnham, K. and Herz, R.S., The hippocampal complex of food-storing birds, Brahz Behav. vol, 34 (1989) 38-317. 23 Stokes, T.M., Leonard, C.M. and Nottebohm, F., The telencephalon, diencephalon and mesencephalon of the canary Serinus canaria in stercotaxic co-ordinates, J. Comp. NeuroL, 156 (1976) 337-374. 24 Vander Wall, S.B., Food lloarding ht Anhnals, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 199. 25 Vernon, J.A. and Butsch, J., ffect of tetraploidy on learning and retention in the salamander, Science, 125 (1957) 133-134. 26 West, M.J. and Gunderson, II.J.G., Unbiased stereological estimation of the number of neurons in the human hippocampus, J. Comp. Neurol., 296 (1991) 1-22. 27 Williams, R.W. and tlcrrup, C., The control of neuron number, Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 11 (1988)423-453. 28 Williams, R.W. and Rakie, P., Three-dimensional counting: an accurate and direct method to estimate numbers of cells in sectioned material, J. Comp. NeuroL, 278 (1988) 344-352.