Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Bachok Campus, Kelantan, Malaysia

Similar documents
Dangerous Wild Animals (Northern Ireland) Order Guidance on the keeping of Ostrich and Emus

Behaviour of captive Ostrich chicks from 10 days to 5 months of age

THE BEHAVIOR OF OSTRICHES IN CAPTIVITY

> BACK TO CONTENTS PAGE

Unit 3 Sustainability and interdependence Sub Topic 3.4: Animal welfare

Production Basics How Do I Raise Poultry for Eggs?

Poultry Skillathon 2016

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema

REARING LAYING HENS IN A BARN SYSTEM WITHOUT BEAK TRIMMING: THE RONDEEL EXAMPLE

STANDING COMMITTEE OF THE EUROPEAN CONVENTION FOR THE PROTECTION OF ANIMALS KEPT FOR FARMING PURPOSES

SHORT DESCRIPTION OF TECHNICAL PAPER CONTENT

Ratite Standards and Guidelines

Slide 1 NO NOTES. Slide 2 NO NOTES. Slide 3 NO NOTES. Slide 4 NO NOTES. Slide 5

Endoparasites and Associated Worm Burden of Captive and Free-Living Ostriches (Struthio camelus) in the Semi-Arid Region of North Eastern Nigeria

CIWF Response to the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply Study April 2015

Animal Study: Adelaide Zoo

CODE OF RECOMMENDATIONS FOR THE WELFARE OF PET GERBILS DUTY OF CARE TO A PET GERBIL UNDER THE ANIMAL WELFARE (GUERNSEY) ORDINANCE, 2012

The welfare of laying hens

The 1999 EU Hens Directive bans the conventional battery cage from 2012.

RECENT ADVANCES IN OSTRICH NUTRITION IN SOUTH AFRICA: EFFECT OF DIETARY ENERGY AND PROTEIN LEVEL ON THE PERFORMANCE OF GROWING OSTRICHES

Laura M. Dixon a & Ian J. H. Duncan a a Department of Animal and Poultry Science,

PRODUCTION BASICS HOW DO I RAISE POULTRY FOR MEAT? Chuck Schuster University of Maryland Extension Central Maryland

Ardea herodias (Great Blue Heron)

FRENZ. World Leading Poultry Layer Standard

4-H Poultry Showmanship Questions

Back to basics - Accommodating birds in the laboratory setting

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

Behavioural effects of food deprivation on red junglefowl (Gallus gallus) and White Leghorn layers. Jenny Lind

Coalition for a Sustainable Egg Supply Richard Blatchford University of California, Davis

Proposed Draft Australian Animal Welfare Standards And Guidelines For Poultry. Submission from the Australian Veterinary Association Ltd

Does it matter if she can t?

A.13 BLAINVILLE S HORNED LIZARD (PHRYNOSOMA BLAINVILLII)

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Caprimulgiformes Family: Podargidae Scientific Name: Podargus strigoides Common Name: Tawny frogmouth

Poultry Skillathon 2017

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

(c) Ensure that necessary and standard veterinary care is provided in a timely manner

Unit A: Introduction to Poultry Science. Lesson 1: Exploring the Poultry Industry

Iguana aggression. A relaxed green iguana. Defensive aggression

Mental stim ulation it s not just for dogs!! By Danielle Middleton- Beck BSc hons, PGDip CABC

The Life of a Battery Hen Sadia Ahmed

feather pecking. Animal Needs Index focuses on housing and management and the plumage

Steggles Sydney Royal School Meat Bird Pairs Competition Support Guide

Effects of a Pre-Molt Calcium and Low-Energy Molt Program on Laying Hen Behavior During and Post-Molt

Presence of Males Within Laying Hens Affects Tonic Immobility Response and Sociality

CODE OF RECOMMENDATIONS FOR THE WELFARE OF PET HAMSTERS DUTY OF CARE TO A PET HAMSTER UNDER THE ANIMAL WELFARE (GUERNSEY) ORDINANCE, 2012

Relationship between hen age, body weight, laying rate, egg weight and rearing system

R A I S I N G Y O U R H O M E C H I C K E N F L O C K

The Livestock & Poultry Industries-I

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

Dimethyl anthranilate based repellents affect cage pecking and feather condition of laying hens

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Anseriformes Family: Anatidae Scientific Name: Mergus squamatus Common Name: Scaly-sided (Chinese) Merganser

This article is downloaded from.

Birds Birds are vertebrates (animals with backbones) with wings and feathers. Most birds can fly, using powerful muscles to flap their wings.

Behaviour of Hens in Cages

Kori Bustard Husbandry. Sara Hallager, Biologist, Smithsonian National Zoological Park

H POULTRY PROJECT

Raising Pastured Poultry in Texas. Kevin Ellis NCAT Poultry Specialist

Effect of Calcium Level of the Developing and Laying Ration on Hatchability of Eggs and on Viability and Growth Rate of Progeny of Young Pullets 1

Scottish Natural Heritage Diversionary feeding of hen harriers on grouse moors. a practical guide

POULTRY STANDARDS The focus of PROOF certification is the on. farm management of livestock in a farming

Education. ESL-Advance

Atlantic Puffins By Guy Belleranti

Laying Hen Welfare. Janice Siegford. Department of Animal Science

Observations on management and production of local chickens kept in Muy Muy, Nicaragua. H. de Vries

ì<(sk$m)=bdddid< +^-Ä-U-Ä-U

Sick Chicken Symptoms You Should Recognize

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist

Housing for Health, Wellness and Success: Standards for Facility Design and Environment. What is a healthy environment made of?

CITY OF CHARLES STURT DOMESTIC LIVESTOCK MANAGEMENT BY-LAW By-law No. 6 of 2014

Scholarship 2017 Biology

Rabbit Scenario: Laboratory vs. Fancier

Anas clypeata (Northern Shoveler)

Section 6. Embryonic Development and Hatchery Management Notes

Care For Us Re#culated Python (Python re/culatus)

Feathered, But Not Ready for Takeoff

Breeding and Managing Pheasants

Chameleons: Biology, Husbandry and Disease Prevention. Paul Stewart, DVM. Origin: Africa (40% of species) and Madagascar (40% of species)

AGONISTIC AND SEXUAL BEHAVIOR OF THE AFRICAN OSTRICH

Going on a Zoo Animal Hunt

The puff adder is a large, sluggish, thick-bodied snake that rarely exceeds a meter in length.

COMMON LABORATORY ANIMALS RODENTS

MORPHOLOGICAL DESCRIPTION OF THE DEVELOPING OSTRICH EMBRYO: A TOOL FOR EMBRYONIC AGE ESTIMATION

How should we treat farm animals? Egg production worksheet Do you agree or disagree with these systems of egg production. Are some better than others?

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Struthioniformes Family: Rheidae Scientific Name: Rhea pennata Common Name: Lesser Rhea

4-H Poultry: Unit 1. The Egg Flock For an egg-producing flock, select one of these birds: production-type Rhode Island Red Leghorn hybrids sex-link

Executive Summary. December Page 2

Sulcata Tortoise. Scientific Name: Geochelone [Centrochelys] Sulcata

Animal Care & Selection

Assorted Guinea Brown egg layers: Black Sex Link Golden Sex Link Red Sex Link

Key considerations in the breeding of macaques and marmosets for scientific purposes

Back to Basics: Mouse Husbandry. Tamara Godbey Clinical Veterinarian University of British Columbia

Urges, Needs, Preferences, Priorities Coming to Terms with the Welfare of Hens

MANAGING AVIARY SYSTEMS TO ACHIEVE OPTIMAL RESULTS. TOPICS:

Trunk Contents. Crane Flight Feathers (3)

Right and next page: Brahma chicks with decent footfeathering, but with no fluff on the inner side of the legs and on the inner toes.

BREEDING AND GENETICS. Comparative Evaluation of Three Commercial Broiler Stocks in Hot Versus Temperate Climates

Egg laying site preferences in Pterostichus melanarius Illiger (Coleoptera: Carabidae)

INTERNATIONAL INDIAN SCHOOL- RIYADH HALF YEARLY WORKSHEET CLASS: III. SUBJECT: EVS LESSONS: 3,4,5,6,7,12,17

STANDING COMMITTEE OF THE EUROPEAN CONVENTION FOR THE PROTECTION OF ANIMALS KEPT FOR FARMING PURPOSES (T-AP)

SOUTH AFRICAN NATIONAL STANDARD

Transcription:

Behaviour of Captive Ostriches (Struthio camelus) at Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Bachok Campus, Kelantan, Malaysia Kamarul Hambali*, Nadia Zakaria, Norashikin Fauzi, Aainaa Amir Faculty of Earth Science, Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Jeli Campus, 17600 Jeli, Kelantan, Malaysia. Received 31 March 2015 Accepted 26 April 2015 Available online 25 May 2015 Keywords: Captive ostriches, behavioural patterns, Bachok Campus, Kelantan, Malaysia *Corresponding author: Dr. Kamarul Hambali, Faculty of Earth Science, Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Jeli Campus, 17600 Jeli, Kelantan, Malaysia. Email: kamarul@umk.edu.my A b stract The behavioural patterns of six captive ostriches (Struthio camelus) were studied at Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Bachok Campus between July and September 2014. The main purpose of the study was to determine the behavioural patterns of ostriches in captivity to provide the useful information for better management action. The ostriches were observed daily for 11 hours from 0700 hrs until 1800 hrs for 45 days by using the focal sampling technique. Six categories of behavioural patterns were observed namely resting, locomotion, ingestive, comfort, aggression and thermoregulation. The highest behaviour observed was walking compared to other behaviours, while the lowest was running. The result of the study showed a significant difference of behavioural patterns of captive ostriches between different observational periods except for locomotion and aggression behaviours. The finding of the study showed that ostriches reared in captivity possessed similar behavioural patterns to those remaining in the wild, although some captive circumstances imposed minor differences. 1. Introduction Struthio camelus is originated from the Greek words meaning of camel sparrow given by the Swedish scientist, Carl Linnaeus. Struthio means "ostrich" and camelus means "camel" based on the similarities between the ostrich and camel; inhabit dry habitat, long neck and two strong, long legs. The ostrich (Struthio camelus) is a sole representative of the order of birds known as ratites (running birds) or Struthioniformes; the heaviest and largest living bird in the wild (Hallam, 1992; Alden et al., 1996). They are flightless birds and the only extant bird with two toes on each foot (Schaller et al., 2011). It is the largest flightless, herbivorous bird, found in a range of open habitat types and is endemic to Africa (Brown et al., 1982). They have long neck and legs and the fastest land speed of bird with the speed up to about 70 km/h 13 (43 mph) (Davies, 2003). They can lay the largest eggs that can weigh up to 1.4 kilograms and the ostrich itself usually weigh from 63 to 103 kilograms or as much as two adult humans (Davies, 2003). For the life span, the maximum longevity reportedly is 50 years (Human Ageing Genomic Resources, 2006). The meaning of behaviour is a response in a particular way to the environmental conditions. Ostriches in the wild habitat have certain behavioural patterns that enable them to adapt to their natural environment. However, behavioural patterns in captivity may differ due to different environmental factors. Normal behaviour of ostrich are usually relate to their daily living activities like socialization, feeding, communication, grooming and sexual activities. Among of the activities are twirling, thermoregulatory, pecking, grooming, trembling, and

threat. As referred to the pecking behaviour, according to Newberry et al., (2007), in the wild, pecking is a natural behaviour that leads to the establishment of a pecking order in the group. While for the abnormal behaviours, captive environments can cause abnormality and several studies have already shown the effect of enclosure size in the behaviour of captive animals (Fisher at al., 1980; Estep et al., 1978; Innis et al., 1985; Hogan et al., 1988; Jensvold et al., 2001). Among of the examples are feather-pecking, toe and face pecking, anorexia and many others. Therefore, this study aimed to investigate the behaviour of ostriches in the enclosure to get useful information and deficiencies that can be overcome to serve as a guide to better management of this species in the future. 2. Materials and Methods The data were recorded by using focal sampling method. The observation was conducted at Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Bachok Campus, Kelantan, Malaysia (6 00'12.0"N 102 24'06.4"E) where the ostriches were selected as the study subject (Fig. 1). A total of six ostriches were kept in captivity and they were comprised of three males and females respectively. The observations were conducted accordingly for every 30 minutes intervals started from 0700 hrs until 1800 hrs. The observations were carried out at three different periods; morning started from 0700 hrs to 1030 hrs, afternoon from 1100 hrs to 1430 hrs and evening from 1500 hrs to 1800 hrs. The comparison of behaviours against periods of time was calculated using Chi-square test. Figure 1: Study area (source: Google Earth). 3. Results and Discussion Resting behaviour of ostriches (S. camelus) can be observed when they stopped moving from doing any activities. There are three positions that demonstrated the resting behaviour of the ostriches namely standing, sitting and sleeping. From the results, the most frequent resting behaviour observed was standing (92%) followed by sitting (8%). These results are similar with William et al., (1993), ostrich normally spent about 16% of their normal daily behaviour on standing followed by sitting which was 3%. Meanwhile, there was none sleeping position that was spotted during the observation period in which it suggested that the ostriches only tend to sleep during night time. This is due to the ostriches are diurnal animals where they are most active early and late in the day (Davies, 2003), but may be active on moonlit nights. This is supported by Degen et al., (1989) and Deeming (1998) that reported most ostriches sit during sunset and were inactive all night. According to period of time, the resting behaviours were highest in the morning followed by evening and afternoon. The difference in resting behavioural patterns between different observational periods was significantly difference (χ2=80.23, df=2, p 0.05). 14

Locomotion behaviour can be divided into three types namely walking, running and trembling. Walking (92%) was the most frequent locomotion behaviour observed followed by running (2%) and trembling (0%). It was supported by Berendsen (1995) which postulated that the ostriches at the farm in Germany spent most their time walking rather than running. According to Mushi et al., (2008), this behaviour was gender-dependent with males spending more time in walking probably because they watching around predators while the females brood over the eggs sitting on the nests. Meanwhile, trembling was a sign of stress like during being cornered or transported, but none of this behaviour was observed. Overall, this locomotion behaviour was highest in the afternoon followed by morning and evening. It was supported by Mushi et al., (2008) which stated that in general, all the age groups of ostriches spent about 60% of the afternoon either standing or walking. The difference in locomotion behavioural patterns between different observational periods was not significantly difference (χ2=4.32, df=2, p 0.05). Ingestive behaviour encompasses all eating and drinking behaviours. In this study, ingestive behaviour was divided into three namely tolerance, drinking and pecking. Based on the observations, pecking (69%) was the most frequent ingestive behaviour observed followed by tolerance (24%) and drinking (7%). Pecking activity was the most observed in the morning due to scanty food supply. Consequently they frequently walked around to forage for a food such as an insect within the enclosure. This was supported by Mushi et al., (2008) which stated that, since no supplementation was done in the game reserve, the ostrich had to make do with the available forage and insects which were abundant in the reserve. Meanwhile, tolerance was high during afternoon and evening periods because it coincided with the time when the food was distributed. It also supported by Sambraus (1994) and Deeming (1998), where they found that the higher food consumption was during the food was supplied. Overall, the difference in ingestive behavioural patterns between different observational periods was significantly difference (χ2=6.62, df=2, p 0.05). According to Mushi et al., (2008), thermoregulations normally accomplished by flapping of wings, open-mouth breathes or yawning and were shown by ostriches in the afternoon as a means of dissipating heat from the body. From the observations, thermoregulation behaviour was found to be the highest in the afternoon followed by evening whereas none of thermoregulation behaviours were observed in the morning. This was due to the high temperature in the afternoon which required the ostriches to dissipate more heat during this period. Similar results were found by Muhsi et al., (2008). The difference in thermoregulation behavioural patterns between different observational periods was significantly difference (χ2=178.76, df=2, p 0.05). Comfort behaviours include sand bathing; care of plumage and other parts of body. During sand bathing, the ostrich would lie down in a dry sandy place. It would then push the neck into the sand using snake-like motions and shovel sand over the body (Muhsi et al., 2008). However, this behaviour has not been observed during the research period. This was due to the ostriches enclosure has no sandy areas. In order to accustom the ostriches comfort behaviour, it would therefore be recommended to set up sandy areas within the enclosure for sand bathing activity to prevent stress among farmed ostriches. For ratites like the ostriches, the body care behaviour comprise of dust bathing and plumage care (Kreibich and Sommer, 1995). The ostriches are slightly different from the other species of birds where they do not have uropygial gland or formally known as the preen gland or the oil gland. The functional of the gland is to clean the feathers naturally. Based on the observations, grooming activity by ostriches were to get rid of foreign objects embedded at their feathers like leaves and dirt. This activity was frequently done in the afternoon followed by evening and morning. This may be because of dry conditions in the afternoon due to high temperature that had been a contributory factor to the highest frequency during this period. The difference in comfort behavioural patterns between different observational periods was significantly difference (χ2=17.1, df=2, p 0.05). The aggression behaviour observed was the threat behaviour. The threat was the behaviour when the ostrich stand tall with tail erect, hissing, wings 15

slightly open and feathers puffed up, especially around the neck. Based on the observations, aggression behaviour was demonstrated when the ostriches faced the disturbance like met the strangers and also when the keeper came to fed them. This was supported by Muhsi et al., (2008) which stated that, aggressive behaviour was demonstrated between hens and cocks and also towards human intruders. This behaviour was exhibited by the male ostriches only. The aggression behaviour was prevalent in the morning compared to both afternoon and evening. The difference in aggression behavioural patterns between different observational periods was not significantly different (χ2=0.57, df=2, p 0.05). 4. Conclusion As the conclusion, behaviours of captive ostriches showed high difference between the different observational periods. Only some behaviour that showed the difference between male and female like the aggression behaviour that only showed by the male. Overall, standing recorded the highest occurrence for the resting behaviour, walking was the highest occurrence for the locomotion behaviour, and pecking showed the highest frequency for the ingestive behaviour. Lastly, it showed that the ostriches reared in captivity displayed similar behavioural patterns to those remaining in the wild, although some captive circumstances impose minor differences. Recommendations The ostrich lack of uropygial gland, where it was believed to help maintain the integrity of the feather structure and the flexibility of feathers that keeps feather barbules from breaking. Thus, in order to care for their feather, normally they would prefer for sand-bathing activity. However, during the carried out research, the ground were covered with the grass, where only small portion of sand, pebble and gravel could be found. This condition had limited the ostriches to do the sand-bathing activity and affected their behaviour. Thus, the management should add more sandy area in each pen, so that it will be available for them to do this activity. Second, the provided shelter in each pen was not adequate for a pair of ostriches. It nearly only could accommodate their drink and food where during the rainy days, they were all wet and shivering. The ostrich were originated from Africa, where they much adapted with the warm condition. Thus, during rainy days, they should have the shelter that could accommodate them for their health. Lastly, based on the interviewed, the unsuitable type of land in the paddock had restricted the growth of plants. Thus, the management should further enhance the surrounding of enclosure on par to the wild. According to Kock, (1996a; 1996b) the supply of a more natural environment appears to ease the stress seen in ostrich chicks bred in captive condition. Acknowledgements We would like to thank the Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Bachok Campus, Kelantan for the permission granted to conduct this study. References Alden, P.C., R.D. Estes, D. Schlitter and B. McBride. (1996). African Birds, In: Collins Guide to African Wild Harper Collins publishers, London, pp: 638-63. Berendsen, K.D. (1995). Behaviour. In: Kreibich, A., Sommer, M. (Eds.), Ostrich Farm Management. Landwirtschafts Verlag, Münster-Hiltrup, pp. 25-32. Brown, L.H., Urban, E.K. and Newman, K. (1982). The Birds of Africa. London, Academic Press. Davies, S.J.J.F. (2003). "Birds I Tinamous and Ratites to Hoatzins". In Hutchins, Michael.Grzimek's Animal Life Encyclopedia 8 (2 ed.). Farmington Hills, MI: Gale Group.pp. 99 101. ISBN 0-7876-5784-0. Deeming, D.C. (1998), Time activity budgets of adult ostriches (Struthio camelus) in a farming environment during the winter, Applied Animal Behavioural Science, Vol 59, pp. 363-371. Degen, A.A., Kam, M. and Rosenstrauch, A. (1989). Time-activity budget of ostriches (Struthio camelus) offered concentrate feed and maintained in outdoor pens. Applied Animal Behavioural Science, 22: 347-358. Estep, D.Q., Fisher, R.B. and Gore, W.T. (1978). Effects of enclosure size and complexity on the activity and sleep of the ester chipmunk Tamias striatus. Behavioral Biology, 23(2): 249-253. Fisher, R.B., Meunier, G.F., O Donoghue, P.J., Rhodes, D.L. and Schafenaker, A.M. (1980). Effects of enclosure size on activity and sleep of a hystricomorph rodent (Octodon degus). Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 16(4): 273-275. Human Ageing Genomic Resources (2006). AnAge Database. Human Ageing Genomic Resources http://genomics.senescence.info/ [Access date:04/10/2014]. 16

Hallam, M.G. (1992). The TOPAZ: Introduction to Practical Ostrich farming. Superior print and Packaging, Harare, Zimbabwe, pp: 2. Hogan, E.S., Houpt, K.A. and Sweeney, K. (1988). The effect of enclosure size in social interaction and daily activity patterns of captive Asiatic wild horse (Equus przewalskii). Applied Animal Behaviour, 21(1-2): 147-168. Innis, G.S., Balph, M.N. and Balph, D.S. (1985). On spatial requirements of captive social animals. Animal Behaviour, 33(2): 680-682. Jensvold, M.A., Sanz, C.M. and Fouts, D.H. (2001). Effect of enclosure size and complexity on the behaviours of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 4(1): 53-69. Kock, J.A. (1996a). Natural rearing of chickens up to three months of age. In: Guidelines for successful ostrich farming. Oudtshoorn: Little Karoo Agricultural Development Centre, p. 24-27. Kock, J.A. (1996b). Artificial rearing of chickens. In: Deeming, D.C. (Ed.) Guidelines for successful ostrich farming. Oudtshoorn: Little Karoo Agricultural Development Centre, p. 18-23. Kreibich, A. and Sommer, M. (1995). Ostrich Farm Management. Landwirtschaftsverlag GmbH, Munster-Hiltrup, Germany. Mushi, E.Z., Binta, M.G. and Lumba N.J. (2008). Behaviour of Wild Ostrich (Struthio camelus) at Mokolodi Nature Reserve, Gaborone, Botswana. Research Journal of Poultry Sciences, 2(1): 1-4. Newberry, R.C., Keeling, L.J., Estevez, I. and Bilčík, B. (2007). Behaviour when young as a predictor of severe feather pecking in adult laying hens: the redirected foraging hypothesis revisited. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, v. 107, n.3-4, p. 262-274. Sambraus, H.H. (1994), The circadian rhythm in the behaviour of ostriches (Struthio camelus) kept in pens. Berliner und Münchener-Tierärztliche. Wochenschrift, 107: 339-341. Schaller, N.U., D Août, K., Villa, R., Herkner, B. and Aerts, P. (2011) Toe function and dynamic pressure distribution in ostrich locomotion. The Journal of Experimental Biology 214: 1123 1130. Williams, J.B., Siegfreid, W.R., Milton, S.J., Adams, N.J., Dean, W.R.J., Duplessis, M.A., Jackson, S. and Nagy, K.A. (1993). Field metabolism, water requirements, foraging behavior, and diet of wild ostriches in the Namib Desert. Ecology, 74: 390-404. 17