By ROGER J. SIGLIN and MILTON W. WELLER

Similar documents
Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

RECOGNITION OF NEST, EGGS, NEST SITE, AND YOUNG IN FEMALE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

(199) THE HATCHING AND FLEDGING OF SOME COOT

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

( 162 ) SOME BREEDING-HABITS OF THE LAPWING.

Animal Biodiversity. Teacher Resources - High School (Cycle 1) Biology Redpath Museum

In the summers of 1977 and 1978, at Gibraltar Point, Lincolnshire, I

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

PREDATION ON RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD EGGS AND NESTLINGS

BREEDING ROBINS AND NEST PREDATORS: EFFECT OF PREDATOR TYPE AND DEFENSE STRATEGY ON INITIAL VOCALIZATION PATTERNS

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

( 142 ) NOTES ON THE GREAT NORTHERN DIVER.

MITOCW MIT9_20F13_lec17.mp3

THE CONDOR OBSERVATIONS ON BREEDING BEHAVIOR IN TRICOLORED RED-WINGS. By DAVID LACK and JOHN T. EMLEN, JR.

(135) OBSERVATIONS IN A ROOKERY DURING THE INCUBATION PERIOD C. M. OGILVIE.

By Hans Frey ¹ ² & Alex Llopis ²

Piping Plover. Below: Note the color of the sand and the plover s back.

King penguin brooding and defending a sub-antarctic skua chick

EIDER JOURNEY It s Summer Time for Eiders On the Breeding Ground

NOTES ON THE SPRING TERRITORY OF THE BLACKBIRD

Minnesota Bird Coloring Book

NEST SITE SELECTION AND PRODUCTIVITY OF GREAT HORNED. Thirty-three active nests (16 artificial, 17 natural) of the Great Horned Owl (Bubo

BREEDING OF AECHMOPHORUS GREBES AT CLEAR LAKE, LAKE COUNTY, CALIFORNIA, DURING JUNE 2015

Hawks Order Falconiformes

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Chapman (1999) provides a comprehensive account.

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

ACTIVITY #2: TURTLE IDENTIFICATION

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

THE CONDOR. AN EXPERIMENTAL STUDY OF TERRITORIAL BEHAVIOR IN BREEDING RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS. By ROBERT W. NERO and JOHN T. EMLEN, JR.

FOOD HABITS OF NESTING COOPER S HAWKS AND GOSHAWKS IN NEW YORK AND PENNSYLVANIA

48 RAPTOR RESEARCH Vol. 15 No. 2. top. < 0.Sm > side HABITUATION TO HUMAN DISTURBANCE IN NESTING ACCIPITERS

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

NEST-DEFENSE BEHAVIOR IN THE RED-WINGED

Antipredation role of clumped nesting by marsh-nesting red-winged blackbirds

OBSERVATIONS OF HAWAIIAN

Chloroceryle americana (Green Kingfisher)

(144) REACTIONS OF SOME PASSERINE BIRDS TO A STUFFED CUCKOO. II. A DETAILED STUDY OF THE WILLOW-WARBLER.

Yellow-throated and Solitary Vireos in Ontario: 4. Egg Laying, Incubation and Cowbird Parasitism

(340) PHOTOGRAPHIC STUDIES OF SOME LESS FAMILIAR BIRDS. LIX. NIGHT HERON.

Animals and plants are adapted to the conditions of the habitats in which they live.

Swans & Geese. Order Anseriformes Family Anserinae

Predator Control. Jennifer L. Rhodes University of Maryland Extension Queen Anne s County

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

Management of Wildlife. By Ulrica Williams and Jade Catterson 5 th May 2009

AGGRESSIVE DISPLAY OF THE CORN-CRAKE.

The Year of the Wasp

Bird-X Goose Chase / Bird Shield Testing Information For Use On: 1. Apples 2. Cherries 3. Grapes 4. Blueberries 5. Corn 6. Sunflowers 7.

Behavioral Defenses Against Brood Parasitism in the American Robin (Turdus migratorius)

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

We are adult American. Field Marks. We are the smallest falcons in North America. Like other falcons, we have long, pointed wings,

Removal of Alaskan Bald Eagles for Translocation to Other States Michael J. Jacobson U.S Fish and Wildlife Service, Juneau, AK

Select Mammals of Loudoun County

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

PEREGRINE FALCON HABITAT MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES ONTARIO MINISTRY OF NATURAL RESOURCES

TEACHER GUIDE: Letter 1: Western Pond Turtle

Protecting Workers in Bear Country

Jan. 28: If you want the local wildlife

Bald Eagles in the Yukon. Wildlife in our backyard

The Development of Behavior

OBSERVATIONS OF PEMBROKE PINES BALD EAGLE NEST - FWC ID# BO-002

NESTING POPULATIONS OF RED-TAILED HAWKS AND HORNED OWLS IN CENTRAL NEW YORK STATE1 BY DONALD C. HAGAR, JR.

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1996) provides a comprehensive account.

What is the date at which most chicks would have been expected to fledge?

Anhinga anhinga (Anhinga or Snake-bird)

Ames, IA Ames, IA (515)

CALLS OF THE WILD Secrets of Animal Speak

Raptor Ecology in the Thunder Basin of Northeast Wyoming

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL

Growth and Development. Embryonic development 2/22/2018. Timing of hatching. Hatching. Young birds and their parents

F RIEDMANN (1963) considers the Lark Sparrow (Chondestes grammacus)

Desensitization and Counter Conditioning

By: Dr. Antje Mewes, Andreas Frei, Jan Dams, Gerardus Scheres. Weltvogelpark Walsrode, Germany.

Species must be adapted to their habitat.

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

Did you know that Snowy Plovers (Charadrius alexandrines char-ad-ree-us alex-an-dreen-us):

EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name

Dog Name Goldie #47 1, 5

Western Painted Turtle Monitoring and Habitat Restoration at Buttertubs Marsh, Nanaimo, BC

Behaviour of cats and dogs

Comparing Adaptations of Birds

Under One Roof. Beehive Management During the Swarming Season in a single hive. By: - Nick Withers

Draw a line from the names below to the animals they match. Red Fox. Wild Turkey. Wood Duck. White-tailed Deer. Black Bear

March to mid May: Mid May to late June:

Turtle Research, Education, and Conservation Program

He was a year older than her and experienced in how to bring up a brood and survive.

FOOTEDNESS IN DOMESTIC PIGEONS

T HE recent and interesting paper by Alexander F. Skutch (1962) stimulated

WHOO S WHOO? The Great Horned Owl as a Terrestrial Indicator Species in the Ecological Risk Assessment of the Tittabawassee River and Floodplain.

BEHAVIORAL DEFENSES TO BROOD PARASITISM BY POTENTIAL HOSTS OF THE BROWN-HEADED COWBIRD

Aggression Social Aggression to Unfamiliar Dogs

REPORT OF ACTIVITIES TURTLE ECOLOGY RESEARCH REPORT Crescent Lake National Wildlife Refuge 31 May to 4 July 2017

Delaware Valley Golden Retriever Rescue 60 Vera Cruz Rd., Reinholds, PA (717) Behavioral Assessment: ID NO:

NQF Level: 1 US No:

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

Breeding behavior of the boreal toad, Bufo boreas boreas (Baird and Girard), in western Montana

AGGRESSION (CATS) DIAGNOSING AND TREATING

Transcription:

432 Vol.65 COMPARATIVE NEST DEFENSE BEHAVIOR OF FOUR SPECIES OF MARSH BIRDS By ROGER J. SIGLIN and MILTON W. WELLER Experimental studies of responses of prey species to potential predators have centered mainly on methods of predator recognition and the roles of instinct and learning in such responses. Experimental work such as that of Nice and Ter Pelkwyk (1941) emphasized the study of hand-reared birds and their innate and conditioned responses to both live and model animals. Considerable work also has been directed to the elements of models which serve as releasers (Tinbergen, 1948). Simmons (1952) has discussed possible motivation and the types of displays found in breeding birds. In general, the intensity of predator reactions is greatest in nest defense and, thus, nest sites are a convenient place to study such behavior. However, some workers have avoided nest sites because of the hypersensitivity of birds there (Altmann, 1956). The study here reported was an attempt to compare nest defense behavior of four species of marsh birds which nested in slightly different habitats and to evaluate the relationship between habitat and predator reactions. Data were obtained on two blackbirds and two terns: Redwinged Blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus) which nested in shoreline emergent and terrestrial plants; Yellow-headed Blackbird (Xanthocephalzu xanthocephalus) which used emergents near open water; Black Tern (ChZidonius niger) which nested on low, unused muskrat houses or built low nests of floating debris at or near the water level; and the Forster Tern (Sterna forsteri) which nested on higher muskrat houses. These species also showed slightly different degrees of colonial nesting, with Yellow-headed Blackbirds and Forster Terns normally being more gregarious than their associates. Experiments were conducted during the summer of 1961 in Iowa at Goose and Little Wall lakes near Jewel1 in Hamilton County and at Dan Green Slough, Barringer Slough, and Rush Lake in Clay and Palo Alto counties near Ruthven. The project was financed by the National Science Foundation. Cecil E. Spatcher of the National Science Foundation High School Teachers Research Participation Program assisted in the field work. METHODS Nest defense behavior was induced by the two-dimensional models illustrated in figure 1 and the mounted specimens listed in table 1. The square and rounded wooden forms and the wooden painted (Chrysemys pi&a) and snapping (Ckelydra serpentina) turtle models were rarely used because many individuals failed to respond to them. The Barred Owl (St& varia) was omitted from later tests because responses to it did not differ significantly from those to the Great Horned Owl (Bubo virginianus). The normal testing procedure was to place models as close to the nest as possible and in a natural position for an approaching predator. In the case of tern nests on muskrat houses, models were placed on the houses and within six inches of the nest. Near elevated blackbird nests or floating tern nests, models were placed on mats of vegetation or on stakes within six inches of the base of the nest. The responses of nesting birds then were observed from a distance to minimize the effect of the observers presence. The sequence in which these models were displayed was constant except in the case of the Yellow-headed Blackbird for which the sequence was varied because birds often were not attracted to the nest site unless a conspicuous model was displayed first. An attempt was made to standardize the length of time each model was dispiayed at the nest. The normal procedure was to record the reactions of the birds for two min-

Sept., 1963 NEST DEFENSE IN MARSH BIRDS 433 Fig. 1. Two-dimensional wooden models used to test responses to foreign objects and snake and turtle-like forms at the nest. utes following the first indication that they noticed the model. If the birds did not react within two to five minutes, the model or mount was replaced by another. TABLE 1 LIST OF MOUNTED ANIMALS USED IN TESTS Fox snake (Elaphe vulpina) Snapping turtle (Chelydra serpentina) Muskrat (Ondatra zibethicus) Mink (Mustela vison) Crow (Corvus brachyrhynchos) Red-tailed Hawk (Buteo jamaicenti) Barred Owl (Strix varia) Great-homed Owl (Bvbo virginianus) RESULTS Figure 2 summarizes the results of the major tests. These data are separated according to intensity of response. A positive response indicates a clear-cut interest in the model or animal as shown by the bird s ( 1) flying to or over the nest and often calling, or (2) diving at or attacking the model. The term attack, when used in reference to redwings and yellowheads, implies striking or pecking the model; in terns it suggests the rapidly repeated diving and calling as well as direct striking of the model. Discussions of species will follow a similar pattern and an attempt is made to divide behavior into three or more levels of intensity. Obviously, these are arbitrary boundaries, and we have no data to demonstrate that these represent levels of motivation. REDWINGED BLACKBIRLJ In a low intensity reaction the male hovered over the nest for a few seconds and then departed. The female often returned to the nest after glancing at the model placed

434 THE CONDOR Vol. 65 YELLOWHEAD REDWING 6oloo 10 60 60 $0 60 60 TO 60 66 100 PER CENT FORSTER S TERN BLACK TERN PER CENT Fig. 2. Total responses (white bar) and proportion of total responses which were considered actual attacks (black bar) for some models or mounts placed at nests of four species of marsh birds. Number of tests indicated to left of each bar. nearby or delayed returning to the nest until the model was removed even though she appeared unconcerned over its presence. A greater intensity of response was indicated when both male and female hovered over the nest or perched nearby while one or both gave a quiet chenr or check call (see Allen, 1914). Other redwings often were attracted but quickly lost interest. The nesting pair eventually became less concerned, and the female sometimes returned to the nest. In a high intensity situation, the male excitedly called chenk or tree, and the female often emitted a high pitched thee. One or both members of the pair attacked the model with intense and repeated pecking. Other birds were attracted by the calls and hovered or perched nearby calling excitedly, but they rarely attacked the model. These visitors gradually departed, but the nesting pair continued to attack the model as long as it was present. Occasionally a male attacked his mate during the peak of the battle; this behavior may have represented redirected aggression. As shown in figure 2, the majority of high intensity reactions were directed toward the mounted snake, crow, owl, and- hawk, but there was considerable individual variation in intensity of response to the different models. YELLOW-HEADED BLACKBIRD The most simple and characteristic response was that the male flew over the nest once or twice and departed. Occasionally the female performed in a similar manner. The female usually would not return to the nest, but this may have been due to the observers presence nearby. In some cases, Black Terns would hover over the yellowhead nest without eliciting further response from the owners. When more intensely interested, both male and female hovered over the nest or perched nearby while the male

Sept., 1963 NEST DEFENSE IN MARSH BIRDS 43.5 gave a c&k call intermittently. They occasionally attacked the mounts while other yellowheads only inspected them. The male and female sometimes remained near the nest but often lost interest rather quickly. Neither the male nor female called as often as did redwings. The least common and most intense situation was shown when the female gave a piercing thee while hovering over the potential predator. The male, also hovering, gave a trill-like alarm call, not unlike the call of the nestling, which attracted other males from adjacent territories. This resulted in a mobbing by the pair and by other birds in the area, with considerable calling and occabional attacks. Yellowheads responded positively to all models except the turtles, but the only models attacked were the straight wooden snake and the mounted snake and crow. The responses of the yellowheads to the different models varied much more in intensity than did the responses of the redwings. In many cases, the stuffed snake, crow, owl, and hawk elicited little response. Some difference in response of yellowheads was attributed to the sequence in which the models were displayed. When the owl or hawk was used first, the yellowheads then reacted more clearly to the other models.; General observations showed that the response to live hawks in flight was intense. Males hovered over the nesting areas while giving the loud trill. This aroused all nesting birds. Simultaneously, redwings gave a high-pitched tee (usually while perched) so that no hawk passed over the marsh without a general alarm. BLACK TERN In a low intensity response, one to several terns would hover over the nest with very little or no calling. They would usually disperse within a few seconds, and in a few cases the female returned to the nest with the model present. When more aroused, one to a dozen or more terns hovered over the model, all of them calling excitedly. A few dived low over the model. Presumably because there was no movement of the model, the birds gradually left except for one or two, probably the nest owners, that continued to hover over the nest and dive at the model. In higher intensity, the birds hovered and called more rapidly and with a louder pitch. Many of the birds dived low over the model, and one or two birds, presumably the owners of the nest, pecked the model each time they dived. The intensity of the response often remained high throughout the duration of the test and few birds departed. FORSTER TERN The reactions of the Forster Terns were similar to those of the Black Terns in all major respects except that the proportion of positive responses which were termed attacks was higher. Nevertheless, Forster Terns rarely dived closer to the models than six inches. This is in contrast to the Black Terns which struck some models as they occasionally strike humans. However, the models attracted larger numbers of Forster Terns than Black Terns, and their loud calls and impressive dives seemed an effective deterrent to any predator. DISCUSSION These tests did not contribute to an understanding of the roles of instinct and learning in predator identification because the work was done only with experienced adults which, presumably, had seen many live predators. A higher response to mounted animals than to two-dimensional models was noted. However, it was apparent in highly aggressive individuals that there was an innate aggressiveness to most foreign objects near the nest. Birds often responded to mounts of usually harmless associates such as musk-

436 THE CONDOR Vol. 65 rats and turtles. Nesting birds showed little interest in live muskrats or turtles on muskrat houses away from the nest but their reactions to models were not tested. One accident produced an interesting result concerning the redwing s ability to identify owls. During an experiment at a redwing nest, the mounted owl was knocked over. The intense hovering and attacking ceased almost immediately. As soon as the owl mount again was erected, the vicious attack resumed. However, no difference in response was noted in a similar experiment with the highly aggressive Forster Terns. The most significant results of the tests were the measurements of quantitative differences in nest defense behavior. These were most clear-cut between the blackbirds: Responses of Yellow-headed Blackbirds to models were considerably more variable and of lower intensity than those of Redwinged Blackbirds, and some curiosity, rather than aggressiveness, may be registered in the records for the yellowhead. Attacks were a common response of redwings but were rare among yellowheads. General observations indicate that attacks on humans also are much less common in Yellow-headed than in Redwinged blackbirds. However, hawks which flew over the marsh always caused a much more intense alarm and flight call in yellowheads than in redwings. Thus, reactions of yellowheads to potential predators can be clear-cut but such aggressive responses seem restricted to aerial predators or, perhaps, to an object actually in the nest. The different reactions in these two species seem to accord with their nest sites. Redwings, which nest near or over land, are in closer contact with land predators (snakes, weasels, mink, raccoon, foxes) than are yellowheads. An intensive pecking and mobbing attack probably is necessary to deter such predators. Yellowheads nest over water where fewer terrestrial forms occur, and a noticeably lower response was recorded to most immobile predators. Both species are subject to predation by hawks and crows and respond to aerial predators, but these could not be tested in these experiments. However, general observations indicate that yellowheads have an elaborate distracting display when an aerial predator appears. In the highly social yellowhead, the mass distraction caused by the hovering and trill call may be quite effective for the whole colony, whereas redwings, which often nest singly or in small groups, might be conspicuous targets if they displayed alone or in small numbers. Terns are well known for their highly social warning and mobbing behavior and are even more respected by predators than are redwings because of the velocity of their dives which are accompanied by piercing cries. Tests with models and mounts demonstrated an intense social type of aggressive behavior at the nest site which involved most of the terns nesting in the locale. There was only a slight difference in nest site selection between the terns that were studied, and a lesser difference in defense behavior is to be expected than in the blackbirds. Both species of terns nested over water some distance from shore in clearings. Although a lesser response to mammals and terrestrial predators might be expected than was found, the shallow nest could easily be destroyed by a muskrat, mink or turtle which climbed on it, and intense diving and pecking might deter the animal. Moreover, the evolution of such defensiveness may be linked with the more common land-nesting terns, as studied by Cullen (1960). It is of interest to compare the intense predator reactions of Black and Forster terns with the less vigorous responses of the Yellow-headed Blackbird since all nest over water. The major habitat difference is that blackbird nests are 8 to 30 inches above the water on emergent vegetation while those of terns are nearer the water and more subject to disturbance by semi-aquatic forms such as turtles, snakes, and muskrats. In general, it appears that predator responses in these species have evolved in conjunction with nest sites and with the species which might be predatory or cause acci-

Sept., 1963 NEST DEFENSE IN MARSH BIRDS 437 dental nest loss. Social behavior and colony-nesting probably also play a role in the nature of predator responses. SUMMARY The reactions of four species of marsh birds to models and mounts of potential predators at the nest suggest that behavior is related to both nest site and social habits. Redwinged Blackbirds, which nest close to shore or on shore, used pecking and mobbing to deter terrestrial predators. Yellow-headed Blackbirds, which nest over water, were much less disturbed by terrestrial forms but reacted conspicuously to aerial predators. Less difference in nest site and behavior occurs between Black and Forster terns, which both nest over water. Both showed highly social behavior and responded aggressively to any mounted animals near the nest. LITERATURE CITED Alien, A. A. 1914. The red-winged blackbird: a study in the ecology of a cat-tail marsh. Proc. Linnaean Sot. N. Y. Nos. 24-25 ; 43-128. Altmann, S. A. 1956. Avian mobbing behavior and predator recognition. Condor, 58:241-253. Cullen, J. M. 1960. Some adaptations in the nesting behaviour of terns. Proc. XII Congr. Internat. Ornith. (Helsinki), 153-157. Nice, M. M., and Ter Pelkwyk, J. 1941. Enemy recognition by the song sparrow. Auk, 58:155-214. Simmons, K. E. L. 1952. The nature of the predator-reactions of breeding birds. Behaviour, 4:161-171 Tinbergen, N. 1948. Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study. Wilson Bull., 60:6-51. Department of Zoology and Entomology, Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa, January 14, 1963. (This is Journal Paper No. J-4.507 of the Iowa Agricultural and Home Economics Experiment Station, Ames, Iowa. Project No. 1504.)