Use of an Infrared Monitor to Record the Frequency and Timing of Parental Nest Visitation by the Blue Rock Thrush Monticola solitarius

Similar documents
Ernst Rupp and Esteban Garrido Grupo Jaragua El Vergel #33, Santo Domingo Dominican Republic

Multiple broods from a hole in the wall: breeding Red-and-yellow Barbets Trachyphonus erythrocephalus in southeast Sudan

短報 (Note) Stump nests of the Ural owl Strix uralensis in Hokkaido. Shigeru MATSUOKA 1)* and Kanomi SHIINA 2)

Breeding Activity Peak Period Range Duration (days) Laying May May 2 to 26. Incubation Early May to mid June Early May to mid June 30 to 34

Seven Nests of Rufescent Tiger-Heron (Tigrisoma lineatum)

Internship Report: Raptor Conservation in Bulgaria

Key concepts of Article 7(4): Version 2008

BLACK OYSTERCATCHER NEST MONITORING PROTOCOL

Did you know that Snowy Plovers (Charadrius alexandrines char-ad-ree-us alex-an-dreen-us):

Ciccaba virgata (Mottled Owl)

Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus) research & monitoring Breeding Season Report- Beypazarı, Turkey

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

Key concepts of Article 7(4): Version 2008

National Geographic. Young Explorer. September issue 2014

OBSERVATIONS ON SWALLOWS AND HOUSE- MARTINS AT THE NEST. BY

Bald Eagles in the Yukon. Wildlife in our backyard

Rock Wren Nesting in an Artificial Rock Wall in Folsom, Sacramento County, California

Florida Field Naturalist

Record of Predation by Sugar Glider on Breeding Eastern Rosellas 33Km NE of Melbourne in November 2016

Amrun Project Feral Animal Monitoring Annual Report August 2017

Hooded Plover Environmental Protection and Biodiversity Conservation Act Nomination

BROOD REDUCTION IN THE CURVE-BILLED THRASHER By ROBERTE.RICKLEFS

Texas Quail Index. Result Demonstration Report 2016

The Long-term Effect of Precipitation on the Breeding Success of Golden Eagles Aquila chrysaetos homeyeri in the Judean and Negev Deserts, Israel

SEASONAL PATTERNS OF NESTING IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD MORTALITY

Hole-nesting birds. In natural conditions great and blue tits breed in holes that are made by e.g. woodpeckers

Intraspecific relationships extra questions and answers (Extension material for Level 3 Biology Study Guide, ISBN , page 153)

SIGNAL WORDS CAUSE/EFFECT COMPARE/CONTRAST DESCRIPTION

Bluebirds & Des Moines City Parks

DO BROWN-HEADED COWBIRDS LAY THEIR EGGS AT RANDOM IN THE NESTS OF RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS?

Supplementary Fig. 1: Comparison of chase parameters for focal pack (a-f, n=1119) and for 4 dogs from 3 other packs (g-m, n=107).

The behaviour of a pair of House Sparrows while rearing young

Keywords: Sea turtle, loggerhead turtle, coastal process, shoreline change, shore protection, beach nourishment, environment, Japan INTRODUCTION

AVIAN HAVEN Wild Bird Rehabilitation Center

Tropical Screech Owl - Megascops choliba

ACTIVITY #6: TODAY S PICNIC SPECIALS ARE

Purple Martin. Adult male Purple Martin

Alligators. very long tail, and a head with very powerful jaws.

Sea Turtle Protection by Means of Coastal Engineering: Field Study on Sea turtle Behavior, Coastal Processes of a Nesting Beach

from an experimental bag net SHIODE, DAISUKE; TAKAHASHI, MUTSUKI Proceedings of the 6th Internationa SEASTAR2000 workshop) (2011): 31-34


Dive-depth distribution of. coriacea), loggerhead (Carretta carretta), olive ridley (Lepidochelys olivacea), and

Great Blue Heron Chick Development. Through the Stages

EIDER JOURNEY It s Summer Time for Eiders On the Breeding Ground

SEALANT, WATERPROOFING & RESTORATION INSTITUTE SPRING PEREGRINE FALCONS: DIS RAPTORS OF WORK AT HEIGHT

Texas Quail Index. Result Demonstration Report 2016

Barn Swallow Nest Monitoring Methods

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema

Procnias averano (Bearded Bellbird)

Fact Sheet: African Penguin Spheniscus demersus

In collaboration with the NJ Division of Fish & Wildlife s Endangered and Nongame Species Program

EXERCISE 14 Marine Birds at Sea World Name

Entertainment Robot aibo Announced

BLUEBIRD NEST BOX REPORT

Crotophaga major (Greater Ani)

Pet-Temp PT-300 Ear Thermometer Frequently Asked Questions

Breeding White Storks( Ciconia ciconia at Chessington World of Adventures Paul Wexler

Harry s Science Investigation 2014

Wilson Bull., 103(4), 199 1, pp

Applied Information and Communication Technology. Unit 3: The Knowledge Worker January 2010 Time: 2 hours 30 minutes

Key concepts of Article 7(4): Version 2008

The Hatching, Growth, and Fledging of Nestling Purple Martins

BREEDING ECOLOGY OF THE LITTLE TERN, STERNA ALBIFRONS PALLAS, 1764 IN SINGAPORE

Sparrowhawks & Goshawks and the Gymnogene

DO DIFFERENT CLUTCH SIZES OF THE TREE SWALLOW (Tachycineta bicolor)

Snowy Plover Management Plan Updated 2015

Coyote (Canis latrans)

For further information on the biology and ecology of this species, Clarke (1995) provides a comprehensive account.

Litter marks indicating infiltration area of stemflow-induced water

THE CHILDREN S ZOO. Scavenger Hunt GRADES K-3

The Essex County Field Naturalists' Club's BLUEBIRD COMMITTEE REPORT FOR 2017

Great Horned Owl (Bubo virginianus) Productivity and Home Range Characteristics in a Shortgrass Prairie. Rosemary A. Frank and R.

Breeding the Common Golden-Backed Woodpecker in Captivity Dinopium javanense

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Caprimulgiformes Family: Podargidae Scientific Name: Podargus strigoides Common Name: Tawny frogmouth

Contrasting Response to Predator and Brood Parasite Signals in the Song Sparrow (melospiza melodia)

A brief report on the 2016/17 monitoring of marine turtles on the São Sebastião peninsula, Mozambique

Dry season survival of Aedes aegypti eggs in various breeding sites

Piping Plover. Below: Note the color of the sand and the plover s back.

Thefirst attempt at Brood Manipulation of the Golden Eagle Aquila chrysaetos in Japan

Ames, IA Ames, IA (515)

2009 Eagle Nest News from Duke Farms eagle nest Written by Larissa Smith, Assistant Biologist

RE-INTRODUCTION OF THE ORIENTAL PIED HORNBILL IN SINGAPORE, WITH EMPHASIS ON ARTIFICIAL NESTS

Successful Use of Alarm/Alert Call Playback to End Canada Goose Problems Dr. Philip C. Whitford, Biology Department, Capital University, Columbus, OH.

[Source: D W Sims and V A Quayla (1998) Nature 393, pages ] (2)

Congratulations on the completion of your project that was supported by The Rufford Small Grants Foundation.

Where Animals and Plants Are Found

Writing: Lesson 31. Today the students will be learning how to write more advanced middle paragraphs using a variety of elaborative techniques.

Evaluation of large-scale baiting programs more surprises from Central West Queensland

Teacher Workbooks. Language Arts Series Internet Reading Comprehension Oceans Theme, Vol. 1

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

A Comparison of morphological differences between Gymnophthalmus spp. in Dominica, West Indies

BBRG-5. SCTB15 Working Paper. Jeffrey J. Polovina 1, Evan Howell 2, Denise M. Parker 2, and George H. Balazs 2

By: Dr. Antje Mewes, Andreas Frei, Jan Dams, Gerardus Scheres. Weltvogelpark Walsrode, Germany.

Bald Head Island Conservancy 2018 Sea Turtle Report Emily Goetz, Coastal Scientist

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Iguana Technical Assistance Workshop. Presented by: Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission

Removal of Alaskan Bald Eagles for Translocation to Other States Michael J. Jacobson U.S Fish and Wildlife Service, Juneau, AK

Full house. the Burrowing Parrots of Patagonia. By JUAN F. MASELLO, CHRISTINA SOMMER and PETRA QUILLFELDT

Best Practice on the Farm

Activity 7 Swallow Census

Transcription:

Zoological Studies 5(1): 16-23 (211) Use of an Infrared Monitor to Record the Frequency and Timing of Parental Nest Visitation by the Blue Rock Thrush Monticola solitarius Toshimitsu Nuka 1,2, *, Yoshitaka Morikawa 3, and Christopher P. Norman 1,4 1 Marine Biosystems Research Center, Chiba University, 1 Uchiura, Kamogawa, Chiba 299-552, Japan 2 Chiba City Wild Bird Society, 5-16-14-12 Makuharihongo, Hanamigawa-ku, Chiba 262-33, Japan. E-mail:tnuka@hotmail.co.jp 3 Faculty of Bioresources, Mie University, 1577 Kurimamachiya-cho, Tsu, Mie 514-857, Japan 4 Japan Scientific Texts, 179-5 Imafuku, Kawagoe, Saitama 35-1151, Japan (Accepted September 7, 21) Toshimitsu Nuka, Yoshitaka Morikawa, and Christopher P. Norman (211) Use of an infrared monitor to record the frequency and timing of parental nest visitation by the Blue Rock Thrush Monticola solitarius. Zoological Studies 5(1): 16-23. We observed the nest visitation behavior of the Blue Rock Thrush Monticola solitarius by setting up an infrared monitoring system (IMS) at the entrance of a Blue Rock Thrush nest hole. Furthermore we carried out visual observations and video image analysis to confirm the IMS data and discuss the efficiency of the IMS. Two consecutive broods in 1999 (Apr.-July) and 1 brood in 26 (Apr.-June) were monitored. The IMS recorded a consistent nest visitation pattern in all 3 broods. During the incubation stage, about 2 ± 8 (mean ± S.D., n = 33) responses per day were recorded as the female emerged from the nest hole to feed at regular intervals. During the nestling stage, about 15 ± 47 (mean ± S.D., n = 42) responses per day were recorded as the parents visited the nest hole to feed the chicks at about 1 min intervals. From the light intensity and IMS data, the daily activity pattern of the Blue Rock Thrush showed clear diurnal activity from before sunrise (-.29 log Lum/m 2 ) to after sunset (-.1 log Lum/m 2 ). The efficiency of the IMS was verified using video and visual observations, and around an 87% accuracy was confirmed. We concluded that after an initial confirmation of the efficiency of the IMS, the visitation behavior of Blue Rock Thrush in the incubation and nestling stages can be estimated using IMS data. For general monitoring of behavioral patterns, the IMS is weatherproof, requires minimal maintenance, and can be used for long-term monitoring. We concluded that the IMS is more efficient than using a video camera for monitoring hole-nesting animals because the time required for analysis is greatly reduced. http://zoolstud.sinica.edu.tw/journals/5.1/16.pdf Key words: Incubation, Infrared monitoring system, Light intensity, Nest hole, Nestling. It is often difficult to observe the activity of animals that use holes or crevices for nest sites by direct visual or video observations because of the high costs and manpower required. A variety of methods (e.g., light- or mechanically triggered systems, time-lapse systems, and so on) for holedwelling and -nesting animals can be used as reviewed by Cutler and Swann (1999). In this study, an infrared monitoring system (IMS) was used to record the nest visitation time of the Blue Rock Thrush Monticola solitarius by setting up the IMS at the entrance of a nest hole. The IMS is weatherproof and inexpensive and has been demonstrated to be effective for monitoring activity patterns (Morikawa et al. 23). The IMS has the potential to monitor the movement patterns of various animals that inhabit holes, especially during low-light-intensity (dusk and dawn) periods, when direct observations are difficult. The Blue Rock Thrush is distributed in southern Europe and North Africa to East and Southeast Asia, and inhabits rocky shores and *To whom correspondence and reprint requests should be addressed. E-mail:tnuka@hotmail.co.jp 16

Nuka et al. Nest Visitation by the Blue Rock Thrush 17 mountain cliffs (Cramp 1988). The total length of this bird is 2-23 cm (Cramp 1988, Clement 2). In Japan, the subspecies M. s. philippensis occurs on rocky shores and island areas (Kiyosu 1978), and is a resident species along the rocky shores of Kamogawa, Chiba (Chiba Prefecture 1976, Nuka et al. 23). Monticola s. philippensis is sexually dimorphic with the male having a rufous-chestnut breast and blue upperparts, while the female is brown over the entire body (Cramp 1988), and therefore sexes are easily distinguishable. However, details of the ecology of the Blue Rock Thrush in Japan remain unclear. It is difficult to observe aspects of the reproductive ecology (e.g., egg incubation and visitation frequency until fledging) in the field without an extensive research effort such as continuous observations because this species nests in holes (Kiyosu 1978, Hayashi 1997, Cramp 1988). In this study, we assessed the nest-visitation behavior during the incubation and nestling stages of the Blue Rock Thrush using an IMS and discuss the parental activity and potential advantages of using the IMS. MATERIALS AND METHODS This study was carried out during 3 Mar.- 9 Aug. 1999 and 15 Apr.-31 July 26 at the Marine Biosystems Research Center, Chiba Univ. (35 7'2''N, 14 11'2''E). The laboratory is located on the west coast of Uchiura Bay, Kamogawa, Boso Peninsula, southeastern Chiba Prefecture, and faces the Pacific Ocean (Fig. 1). This area has a warm coastal climate (Marine Biosystems Research Center, Chiba University 21). The landform is of a rias coastal morphology, and the vegetation consists of evergreen and broadleaf forests. There are 2 round holes which extend into horizontal cavities in a concrete wall which faces the shore in front of the laboratory (Fig. 1). The holes were originally a water drain for the laboratory but now are blocked, and no water drains. These holes are 2 cm in diameter and 14 E 35 3'N Tokyo Chiba Lab 35 N Kamogawa Sea rocky coastal line holes direction of photo Fig. 1. Study area. Arrows on the photograph indicate the site of the nest hole. The gray, thick line, marked rocky coastline, indicates the tidal level at low water at the spring tide. 5 m

18 Zoological Studies 5(1): 16-23 (211) ~55 cm deep. The size and position of the holes horizontally set in a vertical concrete wall largely prevent disturbance by potential predators such as snakes, crows, rats, and birds of prey. Prior to the study years, the Blue Rock Thrushes were annually observed to nest in these holes (T. Nuka, pers. observ.). In the study years, the potential nest hole was identified by the nest building behavior of the female. An IMS, consisting of an infrared (IR) sensor to detect the passage of objects and a battery to run the sensor and data logger, was used to record the response time of the IR sensor (Morikawa et al. 23, costing ~US$3, and includes a PS-351 infrared sensor and beam source, EK Japan, Fukuoka, Japan, and an H6-1 data logger, Onset Computers, Hobo, Bourne, MA, USA). In 1999, the system described in Morikawa et al. (23) was used. This system was originally designed for use in seawater and was packed with silicon bond to weatherproof the system. In 26, the system (Fig. 2) was reconstructed (with a smaller-sized protective case) to reduce the volume of the sensor and beam source parts (achieving an 8% reduction) and also to increase the capacity of the battery (a 14% increase in continuous work time). Changes in the system between 1996 and 26 led to improved sensitivity as the position of the IR beam was set closer to the hole mouth. The IMS was installed at the entrance of the nest hole during 4 Apr.- 28 July 1999 and 15 Apr.- 31 July 26 (Fig. 1). After problems with data downloading during the 1st brood in 1999, battery exchange and data downloading were carried out once every 2 d. Nest visitation frequencies based on the IMS data are presented as the frequency per day or as mean values (± standard deviation; S.D.) per hour. There were no data from 12: on 3 May to 11: on 17 May in 1999 because of battery-system failure. We verified the IMS precision by visual observations in 1999 and by recording and analyzing video images of the entrance of the nest hole in 26. 2 cm Infrared Sensor Control Unit Infrared Sensor 55 4 2 mm Nest hole Infrared Beam Source 55 4 2 mm 8 m Cable Connector Data Logger Rechargeable Battery Unit 6V 4Ah Connector Infrared Sensor Control Unit Infrared Sensor Nest hole Infrared Beam Source Waterproof Case Blue Rock Thrush Fig. 2. Schematic diagram of the weatherproof infrared monitoring system. The photograph is of the male Monticola solitarius when it had alighted at the entrance of the nest hole, directly obscuring the beam of the IMS.

Nuka et al. Nest Visitation by the Blue Rock Thrush 19 The IMS and video were programmed/downloaded using the same computer, and the time setting of the computer was based on the news (NHK) time signal of the national broadcasting network. Similarly during visual observations, the observer set his watch using the NHK news time signal; differences between the IMS and observer quoted times were minimal. In 1999, using 8x binoculars and a 2-45x telescope, visual observations were made opportunistically in the daytime for 68 d (for a total of 97.4 h over 46 d for the 1st brood and 21.2 h over 22 d for the 2nd brood) from the rooftop of the laboratory which is about 4 m from the nest hole, from where we could see the opening of the nest hole. In 26, we set up a video camera to record the entrance of the nest hole opportunistically during the daytime for 24 d (for a total of 16.4 h) during the study period. The video camera was about 2 m from the entrance of the nest hole. The number of visitations by adult birds may be affected by environmental factors (Lin et al. 27). To study the effect of physical factors on bird activities, the effects of rainfall (to examine if rainfall affects the chick-feeding behavior) and light intensity (to examine if there are night behaviors which cannot be observed visually) on nest visitation frequency were analyzed. A lightintensity sensor (StowAway, Onset Computers, Bourne, MA, USA) was installed in the field on 6-22 May and 24 June- 3 July 1999. Mean values are presented with the S.D. Rainfall data were obtained from the online database of the Japan Meteorological Agency (http://www.jma.go.jp/jma/ index.html). In this study, we assessed the visitation frequencies during the incubation stage as the female Blue Rock Thrush left the nest hole to feed during incubation of the eggs and during the nestling stage when both parents brought food items to feed the chicks. The incubation stage was defined as the duration from the day a female remained for an extended period (more than 3 min) in the nest hole to the day before the chicks hatched, which was estimated from observations of when the parents began to carry prey items to the nest hole. The nestling stage was defined as the duration from the day the parents began to carry prey items to the day before the fledglings left the nest hole. Initiation of incubation and the day of hatching were also confirmed by direct observations at night using a step ladder and flashlight on 2 Apr. and 6 June 1999 and by use of a camera fixed to a telescope on 29 Apr. 26 to check whether the female stayed in the nest and whether there were nestlings in the nest, respectively. RESULTS In total, 3 broods, 2 in 1999 and 1 in 26, were measured using the IMS. The IMS data showed a similar pattern for all 3 broods. Comparing the results of the video image analysis with the IMS data, responses of the IMS were recorded according to the nest visitation of the parents (Fig. 3). Responses showed the following 4 patterns: 1) there were 2 IMS responses, one when a parent entered and one when it left the nest hole; 2) only 1 response was recorded either when a parent entered or left the nest hole; 3) multiple responses were recorded; and 4) there was no response. However, during the observations (a total of 16.4 h over 24 d on 16 Apr.- 1 July 26), the most common response 1) 1) 3) 2) 2) 2) IMS Response 2) 2) 2) 2) 9: 9:1 9:2 9:3 9:4 9:5 1: 1:1 1:2 1:3 1:4 1:5 11: Hours Fig. 3. Presence of Monticola solitarius in the nest hole based on infrared monitoring system (IMS) data and video image analysis at 9:-11: on 15 May 26. Black horizontal bars above the bar graphs indicate the presence of Monticola solitarius in the nest hole, which was confirmed by the video image analysis. The thickness of the vertical bars in the graph indicates the duration that the parents stayed at the entrance of the nest hole. 1), 2), and 3) indicate each of the response patterns (see explanation in RESULTS ).

2 Zoological Studies 5(1): 16-23 (211) patterns were 1), 2), and 3) (n = 47, 13, and 21, respectively; totaling 87% of the visually confirmed visitations), and there were few instances (n = 29) of pattern 4) of no response. Based on visual observations and the video-image analysis, multiple responses occurred because the parents remained near the entrance of the nest hole and the body or tail section triggered IMS responses. The cause of no response was because the parent bird did not cross or only partially crossed the optical axis of the IMS, when it flew directly into the nest without alighting at the entrance of the hole or it launched directly into flight from within the nest and flew above the optical axis. However, in most cases, the parent birds stopped at the entrance of the hole when they entered or left the nest. Based on the daily response frequency of the IMS, the breeding stages were deduced, and these were confirmed by visual observations and video-image analysis (Fig. 4). Based on the visual observations, during the incubation stage, the female handled all of the egg incubation. The female came out of the nest hole about once an hour during daylight hours (Table 1), and 2. ± 8.8, (n = 33) response frequencies per day were recorded by the IMS (including type 1, 2, and 3 response patterns). Based on the IMS data (Fig. 4), the incubation stage was 14 d, estimated to be 25 1st brood in 1999 2 15 1 Hatch Nestling Fledge 5 Incubation Battery failure Apr. 4 14 24 May 4 14 24 Response frequency per day 2nd brood in 1999 25 Fledge 2 15 Nestling 1 Hatch 5 Incubation May 25 June 4 14 24 July 4 14 24 25 26 2 15 Hatch Nestling Fledge 1 5 Incubation Apr. 15 25 May 5 15 25 June 4 14 24 Date Fig. 4. Daily response frequency of the infrared monitoring system (IMS) and breeding stages of Monticola solitarius assessed from the IMS response patterns for the 1st and 2nd broods in 1999 and the brood in 26. Each brood is vertically aligned in the figure to the day of hatching. White bars indicate days that rainfall exceeded 1 mm.

Nuka et al. Nest Visitation by the Blue Rock Thrush 21 17-3 Apr. for the 1st brood and 31 May- 13 June for the 2nd brood in 1999 and 28 Apr.- 11 May 26. In the nestling stage, because both male and female visited the nest hole to feed nestlings and to carry droppings outside at about once every 1 min (Table 2), response frequencies per day were higher than in the incubation stage (Mann- Whitney U-test, 1st 1999 brood n1 = 11, n2 = 3, U =, p =.11; 2nd 1999 brood n1 = 8, n2 = 17, U =, p <.1; 26 brood n1 = 14, n2 = 19, U =, p <.1), when 15.8 ± 47.3 (n = 42) responses per day were recorded. In the nestling stage, all observed prey items brought by the parents were insects or lizards; centipedes were the most frequent prey item (T. Nuka, unpubl. data). In the latter part of the nestling stage, response frequencies were about 2 responses per day, due to the grown nestlings coming to the hole entrance (the IMS was triggered by the nestlings) prior to fledging, and adults coming to the entrance to urge the nestlings to fledge. Therefore, the IMS was effective in recording the frequency of nest visits by the adults until the nestlings begin to move to the entrance of the nest hole in preparation to fledge. Nestlings fledged after 18 (2nd brood in 1999) and 19 d (1st brood in 1999 and in 26) from the estimated day of hatching. After fledging, except for the 1st brood of 1999 when the nest was reused, the response frequencies of the IMS were significantly fewer (Mann-Whitney U-test, 1st 1999 brood n1 = 3, n2 = 13, U =, p =.87; 2nd 1999 brood n1 = 17, n2 = 1, U =, p <.1; 26 brood n1 = 19, Table 1. Mean time period (minutes: seconds) that the female was out of and in the nest hole during the incubation stage (data based on direct observations and IMS data). Hyphens indicate no visual confirmation of the data Brood Out of the nest hole Mean SD N 1st, 2 to 3 Apr. in 1999 12m5s 3m46s 12 2nd, 6 to 13 June in 1999 - - - 28 Apr. to 11 May in 26 13m57s 3m56s 21 Brood In the nest hole Mean SD N 1st, 2 to 3 Apr. in 1999 53m3s 11m2s 4 2nd, 6 to 13 June in 1999 - - - 28 Apr. to 11 May in 26 68m54s 43m29s 2 Int (Log Lum/m 2 ) Response frequency.2.1 -.1 -.2 -.3 -.4 5 4 3 2 1-1 16 14 12 1 86 4 2-2 (A) Light Sunrise 1st brood 2nd brood Sunset 2 4 6 8 1 12 14 16 18 2 22 (B) Incubation 2 4 6 8 1 12 14 16 18 2 22 (C) Nestling 2 4 6 8 1 12 14 16 18 2 22 Hours Fig. 5. Light intensity and activity time of Monticola solitarius in 1999. Black triangles with continuous lines and standard deviation (minus direction) are for the 1st brood. Open triangles with broken lines and standard deviation (plus direction) are for the 2nd brood. (A) Light intensity per hour. Data for the 1st brood were based on the period of 6-2 May and the 2nd brood of 24 June- 1 July. The mean sunrise and sunset times during each breeding period are indicated by black and open triangles, respectively, at the top of the figure. (B) Response frequency of the IMS per hour in the incubation stage for the 1st (2-3 Apr.) and 2nd broods (6-13 June). (C) Response frequency of the IMS per hour in the nestling stage for the 1st (during 2-3 and then 17-19 May; see explanation in MATERIALS AND METHODS) and 2nd broods (15 June- 1 July). Table 2. Interval (minutes: seconds) between parental visits to the nest hole in the nestling stage (data based on direct observations and infrared monitoring system data) Brood Mean SD N 1st, 2 to 19 May in 1999 8m15s 7m25s 217 2nd, 14 June to 1 July in 1999 11m13s 11m54s 28 12 to 3 May in 26 1m57s 11m17s 188

22 Zoological Studies 5(1): 16-23 (211) n2 = 1, U =, p <.1). The number of fledglings was 3 birds in the 1st and 2nd broods in 1999 and 4 birds in 26. Since the IMS response patterns for the 3 broods were similar, we deduced that the incubation stage was ~14 d and nestling stage was 18-19 d in this study area. IMS data of 4-7 Apr. 1999 and 15-21 Apr. 26, before the incubation stage, was due to visitation at the entrance of the hole to seek and build the nest by the parents. IMS data of 2-27 May 1999 occurred due to reusing the nest. IMS data after fledging on 2 June 1999 and 1 July 26 occurred by occasional visits of parents to look for prey items. These behaviors were observed by video images and/or visual observations. The relation between rainfall and IMS response frequencies was studied to determine if weather affected Blue Rock Thrush activity. In the incubation and nestling stages, the correlation coefficients between daily rainfall and nest visitation frequency (only for the hours that rainfall was > 1 mm/h between 4: and 19:) were calculated. However, with the exception of when rainfall was particularly heavy at 19 mm/h at 6:-7: on 2 May 26 (only 1 response), it was found that there was no effect of rainfall (see black and white bars in Figure. 4, 1st 1999 brood n = 27, r = -.212, p =.297; 2nd 1999 brood n = 8, r = -.63, p =.1186; 26 brood n = 26, r = -.56, p =.7877; no data for 1st nestling in 1999, 2nd 1999 brood; n = 31, r =.333, p =.667; 26 brood n = 38, r = -.252, p =.1271). From the IMS data and light intensity measured by the light sensor (Figs. 5, 6), response frequencies were negligible at night (1.3 ± 6.9 per night; n = 4). The Blue Rock Thrush exhibited a clear diurnal activity pattern. Particularly during the nestling stages, parental activity of bringing food to the chicks began as soon as it became light before sunrise (-.29 ±.88 log Lum/m 2 ) and finished shortly after sunset (-.1 ±.46 log Lum/m 2 ; Fig. 5). Light was significantly brighter at the end of activity than at the initiation of activity (Mann- Whitney U-test, n1 = 1, n2 = 9, U = 1, p =.42). Since the diurnal length of the 2nd brood in 1999 was longer than that of the 1st brood, the activity time was also longer (Table 3). Response frequency 24 2 16 12 8 4 Sunrise Incubation Nestling Sunset 2 4 6 8 1 12 14 16 18 2 22 Hours Fig. 6. Response frequency of the infrared monitoring system per hour in 26. Black triangles with continuous lines are for the incubation stage. Open triangles with broken lines are for the nestling stage. The mean sunrise and sunset times are indicated by black and open triangles, respectively, at the top of the figure. DISCUSSION From the results of the visual observations in which the parents brought prey items to their chicks in the nestling stage of the 1st brood as frequently as during the 2nd brood in 1999, the behavioral pattern in the 1st nestling stage was considered to be the same as for the IMS response pattern in the 2nd brood (Fig. 5). The incubation (~14 d) and nestling (19 and 18 d) periods were deduced using the IMS in this study, and these were similar to the incubation period (12-15 d) and nestling period (~18 d) in Europe as described by Cramp (1988). The Table 3. Daily initiation and termination of activity time and the duration of activity (hours: minutes) in the incubation and nestling stages measured by the infrared monitoring system Stage Period Initiation time Termination time Duration of activity Mean SD N (Day) Mean SD N (Day) Mean SD N (Day) Incubation 2 to 3 Apr. in 1999 6:5 2:24 11 18:12 :31 11 12h7m 2h47m 11 6 to 13 June in 1999 6:1 2:5 8 18:32 :45 8 12h31m 2h4m 8 28 Apr. to 11 May in 26 4:41 :15 14 18:41 :12 14 14h 25m 14 Nestling 2 to 19 May in 1999 4:12 :9 4 18:46 :1 3 14h41m 2m 2 14 June to 1 July in 1999 4:9 :14 18 19:6 :21 18 14h57m 25m 18 12 to 3 May in 26 4:9 :7 19 18:59 :7 19 14h51m 12m 19

Nuka et al. Nest Visitation by the Blue Rock Thrush 23 activity time of the Blue Rock Thrush in the breeding period measured by the IMS extended from immediately before sunrise to immediately after sunset, which was similar to the activity time (from before sunrise to after sunset) described in western Spain (Cramp 1988). Therefore the durations of the incubation and nestling stages, the hatching day, fledging day, and activity time could be elucidated from the response pattern of the IMS. During the incubation stages in 1999, the daily time at which activity was initiated varied, ranging about 4:3-12:36, whereas the termination time was more regular (Table 3). However, during the nestling stage, both the initiation and termination times of activity showed minimal variations. If some exceptionally late times in the incubation stage in 1999 were excluded (9:17, 9:17, and 1:44 in the 1st brood and 12:36, 6:21, 6:9, and 5:56 in the 2nd brood), then the daily initiation times of activity in 1999 were 4:41 ± :15 (mean ± S.D., n = 8) in the 1st and 4:15 ± :12 (n = 4) in 2nd, and the initiation time of activity was 4:41 ± :15 (n = 14) in the incubation stage in 26. We could not find a reason why the daily initiation time of activity in the incubation stages showed such variability. However, possibilities include responses to some perceived predation threat, variability in the birds' behavior, or the IMS was not triggered (response pattern 4) on entering/exiting the hole. Based on the response pattern of the IMS in the 1st brood in 1999, the high manpower requirement for visual observations was reduced in the 2nd brood. The direct visual observation time on the 2nd brood (a total of 21.2 h for 22 d) was far less than the 1st brood (a total of 97.4 h for 46 d), although the IMS requires an initial period of visual observations to relate the IMS responses to actual behaviors. For long-term usage, apart from periodic maintenance to remove dust etc. that may obscure the infrared sensor and/or beam source, the IMS only requires occasional hard image/ observational data for confirmation of the response patterns. We suggest that the IMS is potentially effective for continuous, non-intrusive monitoring of entrance/exit patterns of hole-dwelling birds, as well as other creatures which change activity patterns depending on the breeding stage. Institute, Chiba; Dr. Y. Nagata, Marine Information Research Center, and C. Namikawa and S. Oda for their support and discussions regarding the study. REFERENCES Chiba Prefecture. 1976. Bird species list of Chiba Prefecture in 1975. Chiba, Japan: Chiba Prefecture. (in Japanese) Clement P. 2. Thrushes. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press. Cramp S, ed. 1988. Handbook of the birds of Europe, the Middle East and North Africa. The birds of the Western Palearctic. Vol. 5. Tyrant Flycatchers to Thrushes. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press. Cutler TL, DE Swann. 1999. Using remote photography in wildlife ecology: a review. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 27: 571-581. Hayashi T. 1997. Isohiyodori (Blue Rock Thrush). In H Higuchi, H Morioka, S Yamagishi, eds. The encyclopaedia of animals in Japan. Tokyo: Heibonsha, pp. 99-1. (in Japanese) Kiyosu Y. 1978. The birds of Japan, revised and enlarged edition. Tokyo: Kodansha. (in Japanese) Lin RS, CT Yao, PF Lee. 27. The diet of Fairy Pitta Pitta nympha nestlings in Taiwan as revealed by videotaping. Zool. Stud. 46: 355-361. Marine Biosystems Research Center, Chiba University. 21. The measurement results of weather and sea weather at Kominato in 1999. Ann. Rep. Mar. Biosystems Res. Ctr. Chiba Univ. 2: 61-65. (in Japanese) Morikawa Y, CP Norman, T Nuka, Y Nagata. 23. Development of a monitoring system to record the activity patterns in juvenile Japanese spiny lobsters, Panulirus japonicus. J. Jpn. Soc. Mar. Surv. Tech. 15: 17-23. Nuka T, CP Norman, Y Morikawa. 23. Avifauna around Marine Biosystems Research Center, Chiba University, and Uchiura-Bay, Amatsu-Kominato, Chiba Prefecture. Bull. Abiko Mus. Birds. 11: 25-36. (in Japanese with English summary) Acknowledgments: We thank the staff of the Marine Biosystems Research Center, Chiba Univ.; K. Kojima, Graduate School of Bioresources, Mie Univ.; K. Kuwabara, Natural History Museum and