Experimental analysis of amblyopia

Similar documents
Consequences of alternating monocular deprivation on eye alignment and convergence in cats. Randolph Blake, M. L. ]. Crawford, and Helmut V. B.

M. uch interest has recently been focused. Visual development in cats. 394 Pettigrew Investigative Ophthalmology. S.

Effects of Early Monocular Lid Suture on Spatial and Temporal Sensitivity of Neurons in Dorsal Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of the Cat

THE POSTNATAL DEVELOPMENT OF THE VISUAL CORTEX AND THE INFLUENCE OF ENVIRONMENT

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified):

abnormal lateral geniculate body. His anatomical study suggested that chiasm instead of remaining uncrossed. They thus reach the wrong hemispheres,

deprived eye (reverse occlusion). beyond 1 year of age; only two of six animals recovered sufficient vision to enable

Differential Effects of Early Monocular Deprivation on Binocular and Monocular Segments of Cat Striate Cortex

Binocular Exposure causes Suppression of the Less Experienced Eye in Cats Previously Reared with Unequal Alternating Monocular Exposure

Do blue-eyed white cats have normal or abnormal retinofugal pathways? R. W. Guillery, T. L. Hickey, and P. D. Spear

preferring rightward movement. A changeover later than 5 weeks of age peak of the critical period for directional deprivation may occur earlier

Effects of Convergent Strabismus on the Development of Physiologically Identified Retinogeniculate Axons ih Cats

geniculate nucleus of kittens raised with convergent squint in one eye,

The Role of Early Experience in the Development and Maintenance of Orientation Selectivity in the Cat's Visual Cortex: M. Stryker

Serendipity and the Siamese Cat: The Discovery That Genes for Coat and Eye Pigment Affect the Brain. Jon H. Kaas

Binocular Interactions in Striate Cortical Neurons of Cats Reared with Discordant Visual Inputs

Binocular Impulse Blockade Prevents the Formation of Ocular Dominance Columns in Cat Visual Cortex

injected eve. (Received 1 November 1977) with electrolytic lesions. A good correspondence was found between the location of

Area Centralis Position Relative to the Optic Disc Projection in Kittens as o Function of Age

CLARSBISHOP AREA IN THE CAT: LOCATION AIVD RETINOTOPICAL PROJECTION

Effects of Retinal Image Degradation on Ocular Growth in Cats

Inhibitory mechanisms in the LGN: A possible substrate for amblyopia?

PATTERN EVOKED RESPONSE DEFICIENCY IN PATTERN DEPRIVED CATS 1

Spatial and Temporal Sensitivity of Normal and Amblyopic Cats

Report. Darkness Alters Maturation of Visual Cortex and Promotes Fast Recovery from Monocular Deprivation

Spatial Analysis of Ocular Dominance Patterns in Monocularly Deprived Cats

Expression of a Surface-Associated Antigen on Y-Cells in the Cat Lateral Geniculate Nucleus Is Regulated by Visual Experience

Morphology of Retinogeniculate X and Y Axon Arbors in Cats Raised With Binocular Lid Suture

spider monkeys by recording extracellularly from single units and stimulating

lowering of the visual acuity. When closure was extended through the first by varying the age at eye closure. Waiting until 1 month of age

A Comparison of Visual Pathways in Boston and Midwestern Siamese Cats

Supplementary Material

log no. VNS23011 Ocular dominance columns in strabismus VNS23~6! :31 pm

THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 233: (1985)

WHY DO ALBINOS AND OTHER HYPOPIGMENTED MUTANTS LACK NORMAL BINOCULAR VISION, AND WHAT ELSE IS ABNORMAL IN THEIR CENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAYS?

Eye disease comes under the spotlight

Lens luxation when the lens gets wobbly

The Critical Period for Ocular Dominance Plasticity in the Ferret s Visual Cortex

(Received 29 June 1972)

Cortical Cell Orientation Selectivity Fails to Develop in the Absence of ON-Center Retinal Ganglion Cell Activity

Rapid Anatomical Plasticity of Horizontal Connections in the Developing Visual Cortex

BEHAVIOUR OF THE DOMESTIC DOG (Canis familiaris)

Development of Neuronal Response Properties in the Cat Dorsal Lateral Geniculate Nucleus During Monocular

THE PRETRIGEMINAL CAT AS AN INSTRUMENT FOR INVESTIGATION OF THE OCULAR FIXATION REFLEX

Behavioural Brain Research

Parallel Processing in the Visual System THE CLASSIFICATION OF RETINAL GANGLION CELLS AND ITS IMPACT ON THE NEUROBIOLOGY OF VISION

Regional Variation in the Representation of the Visual Field in the Visual Cortex of the Siamese Cat

1250 Reports. Axial lengths and refractive errors in kittens reared with an optically induced anisometropia. EARL L. SMITH, III, GREGORY W.

Veterinary Ophthalmology

The contralateral impairment of the orienting ocular-following reflex after lesions of the lateral suprasylvian cortex in cats

RETINITIS PIGMENTOSA*

David H. Hubel. A Biographical Memoir by Robert H. Wurtz

Emergence of Ocular Dominance Columns in Cat Visual Cortex by 2 Weeks of Age

E erimental Brain Research 9 Springer-Verlag 1986

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar

FOOTEDNESS IN DOMESTIC PIGEONS

IIEIIIEEEEEEII. EE IEEEEI h. IihhEEEEE

BY TICKING YES TO ANY RULE ON THIS CHECKLIST YOU AGREE THAT THE FACILITY ALREADY COMPLIES WITH THAT STANDARD.

Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds

Neuroscience Letters

Laminar and Columnar Distribution of Geniculo-cortical Fibers in the Macaque Monkey

Your Eye, My Eye, and the Eye of the Aye Aye: Evolution of Human Vision from 65 Million Years Ago to the Present

Canine Ophthalmology Diseases

Loose Leash Walking. Core Rules Applied:

MANAGING AVIARY SYSTEMS TO ACHIEVE OPTIMAL RESULTS. TOPICS:

1.3. Initial training shall include sufficient obedience training to perform an effective and controlled search.

Taste and Smell. Bởi: OpenStaxCollege

BY TICKING YES TO ANY RULE ON THIS CHECKLIST YOU AGREE THAT THE FACILITY ALREADY COMPLIES WITH THAT STANDARD.

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

(Received 22 November 1984) studies were made on twenty such pairs; eight X on-centre, seven Y on-centre, two

My recollections of Hubel and Wiesel and a brief review of functional circuitry in the visual pathway

Breeding and Managing Pheasants

[Frontiers in Bioscience 13, , May 1, 2008] Binocular phasic coactivation does not prevent ocular dominance segregation

1Ila and V. Canberra, A.C.T. 2601, Australia (Received 21 March 1979)

* Brain Resea,ch, 70 (l974)

MIND TO MIND the Art and Science of Training

GENETIC AND CONGENITAL CAUSES OF FELINE SKIN OR EYE MALAISE

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

PROTOCOL FOR THE HUMANE CARE AND USE OF LIVE VERTEBRATE ANIMALS

Female Persistency Post-Peak - Managing Fertility and Production

Judging the Doberman Head By Bob Vandiver

Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution

Index. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Mobility Issues and Arthritis

PERSONAL ACADEMIC RECORD PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE. Curriculum Vitae for S. Murray Sherman Page 1

MITOCW watch?v=tdodc_n-zca

Brumation (Hibernation) in Chelonians and Snakes

Establishing a routine

Sample Course Layout 1

NAME OF THE FACILITY: BY TICKING YES TO ANY RULE ON THIS CHECKLIST YOU AGREE THAT THE FACILITY ALREADY COMPLIES WITH THAT STANDARD.

IACUC Policy on Humane Endpoints in Animal Use Proposals

Body Condition Scoring Ewes

Temperature Gradient in the Egg-Laying Activities of the Queen Bee

The Development of Behavior

How the eye sees. Properties of light. The light-gathering parts of the eye. 1. Properties of light. 2. The anatomy of the eye. 3.

Rules of Connectivity between Geniculate Cells and Simple Cells in Cat Primary Visual Cortex

The Double-Blind Attack By Matthew B. Devaney

Line Manners Part I. By Jim & Phyllis Dobbs and Alice Woodyard

Please call the Pharmacy Medicines Unit on or for a copy.

Morphology and Axonal Projection Patterns of Individual Neurons in the Cat Perigeniculate Nucleus

Transcription:

Brit. J. Ophthal. (I974) 58, I76 Experimental analysis of amblyopia and strabismus COLIN BLAKEMORE AND RICHARD C. VAN SLUYTERS The Physiological Laboratory, Cambridge In the past few years physiological experiments have brought us a little closer to an understanding of some forms of developmental amblyopia-deficits of visual acuity of central, not retinal, origin, which are not correctable by optical means. While amblyopia has become a catch-all term that covers the symptoms of a multitude of disorders, there are certain stereotyped experimental procedures that invariably lead to gross disturbances in the visual system. In this paper we shall describe a few of these experiments and attempt to relate them to some of the clinical origins of amblyopia in man. The cat has been the subject of a great deal of research on the development of the visual system, though it is clear that most of the observations, with some minor differences, also apply to monkeys (see von Noorden, 1974). By extrapolation, the situation is probably similar in man. Cats, monkeys, and man are all highly binocular animals. They have roughly the same amount of binocular overlap of the two visual fields, and their eyes are extremely mobile, showing disjunctive as well as conjugate movements. There is now behavioural evidence that both cats (Fox and Blake, 197i) and monkeys (Bough, I970) normally have stereoscopic vision, being able to discriminate the relative distances of objects solely on the basis of the retinal disparity of their images. Indeed, from neurophysiological experiments there is now a fair understanding of the actual neural mechanism of stereopsis. Organization of the visual pathway and the basis of binocular vision Analysis of retinal disparity seems to demand a convergence of information from the two eyes at some point in the visual pathway. The opportunity for binocular input to individual neurones exists, of course, at any point central to the partial decussation of optic nerve fibres in the optic chiasm, but at the first synaptic relay in the lateral geniculate nucleus very few cells can actually be excited by visual stimuli shown to either eye. The nucleus is, in fact, divided into distinct laminae, each with principal input from one eye or the other (Kaas, Guillery, and Allman, I972). However, massive binocular interaction certainly occurs at the termination of geniculate cell axons in the primary visual cortex (area I 7). Most neurones of the visual cortex have excitatory input from both eyes and each cell has a specific optimal stimulus or triggerfeature that will cause it to respond. The required stimulus is almost always a dark or light bar or a black-white edge at a particular orientation (Hubel and Wiesel, I962, I968). Different cells respond to different orientations but the preferred orientation is always quite similar in the two eyes (Blakemore, Fiorentini, and Maffei, 1972). These binocular cortical cells appear to play a part in binocular depth discrimination. The two receptive fields (the areas of the two retinae from which a cortical cell gathers input) are not always on exactly corresponding points. There is considerable variation in

Experimental analysis of amblyopia and strabismus 177 the retinal disparity of the two receptive fields, over a total range of several degrees (Barlow, Blakemore, and Pettigrew, I967; Joshua and Bishop, I970; Hubel and Wiesel, I 970a). What is more, neurones often demonstrate a much stronger response when a target of the correct orientation appears in the correct position on both receptive fields, than when only one eye is open or when both eyes are open but the disparity of the two images is even slightly inappropriate (Barlow and others, I967; Joshua and Bishop, I970). This is a system, then, of disparity detecting cells: each neurone responds best when an object of the appropriate shape appears at a particular distance from the animal (for a fixed eye position). The integrity of this delicate and beautiful mechanism depends on a period of intensive development early in the animal's life. Disturbance of the visual input during this crucial phase leads to a level of disorganization of the visual cortex quite sufficient to explain the appearance of amblyopia and the loss of binocular visual function. "Deprivation amblyopia" If a kitten is reared in total darkness or, more conveniently, if it is binocularly deprived by suturing closed the lids of both eyes, it subsequently has gross deficits in visuomotor coordination, almost to the point of total blindness, with slow and only partial recovery (Ganz, Hirsch, and Tieman, I972). In such an animal the visual cortex is also abnormal. Although the majority of cells are still binocularly driven, many of them respond equally to edges of all orientations (Wiesel and Hubel, I965a), and they are not at all selective for the retinal disparity of stimuli: some workers report that in such binocularly deprived cats there are no truly orientation selective cells (Barlow and Pettigrew, 197I). Here then is a fairly close correspondence between the animal's behavioural ability and the organization of its cortical neurones. Deprivation of experience in only one eye has even more dramatic consequences. In a monocularly deprived kitten, cortical neurones have normal orientation selective properties but virtually every cell can be driven only through the visually experienced eye (Wiesel and Hubel, i965a). Such a monocularly deprived animal has good vision through the experienced eye, but appears to be almost totally blind when forced to use its deprived eye. Clinical experience shows that, in human infants also, degradation of the visual image in one.eye is a particularly potent cause of amblyopia. Fig. i shows a series of histograms that illustrate the extent to which cortical cells can be influenced by one eye or the other. The responses of cells are tested through both eyes in turn and the cell is classified into one of seven ocular dominance groups (Hubel and Wiesel, I962): A B c Normal Monocularly Strabismic bo 180- deprived 180- FIG. I Ocular domznance histograms for samples of cortical cells (see text for an ex- 150- I50- planation of the categories) 40 * 120-120- A. 224 cells from normal cats (Hubel and Wiesel, I962) 90-90 - B. I84 cells from kittens reared with the contralateral eye deprived (Wiesel and z 20 60 60 Hubel, I965a) 30- C. 384 cells from kittens with artificial c ostrabismus (Hubel and Wiesel, I965) C Ocular dominance _-0

178 Colin Blakemore and Richard C. Van Sluyters (i) Monocular cells driven only by the eye contralateral to the hemisphere containing the cell. (2) Binocular cells much more strongly driven by the contralateral than the ipsilateral eye. (3) Cells driven slightly more strongly by the contralateral than the ipsilateral eye. (4) Both eyes equal. (5) Ispilateral slightly stronger than contralateral. (6) Ipsilateral much stronger than contralateral. (7) Monocular cells driven only by the ipsilateral eye. Fig. i A shows a large sample of cells from normal adult cats while Fig. I B illustrates the results from kittens deprived of visual experience through the contralateral eye. In the latter animals, scarcely any cells are influenced by visual stimuli presented to the deprived (contralateral) retina. These disastrous effects of uniocular deprivation in kittens occur only if one eye is occluded at some time between about 3 weeks and 3 months of age (Hubel and Wiesel, I970b), and they cannot be reversed by a prolonged period of binocular vision after the initial deprivation. However, a procedure of "cross-patching" (suturing the lids of the experienced eye after the deprived eye has been re-opened) can lead to a total switch-over in ocular dominance, so that all cells are driven only by the initially deprived eye (Blakemore and Van Sluyters, I974). This reversal again occurs only if the cross-patching takes place within the 3-week to 3-month "sensitive period". A judicious choice of the timing and duration of cross-patching can produce an animal with equal numbers of cortical cells driven by the two eyes, but very few binocular neurones. Such an animal has reasonable visual ability through both eyes but presumably is deficient in stereopsis. So, in amblyopic humans, cross-patching may be expected to improve the acuity of the amblyopic eye without improving stereoscopic vision. "Strabismic amblyopia" One of the commonest forms of amblyopia in humans is the reduction of acuity (as well as stereoscopic vision) after early strabismus. No doubt there are many causes of strabismus, but some squints are probably congenital and the ocular misalignment is secondary to a gross genetic abnormality in the visual pathway. In the Siamese cat, which often exhibits an enormous esotropia, the lamination of the lateral geniculate nucleus, the retinotopic organization of the visual cortex, and the percentage of binocular cortical cells are all highly abnormal (Hubel and Wiesel, I 97 I). On the other hand, the surgical induction of strabismus, by section of the medial rectus muscle, in an otherwise normal kitten, also has considerable effect on the visual cortex (Fig. I C). Very few cells are binocularly driven and there are separate populations of neurones responding through one eye or the other (Hubel and Wiesel, I965). There is even some evidence that the strabismic eye dominates a smaller number of cortical cells than the unoperated eye (Wickelgren-Gordon, I972). Here, then, is a good correlation with "amblyopia ex anopsia" and the lack of stereopsis experienced by strabismic humans. It seems from all these experiments that the necessary condition for the maintenance of normal binocularity in the visual cortex is simultaneous congruent stimulation through both eyes. Almost certainly, then, the discrepancies in the two retinal images caused by anisometropia, aniseikonia, and squint can cause a loss of binocularity in the visual cortex of human infants. "Meridional amblyopia" Discrepancy between the two eyes is not the only condition that leads to reorganization of

Experimental analysis of aniblyopia and strabismus the visual cortex. There is now evidence that the visual system simply lacks the ability to process information that it has not experienced early in life. If a kitten is reared in such a way that its entire visual experience is limited to an array of black-white vertical edges, its cortex ultimately contains no neurones responding optimally to horizontal lines (Blakemore and Cooper, I970; Hirsch and Spinelli, I97i) and the kitten has a specific reduction in visual acuity for horizontal patterns (Muir and Mitchell, i973). This environmental modification of orientation selectivity can also take place only during the 3-week to 3-month sensitive period (Blakemore, 1974), and it can occur after as little as I hour of exposure to stripes (Blakemore and Mitchell, 1973). In humans too there is a close analogue of this experimental procedure. Infants with extreme ocular astigmatism (which on average defocuses one meridian much more than the orthogonal), if they do not receive cylindrical spectacle correction early in life, are left with an uncorrectable "meridional amblyopia". Acuity for contours that were originally out of focus is considerably lower than that for the best focused meridian, even when the optical error is perfectly corrected (Mitchell, Freeman, Millodot, and Haegerstrom, 1973). The difference in acuity between the two principal meridians can be as much as a factor of two. Teleologically, it would be gratifying to discover some functional value in this process of environmental modification (in the same way that the loss of binocularity after strabismus can be construed as a protection against the conflict of two inter-dependent mappings between visual and motor space). The obvious possible adaptive function is that this plasticity ensures that the powers of detection of the visual system are optimally matched to the visual environment in which the animal finds itself (or, more exactly, to the environment it lived in which it was young). However, an alternative, or additional, function comes to mind. An essential property of binocular neurones, if they are really to act as disparity detectors, is that their optimal features should be quite closely matched in the two eyes, so that they discharge very strongly only when the two similar images of a single object appear on the receptive fields. Modification of orientation selectivity early in life may be important in ensuring that binocular neurones do indeed adopt very similar preferred orientations through the two eyes (Blakemore and Van Sluyters, 1974). A method qfpreventing some consequences of strabismus During the past year or so we have been following up a quite fortuitous observation that we hope may be of direct value in the clinical treatment of strabismus (Van Sluyters and Blakemore, 1973; and in preparation). We have noticed that normal kittens kept in the dark, except for regular exposure to striped patterns displayed on the inside walls of a large cylindrical enclosure, often exhibit an enormous "natural" strabismus-exotropias, esotropias, and hypertropias all being common. And yet the cortical neurones of these naturally squinting cats are quite normal in their binocularity (although the distribution of preferred orientations is, of course, strongly biased towards the orientation of the stripes in the cylinder). Two questions, then, are posed by this curious observation: (a) Why do kittens reared in stripes develop strabismus? (b) Why does this strabismus not lead to a loss of binocularity in the visual cortex? In fact, the first question should more properly be reframed as "Why do kittens reared in stripes not adopt correct ocular alignment?", because kittens are known to be born with gross divergence of the visual axes (Sherman, I972). With normal visual experience the eyes gradually adopt their correct convergence angle by the age of about 6 weeks. Presumably the visual axes normally stop converging when the images of the visual world are 1179

i8o Colin Blakemore and Richard C. Van Sluyters correctly lined up, in rough correspondence on the two retinae, and binocular cells are therefore consistently stimulated by correlated images. In the case of a kitten viewing an environment of repetitive vertical stripes, there is no unique convergence angle at which the visual world is "locked" in correspondence on the two retinae. Wherever the two eyes are pointing the views they gain are very similarnamely an array of vertical black-white edges. So it is hardly surprising that the final convergence angle adopted is often not appropriate. Likewise, whatever the actual alignment of the visual axes, cortical cells will constantly be stimulated simultaneously by virtually identical features through the two eyes. Therefore the necessary conditions for the maintenance of binocularity are fulfilled, despite the presence of a strabismus. So it is the repetitive nature of the striped stimulus and the multiple ambiguity of its retinal disparity that seem to create the strabismus and to maintain cortical binocularity. The next step was straightforward. Would a kitten with an enormous artificial strabismus, reared in a striped environment, also retain binocular cortical neurones? We have now reared a number of animals in repetitive environments after the surgical induction of strabismus and in every case binocularity has been quite normal. Figs 2 and 3 (opposite) show ocular dominance histograms from two kittens with large exotropias, that were housed in the dark with regular exposure in a striped cylinder. One kitten was exposed to vertical stripes (Fig. 2) and the other to horizontal (Fig. 3). Clearly there is no obvious loss of binocularity compared with the normal adult cat (Fig. I A), quite unlike the typical strabismic animal (Fig. I C). Also shown in Figs 2 and 3 are polar diagrams of the distributions of preferred orientations. Each line on these diagrams represents the optimal orientation for a single cortical cell. These show the very clear bias in the distribution of preferred orientations caused by the exposure to stripes. Now we are working to try to discover the minimum amount of exposure to repetitive texture necessary to keep up fairly normal cortical binocularity. Our hope is that we may be able to develop a simple regimen of visual treatment that could be employed with very young children exhibiting strabismus, before the age at which surgical re-alignment can be very accurately performed. If such a procedure would, as in the kitten, sustain the binocularity of cortical cells despite the strabismus, there is a possibility that, once the eyes are re-aligned by surgical or orthoptic treatment, the patient may have the chance not only of not suffering an amblyopia but also of enjoying binocular fusion and stereopsis. R. C. Van Sluyters holds a National Institutes of Health Special Fellowship. Our research on strabismus and amblyopia in kittens has been generously supported by a Programme Grant (No. G972/463/B) from the Medical Research Council. Discussion VON NOORDEN It is obviously vital to know the best time to align the eyes by optical or surgical means in cases of congenital strabismus, and I have planned monkey experiments to this end. On the other hand, it is unproven that congenital strabismus is a primary motor defect and that it is only necessary to align the eyes, in order to develop binocular vision. It is also possible that we are dealing with a primary central sensory defect: an inability of cortical neurons to respond to binocular foveal visual input. A secondary deviation may then develop as a result of this congenital defect, as there is no central reward for keeping the eye straight. I do not feel, therefore, that it is reasonable to draw all the conclusions that Dr. Blakemore has drawn in the case of congenital strabismus.

9-8- -V -6 5- E 4 z -O 3 z 3-2 - 10- I1-0 Eye dominance 7-6 - 5-4 - 2- C Eye Edom'inance 2 d 4 H ---w // I V V t-:.--..777 Experimental analysis of amblyopia and strabismus FIG. 2 Sample of 24 cortical cellsfrom a kitten with a large artificial exotropia reared in a darkroom with regular exposure to an environment of vertical edges. On the lejt is a histogram of ocular dominance, on the right a polar diagram in which each line shows the preferred orientation for one neurone FIG. 3 A sample of 27 cortical neurones, as in Fig. 2, for a strabismic kitten exposed only to horizontal edges BLAKEMORE The treatment we have suggested might be more appropriate in strabismus associated with a gross anisometropia or hypermetropia. However, if one keeps up the binocular stimulation of the cortex while not correcting the strabismus, then one may generate another problem a condition far worse than amblyopia, namely the diplopia which should result. However, if there is binocularity in the cortex, then the chances of normal binocular vision after surgical correction are much, much better. REINHART If I understood correctly, there was no correlation between loss of cortical binocularity and behavioural evidence of lowered visual acuity. Can amblyopia be produced by strabismus rather than by monocular deprivation? VAN SLUYTERS An animal with reduced cortical binocularity as a result of a neonatal strabismus shows no apparent loss of visual acuity when tested with both eyes open or with either eye alone. Certainly the most pronounced effect that we have noticed was in the cells of the visual cortex. They lose binocularity and remain connected to either the squinting eye or the normal eye, but otherwise appear completely normal in their receptive field properties, responsiveness, and so forth. These cortical changes would suggest a loss in binocular visual function with little change in visual acuity. Unfortunately, tests of binocular visual ability are very difficclt to administer to kittens, so we have restricted our behavoural testing to a repertoire of simple visual tasks. -a- H I8I

x82 Colin Blakemore and Richard C. Van Sluyters BLAKEMORE Yes; our tests of behavioural function are extremely crude by the standards that are applied to humans, and they would probably only reveal a deficit so great that it would be regarded in the human as some form of blindness, not amblyopia. HERMANN Has Dr. Blakemore made any of the animals convergent rather than divergent, as this is a situation much more commonly found in man? BLAKEMORE I do not know of anyone who has recorded from the cortex of cats with an experimental esotropia. But there have been experiments in the Siamese cat, which often exhibits a convergent squint, known to be due to genetic rather than optical or artificially-induced factors. The cerebral organization is totally different from that found in experimental exotropic animals. IKEDA It is extremely difficult to produce a convergent squint in the cat. We found that several muscles had to be removed completely before a permanent squint with a sign of reduction of acuity could be achieved. WHITWELL Is a tarsorrhaphy not a very drastic form of occlusion and does not the actual trauma of the operation in some way affect the responses obtained? Will a contact lens or something rather more subtle produce the same results. BLAKEMORE Occlusion has, in fact, been obtained by using translucent contact lenses or by suturing the nictitating membrane to the opposite area of the conjunctiva, but the results have been the same. The animals do not experience any obvious discomfort and are certainly not adversely affected by the lid-suturing operation. Our method is simple and sure, and we therefore prefer it. Br J Ophthalmol: first published as 10.1136/bjo.58.3.176 on 1 March 1974. Downloaded from http://bjo.bmj.com/ on 17 March 2019 by guest. Protected by copyright.