DIALLEL CP~OSSINGS WITH THE DOMESTtO FOWL. BY JOHS. SCHMIDT, D.Sc, Director of the Carlsberg Physiological Laboratory, Copenhagen, _Den~nar/c.

Similar documents
Probability and Heredity

Biology 201 (Genetics) Exam #1 120 points 22 September 2006

The Inheritance of Coat Colour in the Cardigan Welsh Corgi by Ken Linacre

Sex-Influenced (Autosomes) P Horned x Hornless HH H'H' H H' F 1 Horned x Hornless HH' HH' 1/2 H 1/2 H' 1/2 H 1/2 H' F 2 Genotypes Phenotypes

Understandings, Applications and Skills (This is what you maybe assessed on)

INHERITANCE OF BODY WEIGHT IN DOMESTIC FOWL. Single Comb White Leghorn breeds of fowl and in their hybrids.

Genetics. What s Genetics? An organism s heredity is the set of characteristics it receives from its parents.

Seed color is either. that Studies Heredity. = Any Characteristic that can be passed from parents to offspring

Next Wednesday declaration of invasive species due I will have Rubric posted tonight Paper is due in turnitin beginning of class 5/14/1

Part One: Introduction to Pedigree teaches students how to use Pedigree tools to create and analyze pedigrees.

The purpose of this lab was to examine inheritance patters in cats through a

Mendelian Genetics Problem Set

W. E. CASTLE C. C. LITTLE. Castle, W. E., and C. C. Little On a modified Mendelian ratio among yellow mice. Science, N.S., 32:

TRICOLOR IIVHERITANCE TORTOISESHELL CATS'

Here are some ground rules that you should ALWAYS follow when tackling an Inheritance Problem:

3. Complete the Punnett square for heterozygous yellow (yellow is dominant): What is the genotype: and what is the phenotype:

Science 10-Biology Activity 17 Worksheet on More Complex Genetics

Biology 3201 Sex Linked Review Mr.Gillam Name:

LINKAGE OF ALBINO ALLELOMORPHS IN RATS AND MICE'

Inheritance of Livershunt in Irish Wolfhounds By Maura Lyons PhD

9-2 Probability and Punnett. Squares Probability and Punnett Squares. Slide 1 of 21. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Biology 100. ALE #8. Mendelian Genetics and Inheritance Practice Problems

SHEEP SIRE REFERENCING SCHEMES - NEW OPPORTUNITIES FOR PEDIGREE BREEDERS AND LAMB PRODUCERS a. G. Simm and N.R. Wray

Genetics. Labrador Retrievers as a Model System to Study Inheritance of Hair Color. Contents of this Section

Mendelian Genetics Using Drosophila melanogaster Biology 12, Investigation 1

Breeding Icelandic Sheepdog article for ISIC 2012 Wilma Roem

Improving Mongrel Farm Flocks Through Selected Standardbred Cockerels

I yellow, a great assortment of shades of red and yellow being known. The

Virtual Lab: Sex-Linked Traits Worksheet. 1. Please make sure you have read through all of the information in the

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics

Mendelian Genetics 1

ECONOMIC studies have shown definite

Heredity. What s heredity? An organism s heredity is the set of characteristics it receives from its parents. Today, known as genetics.

We are learning to analyze data to solve basic genetic problems

ON COMMERCIAL poultry farms during

Genetics & Punnett Square Notes

Right and next page: Brahma chicks with decent footfeathering, but with no fluff on the inner side of the legs and on the inner toes.

PLEASE PUT YOUR NAME ON ALL PAGES, SINCE THEY WILL BE SEPARATED DURING GRADING.

husband P, R, or?: _? P P R P_ (a). What is the genotype of the female in generation 2. Show the arrangement of alleles on the X- chromosomes below.

Furry Family Genetics

Lesson 4.7: Life Science Genetics & Selective Breeding

Incomplete Dominance, Co-Dominance, and Sex-linked dominance NON-MENDELIAN GENETICS

Genetics Practice Problems. 1. For each genotype, indicate whether it is heterozygous (HE) or homozygous (HO) AA Bb Cc Dd.

What is Genetics? Genetics is the scientific study of heredity

YOUNG MATURING PIGEONS By Mick Bassett

Selective Breeding. Selective Breeding

Breeder Cobb 700. The Cobb 700 has been introduced to meet the. Ten years of research to develop Cobb 700. Breeder Performance

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

13. Cell division is. assortment. telophase. cytokinesis.

17 Inherited change Exam-style questions. AQA Biology

Problem 1. What is the simplest explanation for the inheritance of these colors in chickens?

Genetics Lab #4: Review of Mendelian Genetics

7. Flock book and computer registration and selection

Phenotypic and Genetic Variation in Rapid Cycling Brassica Parts III & IV

Pedigree Analysis and How Breeding Decisions Affect Genes

The Dihybrid Problem Solve

Breed Selection for a Small-Scale Egg-Production Enterprise

THE MASKING OF SEPIA BY WHITE, TWO RECESSIVE

UNIT 6 Genes and Inheritance sciencepeek.com

Approximating the position of a hidden agent in a graph

Name: Block: Date: Packet #12 Unit 6: Heredity

PIMA COUNTY 4-H/FFA DEPARTMENT M POULTRY

Determining the Inheritance Patterns of Purple Eye, Lobe Eye, and Yellow Body Traits of. Drosophilia Flies. Introduction

3. records of distribution for proteins and feeds are being kept to facilitate tracing throughout the animal feed and animal production chain.

EVALUATION OF PRODUCTIVE TRAITS OF CHICKEN LINES FROM THE NATIONAL GENE POOL

Mendel s Laws: Their Application to Solving Genetics Problem

Breeding Bunnies. Purpose: To model the changes in gene frequency over several generations. 50 orange beads 50 purple beads 1 paper bag 3 cups

Do Now: Answer the following question based on the information below.

Genetics Problem Set

S7L2_Genetics and S7L5_Theory of Evolution (Thrower)

TECHNICAL BULLETIN Claude Toudic Broiler Specialist June 2006

Sex-linked Inheritance

Genetics and Probability

Baby Steps Through the PUNNETT SQUARE

Genetics Problems. Character Dominant Recessive

LAB. NATURAL SELECTION

Sections 2.1. and 2.2. (Single gene inheritance, The chromosomal basis of single-gene inheritance patterns)

Introduction to Cladistic Analysis

Welcome to Jeopardy! Genetics. Please get your blood typing lab out for me to check. Come up to my desk with your partner

Cow Exercise 1 Answer Key

AS91157 Demonstrate understanding of genetic variation and change. Punnett Squares

Naked Bunny Evolution

Completing your Post-Birth Weight Performance Recording Forms

Mendelian Genetics Problems

Thursday, April 16, 2015 HEREDITY

Genes What are they good for? STUDENT HANDOUT. Module 4

Breeding Self cavies - some top fanciers tell their secrets

Kentucky Academic Standards

Name period date assigned date due date returned. The Genetics of Garden Peas

TOPIC 8: PUNNETT SQUARES

Today: Mendel s Technique: What Mendel Observes: Mendelian Genetics: Consider this. Mendelian Genetics and Problems (In-Class 6)

Re: Sample ID: Letzty [ ref:_00di0ijjl._500i06g6gf:ref ] 1 message

RECESSIVE BUDGIES: A BEGINNERS INTRODUCTION TO RECESSIVES IN BUDGERIGARS.

This AHT Information Sheet contains details on late-onset PRA in three breeds: Gordon Setters, Irish Setters and Tibetan Terriers.

THE INDIVIDUALITY OF SOWS IN REGARD TO SIZE OF LITTERS

Name: Period: Student Exploration: Mouse Genetics (One Trait)

Chapter 11-2 Probability and Punnett Squares Notes

ASU Martin Luther King, Jr. Celebration 2004 Student Essay Winners Secondary. First Place. Catherine Dodge

Agricultural Institute - Stara Zagora, Bulgaria. Key words: hens, еggs, breeding, live weight, productivity, feed consumption, hatchability

Transcription:

DIALLEL CP~OSSINGS WITH THE DOMESTtO FOWL. BY JOHS. SCHMIDT, D.Sc, Director of the Carlsberg Physiological Laboratory, Copenhagen, _Den~nar/c. Ii~ three previous papers (1919, 1 ; 1919, 2 ; 1921) I have described the results of diallel crossings with tile eonnnon trout (Saline trutta L.)*. The task being to elassif~ or to value a eerbain number of individuals in regard to some g'uantitative character, i.e. a character which may be analysed by counting, measuring or weighing--the maturer of proceeding in diallel crossing is as follows. Each female is paired with each male, care being taken that the offspring is raised under external conditions as uniform as possible. If there are a females and b males c~ b different combinations will occur among ~he offspring of the diallel crossings. Within each offspring-combination the character in question is analysed --by counting, measuring, or weighing each individual--and then the average value of all individuals is determined, it is with suck average val.ues--determiued by.analysing a number of individuals, as ga'eat as possible ; and developed under external conditions as uniform as possible --that the method of d'iallel crossings operates. Already the first experiments showed the necessity of distinguishing clearly between the purely personal value of an individum--in the case under consideration the number of vertebrae that may be counted in ~he individual at hand--and the gene~'ative value, i.e. the value it imparts to its offspring. The generative value of an individual may differ widely from its personal value, so that from the latter few or no conclusions can be drawn with regard to the former. This parr fact is largely responsible Ibr the difficulties which the work with quantitative characters entails, not only upon the student of genetics, but also upon the practical breeder. _R is the generative value which both of ~,hem need to know, the geneticist when he has to classify the ofs from a crossing experiment, and the breeder when he has to select-- i ~'Tfa~ valeur de Findividu 's [Rre de g&n4ra[eur, apprdei4e suivant la md[hode du Croisdmen~ diall&le " (6'.IL Laboratoire Carlsberg, Copenhagen, 1919); "/)iallel crossings wrh "~,rou~ (Saline ~rutta L.) (Journal of Genetics, ix. 1919); "The numerical significance of fused vertebrae" (6'.R. Laboratoire Ca~rlsberg, Copenhagen, 1921).

2 Diallel 9 wita the Domestic Fowl perhaps among a number of individuals personally very similar--those best fit to breed fi'om. A fi'equently occurring special case of diallel crossings is when b= t, i.e. when a single male--let us call it x--is paired with a females. 1 x a = a different offspring-combinations will then arise, Each of these will, provided a sufficiently large nmnber of individuals be present, exhibit; a full representation of all t;he different gametes possessed by the father x, which is common to all of them. The a offspring-combinations, therefore, differ only in those gametes coming from each of the a mothers. From this it follows that we are able to compare directly the average values of the c~ offspring-combinations, and to ge~ thereby an expression of the differences between the generative values of the females, the common male x cancelling, so to speak, in such a comparison. These differences are thus independenl of the father common to all offspringcombinations. After pairing a number of females with the same male we can t;herefore determine the differences between their generative values irrespective of t;he male, and similarly, we may, of course, determine the differences between a number of males, after having paired them with the same female. This is the leading principle in the method of diallel crossi,~gs. When this method was described, in 1919, we did not know the way in which such quantitative characters are inherited, and the manner of proceeding had therefore necessarily to be entirely empirical ~. In consequence of its principle the method, however, is self-checlci~g. When pairing the same a females, which were paired with x, with other males (y, z, v), and calculating the differences between the values of the females in the same way as after the first pairing with male x, the same d~'erences were fo~o~d in cell cases. In this way the automatic cheeldng of the method yielded experimental evidence of the,rightness of the principle of diallel crossings. In our ~rout experiments we were dealing with heterozygous individuals the genetic structure of which was entirely unknown. Nevertheless it proved possible to determine the differences between the various individuals--in other words to classify them--with no less certainty than when classifying a number of Pure Lines. On that subject I refer to my previous papers (1919, 1 and 1919, 2). With a character like "number of vertebrae" we have to do with so-called integrated variates, which are generally realised only as integral 1 Since then I have been able to clear up the genetic behaviour of the number of vertebrae. A detailed report is expected to appear shortly.

Jogs. ScgMI]),r 2 numbers. The diallel crossings showed that not only the generative values, or the differences between them, maybe any fi'action, but further, that the personal values may be fractions. Certain "fusions" of two vertebrae in the trout thus had to be interpreted as " attempts" at realising fi'actional parts of a vertebra (1921). This would seem to mean that no fundamental difference exists between "integrated variates" and "classvariates." When it was established that the method of diallel crossings could be applied to characters as different as the number of vertebrae in fishes and the length of petals in a species of IVicotiana (1919, 1) there was reason to believe that the method might be generally employed in the study of quantitative characters. It appeared to me, however, of importance to have the method tested for as many species as possible. After having tried various vertebrates I selected the domestic fowl which proved to'be a useful subject. The experiments were made in 1919 and 192 in the Carlsberg Laboratory, Copenhagen, and the character examined was again the number of vertebrae. This number proved to vary more than was expected. As a matter of fact, in our small stock we found a range of 6, the number of vertebrae varying from to 44. The countings were made by Mr Vilh. EgG, M.Sc., who has worked out a very reliable technique. Two cocks (x and y) and five hens (a, b, d, e, f) were used i~, the experiment. Most of them were mongrels; y was a Faverolles cock and e a Plymouth Rock hen. Table I shows the result of the diallel crossings. For each of the ten (2 5) offspring-samples the number of vertebrae in each individual chicken is shown graphically. The average numbers of vertebrae of the various offspring-combinations are given in Table II, the figures in each case being denoted by the symbols of the parents, thus : x+a 132 means of the ten equations 2 -- "53 etc. ~ -- 4,1"21 etc. we are, in the manner described in my previous papers, able to calculate the generative values of the 7 parents and to compare them with their personal values. Several of the animals being, however, still alive, we do not know all the personal values as yet and we may therefore better postpone such a comparison lmtil all the figures in question are available

244 Diallel Crossings zoith, the Domestic Fowl (for the hens e mid f the personal values a:e given in l;he foot-note on page 24,5). TABLE I. Diallel ~'os~ngs of two cocks (x and y) ~oith five hens (a, b, d, e, f). The graphs show the nwmber of vertebrae in each individual of the o~pring. Femules ~M:ule ~: ]~Iale y O a b, d e.{ f 4O O TABLE II. Average numbers of vertebrae in the ten offspring-combinations (first and second columns)and di~'erence between the generative values of cocks y and x (third column).,11 4O x+a ="53 y+a ="21 "68 2 2 + bx = -88 y+b =.57 "69 ') "~:4 x + d =.96 Y + d =" 59 "63 2 2 x+~ 2 ="53 Y +e ----"2 "67 2 ~ = "11 Y 2 +f = "78 "67 For the time being we shall therefore content ourselves with an examination of the applicability of the method to the present subject-- number of vertebrae in the domestic fowl--in other words to examine whether the method already tested for other species also holds good in the present case. T

Jot!s. S(JHM~DT 24:5 As the two cocks x and y both enter into all 5 crossings, the differences between their generative values have been determined 5 times, each determination being made independently of the other. In Table I the offspring-combinations of a vertical row have the same father (x and y) and those of a horizontal row have the same mother (a, b, d, e,f). Already an inspection of the graphs shows that cock y must have a greater generative value than cock x. Suppose the property of imparting a great number of vmsebrae to its offspring is a valuable character, we are enabled by our experiment to classit) cock y as being superior to cock x. In Table II we find numerical expressions tbr the five determinations y-x of-if-, that is to say of the differences between the generative values of y and w. It must be remembered that the determinations were made independently of each other, by means of five different hens. We see that the five val~tes for y - w vary between "63 and "69. In spite of the number of offspring-individuals in some cases being but small, these rallies agree so well that no reasonable do~tbt can exist that the principle of diallel crossings holds good here as i'n the previously ewstained cases. Provided that the character in question had been of practical importance itself, or if another character of practical importance, e.g. a great egg-laying capacity were associated with a high number ot' vertebrae, our valuation of the two cocks would have been of practical importance. Through a direct exmnination of the personal values of the two animals little or nothing could have been concluded in regard to their generative values ~. Note added during printing. Cock y died on 14/3/1929, and on examination proved to possess vertebrae, i.e. the same number as cock w which died in 192. This yields a fresh instance of the fact that two personally identical individuals may differ widely in regard to the generative values. The difference between the generative values of cocks y and w, or rather, the half difference (~), appears fi'om Table II. 1 I may mention here that hens e and f both proved to possess vertebrae (personal values). From Table II it appears that they are far from being identical in regard to generative values. The difference between the latter amounts to no less than 1"16 (~e='58).