FORAGING MODE OF THE SAND LIZARD, Lacerta agilis, AT THE BEGINNING OF ITS YEARLY ACTIVITY PERIOD. Szilárd Nemes 1

Similar documents
Effects of movement and eating on chemosensory tongue-flicking and on labial-licking in the leopard gecko (Eublepharis macularius)

Correlated evolution of thermal characteristics and foraging strategy in lacertid lizards

CAMBRIDGE, MASS. 26 MARCH 2010 NUMBER 519 CRUISE FORAGING OF INVASIVE CHAMELEON (CHAMAELEO JACKSONII XANTHOLOPHUS) IN HAWAI I

Foraging by the Omnivorous Lizard Podarcis lilfordi: Effects of Nectivory in an Ancestrally Insectivorous Active Forager

Historical introduction: on widely foraging for Kalahari lizards

Effects of prey availability and climate across a decade for a desert-dwelling, ectothermic mesopredator. R. Anderson Western Washington University

HOW OFTEN DO LIZARDS "RUN ON EMPTY"?

Pheromone-Mediated Intrasexual Aggression in Male Lizards, Podarcis hispanicus

Reproductive cycle of the common rough-scaled lizard, Ichnotropis squamulosa (Squamata: Lacertidae) from southern Africa.

Foraging mode of the chameleon, Bradypodion pumilum: a challenge to the sit-and-wait versus active forager paradigm?

Lizard malaria: cost to vertebrate host's reproductive success

LIZARD ECOLOGY. D ONALD B. MILES is Professor in the Department of Biological Sciences at Ohio University.

DECREASED SPRINT SPEED AS A COST OF REPRODUCTION IN THE LIZARD SCELOPORUS OCCIDENTALS: VARIATION AMONG POPULATIONS

Mother offspring recognition in two Australian lizards, Tiliqua rugosa and Egernia stokesii

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour

Maturity and Other Reproductive Traits of the Kanahebi Lizard Takydromus tachydromoides (Sauria, Lacertidae) in Mito

Flexibility in antipredatory behavior allows wall lizards to cope with multiple types of predators

Cnemidophorus lemniscatus (Rainbow Whiptail)

A Population Analysis of the Common Wall Lizard Podarcis muralis in Southwestern France

Sex identification of juvenile sand lizards, Lacerta agilis using digital images

Prey Chemical Discrimination and

This item is the archived peer-reviewed author-version of:

How Often Do Lizards "Run on Empty"? Raymond B. Huey; Eric R. Pianka; Laurie J. Vitt. Ecology, Vol. 82, No. 1. (Jan., 2001), pp. 1-7.

(Received May 6, 1994; accepted June 27, 1994)

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

Spatial and reversal learning in congeneric lizards with different foraging strategies

The Long-term Effect of Precipitation on the Breeding Success of Golden Eagles Aquila chrysaetos homeyeri in the Judean and Negev Deserts, Israel

Seasonal Shifts in Reproductive Investment of Female Northern Grass Lizards ( Takydromus septentrionalis

ECOLOGICAL ASPECTS ON LIZARD POPULATIONS FROM OBCINELE BUCOVINEI (SUCEAVA)

Chemosensory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by lizards: implications of field spatial relationships between males

Omnivorous lacertid lizards (Gallotia) from El Hierro, Canary Islands, can identify prey and plant food using only chemical cues

The Role of Thermoregulation in Lizard Biology: Predatory Efficiency in a Temperate Diurnal Basker

Ecological Archives E A2

Prof. Neil. J.L. Heideman

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Objectives: Outline: Idaho Amphibians and Reptiles. Characteristics of Amphibians. Types and Numbers of Amphibians

All about snakes. What are snakes? Are snakes just lizards without legs? If you want to know more

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF CTENOPHORUS CAUDICINCTUS (AGAMIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

Reliable proxies for glandular secretion production in lacertid lizards

4B: The Pheasant Case: Handout. Case Three Ring-Necked Pheasants. Case materials: Case assignment

08 alberts part2 7/23/03 9:10 AM Page 95 PART TWO. Behavior and Ecology

Colorful tails fade when lizards adopt less risky behaviors

Dipsas trinitatis (Trinidad Snail-eating Snake)

Tail Autotomy Does Not Increase Locomotor Costs in the Oriental Leaf-toed Gecko Hemidactylus bowringii

Journal of Environmental Biology January 2008, 29(1) (2008)

Influence of Incubation Temperature on Morphology, Locomotor Performance, and Early Growth of Hatchling Wall Lizards (Podarcis muralis)

Introduction. Miyuki Fukudome 1 Yoshifumi Yamawaki

The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis)

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

Lacerta vivipara Jacquin

ethology international journal of behavioural biology

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF TWO SPECIES OF EGERNIA (SCINCIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

Tail bifurcation in Common wall lizard (Podarcis muralis LAURENTI, 1768) from Liguria, Italy. Lukáš Pola & Daniel Koleška.

Sources And Consequences of Ecological Intraspecific Variation In The Florida Scrub Lizard (Sceloporus Woodi)

Silk feeding as an alternative foraging tactic in a kleptoparasitic spider under seasonally changing environments

Effect of Tail Loss on Sprint Speed and Growth in Newborn Skinks, Niveoscincus metallicus

The Origin of Species: Lizards in an Evolutionary Tree

Plestiodon (=Eumeces) fasciatus Family Scincidae

History and the Global Ecology of Squamate Reptiles

Supplementary Fig. 1: Comparison of chase parameters for focal pack (a-f, n=1119) and for 4 dogs from 3 other packs (g-m, n=107).

Sheikh Muhammad Abdur Rashid Population ecology and management of Water Monitors, Varanus salvator (Laurenti 1768) at Sungei Buloh Wetland Reserve,

VIRIDOR WASTE MANAGEMENT LIMITED. Parkwood Springs Landfill, Sheffield. Reptile Survey Report

Who Cares? The Evolution of Parental Care in Squamate Reptiles. Ben Halliwell Geoffrey While, Tobias Uller

BODY TEMPERATURE, THERMAL TOLERANCE AND INFLUENCE OF TEMPERATURE ON SPRINT SPEED AND FOOD ASSIMILATION IN ADULT GRASS LIZARDS,

Supporting Online Material for

Phenotypic Effects of Thermal Mean and Fluctuations on Embryonic Development and Hatchling Traits in a Lacertid Lizard, Takydromus septentrionalis

Meal Size Effects on Antipredator Behavior of Hatchling Trinket Snakes, Elaphe helena

Differential Avoidance of Snake Odours by a Lizard: Evidence for Prioritized Avoidance Based on Risk. Jessica Stapley

Byall, C., H. M. Smith, and D. Chiszar Response of Brown Tree Snakes (Boiga

SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN HEAD SIZE IN THE LITTLE BROWN SKINK (SCINCELLA LATERALIS)

Movement correlates of lizards dorsal pigmentation patterns

DOES VIVIPARITY EVOLVE IN COLD CLIMATE REPTILES BECAUSE PREGNANT FEMALES MAINTAIN STABLE (NOT HIGH) BODY TEMPERATURES?

Prey processing in lizards: behavioral variation in sit-and-wait and widely foraging taxa

THE HERPETOLOGICAL JOURNAL

The effect of plant consumption in the overall diet of an omnivorous lizard

Reproductive Strategy and Cycle of the Toad-headed Agama Phrynocephalus grumgrzimailoi (Agamidae) in Xinjiang, China

COMPARING BODY CONDITION ESTIMATES OF ZOO BROTHER S ISLAND TUATARA (SPHENODON GUNTHERI) TO THAT OF THE WILD, A CLINICAL CASE

8/19/2013. What is convergence? Topic 11: Convergence. What is convergence? What is convergence? What is convergence? What is convergence?

The Diet and Foraging Strategy of Varanus acanthurus

Appendix 6.4. Reptile Survey

Station 1 1. (3 points) Identification: Station 2 6. (3 points) Identification:

The relationship between limb morphology, kinematics, and force during running: the evolution of locomotor dynamics in lizardsbij_

Multiple paternity in clutches of common lizard Lacerta vivipara: data from microsatellite markers

Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles

Animal Biodiversity. Teacher Resources - High School (Cycle 1) Biology Redpath Museum

Western North American Naturalist

7 CONGRESSO NAZIONALE

Species Fact Sheets. Order: Gruiformes Family: Cariamidae Scientific Name: Cariama cristata Common Name: Red-legged seriema

John Thompson June 09, 2016 Thompson Holdings, LLC P.O. Box 775 Springhouse, Pa

Red-Tailed Hawk Buteo jamaicensis

Today there are approximately 250 species of turtles and tortoises.

Proximate Causes of Intraspecific Variation in Locomotor Performance in the Lizard Gallotia galloti

An assesstnent of the itnportance of heathlands as habitats for reptiles

Vigilance Behaviour in Barnacle Geese

Result Demonstration Report

Biology. Slide 1of 50. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

A Comparison of morphological differences between Gymnophthalmus spp. in Dominica, West Indies

SOAR Research Proposal Summer How do sand boas capture prey they can t see?

Temperature Relationships of Two Oklahoma Lizards

This is the book of David, the son of Abraham and the father of Jesus Christ Matthew 1.1. Declaration

Transcription:

Russian Journal of Herpetology Vol. 9, No. 1, 2002, pp. 57 62 FORAGING MODE OF THE SAND LIZARD, Lacerta agilis, AT THE BEGINNING OF ITS YEARLY ACTIVITY PERIOD Szilárd Nemes 1 Submitted December 27, 2000. At the beginning of its yearly activity period Lacerta agilis behaves as an ambush foraging lizard. Proportion of attacks on prey discovered while lizards are moving (PAM), movement per minute (MPM) and percent of time spent moving (PTM) are low. A correlation was found between MPM and PTM. There are no significant differences between PTM and MPM among sexes or age categories. Prey search locomotion is rarely used and is discontinuous when it occurs. Pause duration has a positive correlation with movement length. Lizards counterbalance long movements with longer pauses in order to increase the probability of prey detection and capture. Contrary to other ambush foraging lizards, Lacerta agilis frequently tongue flicks, probably in order to detect and identify prey animals. Key words: Lacerta agilis, Lacertidae, foraging mode, ambush foraging, tongue flicks, Romania. INTRODUCTION Over 30 years ago, Pianka (1966) described two modes of foraging: ambush foraging and active foraging. Ambush predators are traditionally considered sedentary, waiting for mobile prey and attacking when the prey passes into the perceptual field; active foraging species spend much of their time moving actively while searching for prey (Huey and Pianka, 1981). Foraging mode in lizards has a central role in the understanding of ecological and life history characteristics such as prey types and predators (Huey and Pianka, 1981), energy utilisation (Anderson and Krasnov, 1988), reproduction (Vitt, 1990; Colli et al., 1997), relative clutch mass (Vitt and Congdon, 1978; Vitt and Price, 1982), locomotor capacity (Huey et al., 1984), predator escape modes (Vitt, 1983), learning ability (Day et al., 1999), and chemosensory behaviour (Cooper, 1994, 1995, 1997). Perry (1999), based on precise quantitative information and a phylogenetic autocorrelative test of 83 species in 12 families of lizards, pointed out that 1 Grup Scolar Agricol, Str. Cicului nr. 50, Sf. Gheorghe Ro-4000, Jud. Covasna, Romania. E-mail: nemessz@cii.educv.ro Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Szilárd Nemes, Str. Gábor Áron 28 28 3, Sf. Gheorghe Ro-4000, Jud. Covasna, Romania. there was a family level conservation of the foraging modes of lizards. The family Lacertidae displays the widest spectrum of foraging modes; both foraging modes may even be exploited even within the same genus (Pianka, 1993). In spite of the fact that ambush species such as Pedioplanis lineoocellata, Meroles suborbitalis (Pianka et al., 1979), and Acanthodactylus scutellatus (Perry et al., 1990) exist within the family, the Lecertidae is generally considered to consist chiefly of active foragers. This is because even sit-and-wait lacertids spend a much higher proportion of time moving than do iguanian lizards that use the same foraging mode (Cooper, 1994). Intrafamilial and intaragenic variation in foraging mode is also known in the Gekkonidae (Werner et al., 1997) and Scincidae (Cooper and Whiting, 2000). This paper presents several aspects of the foraging behaviour of the sand lizard, Lacerta agilis, at the beginning of its yearly activity period. Specifically, I investigated: 1) Whether Lacerta agilis actively searches for food as do most other lacertids or is sedentary and waits for mobile prey; 2) Whether there is a difference in foraging mode of Lacerta agilis between sexes or age categories; and 3) Whether Lacerta agilis tongue flicks to detect prey items. 1026-2296 2002 0901-0057 2002 Folium Publishing Company

58 Szilárd Nemes MATERIAL AND METHODS Study Animals Lacerta agilis is a medium sized (up to 90 mm snout-vent length and 220 mm total length, 20 g) oviparous, insectivorous heliothermic lizard that is widely spread over Europe from England in the west to Russia in the east and from Sweden in the north to France in the south. Study Site Sf. Gheorghe is situated in the geographic centre of Romania in the north-eastern part of Brasov depression. The relief is represented by hilly wooded area and plain meadows. The climate is characterised by 7.6 C average annual temperature, with relatively hot summers (18 C) and cold winters ( 4.7 C). Annual rainfall averages about 549 (400 600) mm per year with the driest month (February) averaging approximately 22.4 mm and the wettest month (July) averaging approximately 84.4 mm. When the study was conducted the average daily temperature was 13.4 C the maximum (25.9 C) was reached in the afternoon of 30th of April, the minimum ( 1.6 C) in the night of 3rd of May. The relative humidity of the air was 65.5% ranging between 22 and 100%. Only a low quantity of rainfall was measured (15 mm). This study took place in meadows near Sf. Gheorghe from April 20 through May 7, 2000. The habitat is on a 10 20 slope with southern exposure 80% covered by herbs and grasses with an average height of 15 30 cm as well as by Crataegus monogyna, Rosa canina, Juniperus communis bushes and small Fagus sylvatica (15 50 cm) trees. The study site is surrounded in the east, west and south by Fagus sylvatica forest while in the north, at the base of the slope, it is bordered by a brook, two artificial lakes, and a road. Sampling Methods To examine foraging activity, the habitat was visited daily. Observations on foraging were restricted to sunny days at times when the lizards were active. Lizards were detected by walking slowly through the habitat while scanning the area for activity. When a lizard was detected, the observer remained motionless and waited until convinced that normal activity of the lizard had not been disturbed. The observation of each individual for 10 consecutive minutes was attempted but it was not always possible because lizards sometimes went out of sight. Eleven sand lizards were observed for the full 600 sec and the average observation period of the 25 individuals (10 adult males, 5 adult females, and 10 juveniles) was 442.84 sec. Observations of less than 3 min were disregarded as were those in which lizards had been disturbed by the observer. Care was taken to avoid the observation of the same individual more than once. Sex, age category (juvenile or adult), and observation time of each individual were recorded. Time spent moving and perching, tongueflicking behaviour and feeding attempts (successful and unsuccessful) were noted. In addition feeding attempts were scored as having been initiated after an active search or as a result of an ambush. Tongue flicks were considered associated with movement if they appeared within 1 second after movement cessation. Tongue flicking behaviour was observed only in adults as comparative data are available only for this age class. Postural adjustments, head, limb or tail movements were not recorded. Movements made in order to take up new ambushing positions were recorded, as were searching movements. Data Analysis From the raw data three indices of the foraging activity were calculated: the number of movements per minute (MPM), percentage of time spent moving (PTM) (Pianka et al., 1979) and the percentage of attacks on prey discovered while lizards were moving compared to total attacks (PAM) (Cooper et al., 1999). Owning to the non normal distribution of MPM and PTM the significance level of differences between sexes and age categories were tested by the nonparametric Kruskal Wallis one-way analysis of variance (Précsényi, 1995). Relationship between MPM and PTM, and pause and movement duration were examined using Spearman rank correlation. Mann Whitney U-test was performed to examine differences between the duration of the movement made by the lizard when shifting to other perching site and the duration of active prey search movement made between two pauses. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Based on the statistical measure of MPM and PTM the sand lizard is an ambush predator, both having very low values (MPM = 0.215 ± 0.25; PTM = 1.59 ± 2.41). The third index, PAM also indicates ambush foraging (PAM = 0.00) as all of the prey capture events (n = 4) occurred during perching. These values, low as in other ambush foraging lizards

Sand Lizard Springtime Foraging 59 (Table 1), suggest that L. agilis is sedentary and waits for mobile prey. For lacertids the two indices, MPM and PTM, give the same verdict. For L. agilis there is a significant positive correlation between MPM and PTM (r s = 0.957, p < 0.001) (Fig. 1). A correlation was found between the two indices for four species of Israeli lacertids lizards (Perry et al., 1990). The average values of MPM and PTM differ within sexes or age categories (Table 2) but the difference is not significant (MPM: H = 2.179, df = 2, p > 0.05; PTM: H = 1.709, df =2,p > 0.05). Among lizards there are usually sexual differences in the foraging mode, with males being more active than females (Perry, 1996, quoted by Werner et al., 1997). During mid-april through mid-may there are significant differences in the foraging mode of the two sexes of Cnemidophorus tigris (Anderson, 1993, 1994). The lizards foraging mode can vary within species according to sex, age, place, time, and food availability (Ananjeva and Tesellarius, 1986). There may be differences in the foraging mode of the sand lizard throughout the whole year. The weight of the food, and the number and diversity of invertebrates RTM 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 MPM Fig. 1. Relationship between time spent moving (PTM) and the number of movements per minute (MPM). consumed by Romanian sand lizards increase from April to June, when a maximum is reached. This is followed by slight decreases in July and August, and reaches a minimum in October (Valenciuc et al., 1988). At the beginning of its activity period, the sand lizard attacks 0.021 prey per minute. Cnemidophorus tigris spends about 87% of its time in foraging related TABLE 1. Lizards Foraging Mode Indices Obtained from Literature and from the Present Study Family Species n FM MPM PTM Reference Lacertidae Acanthodactylus boskianus 7 A 2.01 28.8 Perry et al., 1990 Acanthodactylus scutelltus 26 SW 1.01 7.7 Perry et al., 1990 Lacerta laevis 16 A 1.61 30.5 Perry et al., 1990 Lacerta agilis 25 SW 0.21 1.59 Present study Gekkonidae Gekko japonicus 12 SW 0.15 8.94 Werner et al., 1997 Pachydactylus turneri 11 SW 0 0 Cooper et al., 1999 Hemidactylus turcicus SW 0.44 1.91 Perry, 1999 Chamaeleontidae Agama atra 24 SW 0.27 0.01 Cooper et al., 1999 Cordylidae Platysaurus capensis 22 SW 1.27 6.62 Cooper et al., 1997 Note. FM, foraging mode; MPM, movement per minute; PTM, percent of time spent moving; SW, sit-and-wait or ambush foraging; A, active foraging. TABLE 2. Descriptive Statistics for Movement per Minute (MPM) and Percent of Time Spent Moving (PTM) Parameter Mean SD SE Min Max Range CV MPM 0.21 0.25 0.05 0 0.78 0.78 119.6 PTM 1.59 2.41 0.48 0 7.82 7.82 151.2 Males MPM 0.2 0.26 0.08 0 0.789 0.789 127.6 PTM 1.26 2.05 0.64 0 6.57 6.57 161.8 Females MPM 0.07 0.11 0.05 0 0.27 0.27 159.6 PTM 0.4 0.6 0.27 0 1.35 1.35 149.6 Juveniles MPM 0.29 0.28 0.09 0 0.78 0.78 97.7 PTM 2.52 3.05 0.96 0 7.82 7.82 120.9 Note. n = 25, 10 males, 5 females, 10 juveniles; SD, standard deviation; SE, standard error; CV, coefficient of variance.

60 Szilárd Nemes movement (i.e., PTM = 87) and attacks 0.07 prey per minute (Anderson, 1993). Kentropyx calcarata spends 31.4% of its activity time moving and attacks 0.06 prey per minute; Mabuya nigropunctata moves 6.55% of its activity time and attacks 0.03 prey per minute (Vitt et al., 1997). The extreme ambush predator, Corytophanus cristatus feeds only on large arthropods, prey capture being infrequent (perhaps not even daily) (Andrews, 1979). Most of its time, L. agilis perches motionless, usually near bushes or other shelter, occasionally changing to a different perch site in the vicinity of the same shelter. Prey search locomotion is rarely exhibited. The active searching sand lizard moves in a discontinuous fashion alternating bursts of locomotion with short pauses (pause-travel locomotion or saltatory search) as observed at other lacertids (Podarcis muralis, P. pityuensis, Lacerta viridis, and L. trilineata) by Avery et al. (1987). Among sand lizards, pause duration has a positive correlation with the movement length (r s = 0.711, p < 0.05). There can be a trade-off between the length of movement and the success of prey searching. Lizards counterbalance long movements with longer pauses in order to increase the probability of prey detection and capture. The pauses increase the probability that prey at any distance from snout will be eaten compared with a moving lizard (Avery, 1993). There is no significant difference (z = 1.1704, p > 0.05) between the duration of the movement made by the lizard when shifting to other perching site and the duration of active prey search movement made between two pauses (Table 3). Ambush and active foraging lizards differ in sensory modality; ambushers using visual cues while active foragers visual or olfactory cues (Perry and Pianka, 1997). Only 26% of adult Lacerta agilis flicked their tongue to the substrate, performing 29 tongue flicks in 7 cases at a frequency of 0.245 tongue flick per minute. All individuals performed more than one tongue flick in a rapid succession (4.14 ± 2.03). Six times tongue flicking occurred at the end of search movement, once preceding prey capture. Like sand lizards, phrynosomatid lizards restricted tongue flicking of substrates to the end of transitional movements (Cooper et al., 1994). Sand lizards performed considerably more tongue flicks per minute than did phrynosomatid lizards (Table 4). Phrynosomatid lizards are considered ambushers, whereas lacertids are considered active foragers (Cooper, 1994). Families of active foragers use their tongue to detect and identify prey, families of ambush foragers lack prey chemical discrimination (Cooper, 1994). Active foragers take advantage of the trail left by prey animals (Schwenk, 1994) when seeking out prey by moving widely through the environment. Foraging mode (Perry, 1999) and chemoreception (Schwenk, 1993) are strongly influenced by phylogeny and demonstrate a correlated evolution (Cooper, 1997). Families of ambush foragers have lost prey chemical discrimination (Cooper and van Wyk, 1994). Cordylid lizards, members of Scleroglossa, as well as lacertid lizards, are ambush foragers (Cooper et al., 1997) and lack tongue mediated chemosensory behaviour (Cooper and van Wyk, 1994; Cooper and Steele, 1999). Lacerta agilis, also adopt ambush foraging but use tongue mediated chemosensory behaviour. TABLE 3. Descriptive Statistics for Active Search Movement and Shift Movement Length (in seconds) Parameter n Mean SD SE Min Max Range CV Mode Shift 21 3.85 3.11 0.68 1 12 11 80.9 2 Active search 11 4.9 2.07 0.62 2 8 6 42.9 5 Note. SD, standard deviation; SE, standard error; CV, coefficient of variance. TABLE 4. Tongue Flicking Rate at Phrynosmatidae (P) and Lacertidae (L) lizards Species n t, min Em Ot Tf per min PTF, % Reference Sceloporus jarrovi (P) 48 476.07 5 0 0.019 10 Cooper et al., 1994 Sceloporus vigatus (P) 42 415.9 10 1 0.067 24 Cooper et al., 1994 Urosaurus ornatus (P) 34 33.67 7 0 0.033 21 Cooper et al., 1994 Cophosaurus texanus (P) 6 56.62 1 0 0.018 16 Cooper et al., 1994 Lacerta agilis (L) 15 110.16 6 1 0.245 26 Present study Note. t, observation time; Tongue flick appearance: Em, end of movement; Ot, other times; PTF, percent of tongue flicking lizards.

Sand Lizard Springtime Foraging 61 These contradictions between adaptation to ambush foraging and the presence of tongue mediated chemosensory sampling are only apparent because chemoreception in squamates reflect family level features and not adaptation to local conditions by different species (Schwenk, 1993). Tongue mediated chemosensory sampling is a common feature of lacertid lizards, which can be used to detect prey items (Cooper, 1990, 1991) as well as predators (Thoen et al., 1986). Acknowledgements. I am grateful to Kovásznay Csengele for field assistance and for help preparing the manuscript. An anonymous reviewer gives helpful advice. This study was supported by Iskola Foundation. REFERENCES Ananjeva N. B. and Tsellarius Y. A. (1986), On the factors determining desert lizards diet, in: Roèek Z. (ed.), Studies in Herpetology. Proc. Eur. Herpetol. Meeting, Prague, Charles University, Prague, pp. 445 448. Anderson R. A. (1993), Analysis of foraging mode ia a lizard Cnemidophorus tigris: Salient features and environmental effects, in: Wright J. W. and Vitt L. J. (eds.), Biology of Whiptail Lizards (genus Cnemidophorus), Oklahoma Museum of Natural History, Norman (Oklahoma), USA, pp. 83 116. Anderson R. A. (1994), Functional and populational responses of the lizard Cnemidophorus tigris to environmental fluctuations, Am. Zool., 34, 409 421. Anderson R. A. and Karasov W. H. (1988), Energetics of the lizard Cnemidophorus tigris and life history consequences of food-acquisition mode, Ecol. Monogr., 58, 79 110. Andrews R. M. (1979), The lizard Coryphanes critatus: an extreme Sit-and-Wait predator, Biotropica, 11(2), 136 139. Avery R. A. (1993), Experimental analysis of lizard pause-travel movement: pauses increase probability of prey capture, Amphibia Reptilia, 14, 423 427. Avery R. A., Mueller C. F., Jones S. M., Smith J. A., and Bond D. J. (1987), Speed and movement patterns of European lacertid lizards: a comparative study, J. Herpetol., 21(4), 324 329. Colli G. R., Péres A. K. and Zatz M. G. (1997), Foraging mode and reproductive seasonality in tropical lizards, J. Herpetol., 31(4), 490 499. Cooper W. E., Jr. (1990), Prey odor detection by Teiid and Lacertid lizards and the relationship of prey odor detection to foraging mode in lizard families, Copeia, 1990(1), 237 242. Cooper W. E., Jr. (1991), Responses to prey chemicals by a lacertid lizard, Podarcis muralis: prey chemical discrimination and post strike elevation in tongue-flick rate, J. Chem. Ecol., 17, 849 863. Cooper W. E., Jr. (1994), Chemical discrimination by tongue-flicking in lizards: a review with hypotheses on its origin and its ecological and phylogenetic relationships, J. Chem. Ecol., 20, 439 487. Cooper W. E., Jr. (1995), Foraging mode, prey chemical discrimination, and phylogeny in lizards, Anim. Behav., 50, 973 985. Cooper W. E., Jr. (1997), Correlated evolution of prey chemical discrimination with foraging, lingual morphology and vomeronasal chemoreceptor abundance in lizards, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 41, 257 265. Cooper W. E., Jr., Vitt L. J., and Caldwell J. P. (1994), Movement and substrate tongue flicks in Phrynosomatid lizards, Copeia, 1994(1), 234 237. Cooper W. E., Jr., Whiting M. J., and van Wyk J. H. (1997), Foraging mode of cordyliform lizards, South Africa J. Zool., 32, 9 13 Cooper W. E., Jr., and Steele L. J. (1999), Lingually mediated discrimination among prey chemicals and control stimuli in cordyliformlizards: presence in a gerrhosaurid and absence in two cordylids, Herpetologica, 55, 361 368. Cooper W. E., Jr., Whiting M. J., van Wyk J. H., and Mouton P. l. e F. N. (1999), Movement and attack based indices of foraging mode and ambush foraging in some gekkonid and agamine lizards from southern Africa, Amphibia Reptilia, 20, 391 399. Day L. B., Crews D., and Wilczynski W. (1999), Spatial and reversal learning in congeneric lizards with different foraging strategies, Anim. Behav., 57, 393 407. Huey R. B. and Pianka E. R. (1981), Ecological consequences of foraging mode, Ecology, 62, 991 999. Huey R. B., Bennett A. F., John-Adler H. and Nagy K. A. (1984), Locomotor capacity and foraging behaviour of Kalahari Lacertid lizards, Anim. Behav., 32, 41 50. Perry G. (1999), The evolution of search modes: ecological versus phylogenetic perspectives, Am. Nat., 153, 98 109. Perry G. and Pianka E. R. (1997), Animal foraging past present and future, Tree, 12, 360 364. Perry G., Lampl I., Lerner A., Rothenstein D., Shani E., Sivan N., and Werner Y. L. (1990), Foraging mode in lacertid lizards: variation and correlates, Amphibia Reptilia, 11, 373 384. Pianka E. R. (1966), Convexity, desert lizards, and spatial heterogenity, Ecology, 47, 360 364. Pianka E. R. (1993), The many dimensions of a lizard s ecological niche, in: Valakos, Böhme, Pérez-Mellado, and Maragou (eds.), Lacertids of the Mediterranean region, Hellenic Zool. Soc., pp. 121 154 Pianka E. R., Huey R. B. and Lawlor L. L. (1979), Niche segregation in desert lizards, in: Horn J. D., Mitchell, and Stains G. R. (eds.), Analysis of Ecological

62 Szilárd Nemes Systems, Ohio State University Press, Columbus, pp. 67 115. Précsény I. (eds.) (1995), Alapvetõ kutatástervezési statisztikai és projectrtekelsi módszerek a szupraindividualis biológiában, KLTE Evolucios, Állattani és Humánbiológia Tanszék, Debrecen [in Romanian]. Schwenk K. (1993), The evolution of chemoreception in squamate reptiles: A phylogenetic approach, Brain Behav. Evol., 41, 124 137. Schwenk K. (1994), Why snakes have forked tongues, Science, 263, 1573 1577. Thoen C., Bauwens D., and Verheyen R. F. (1986), Chemoreceptive and behavioural responses of the common lizard Lacerta vivipara to snake chemical deposits, Anim. Behav., 34, 1805 1813. Valenciuc N., Varvara M., and Brînzã V. (1988), Researces concerning the food and the intensity of feeding in Lacerta agilis agilis L. 175, in: Analele Stiintifice ale Universitãþii Al. I. Cuza din Iasi. Tomul XXXIV. Sec. IIa. Biologie, pp. 71 76 [in Romanian]. Vitt L. J. (1983), Tail loss in lizards: the significance of foraging and predator escape modes, Herpetologica, 39, 151 162. Vitt L. J. (1990), The influence of foraging mode and phylogeny on seasonality of tropical lizards reproduction, Papéis Avulsos Zool., 37(6), 107 123. Vitt L. J. and Caldwell J. P. (1978), Body shape, reproductive effort, and relative clutch mass in lizards: resolution of a paradox, Am. Nat., 112, 595 608. Vitt L. J. and Price H. J. (1982), Ecological and evolutionary determinants of relative clutch mass in lizards, Herpetologica, 38, 237 255. Vitt L. J., Zani P. A. and Narinho-Lima C. (1997), Helioterms in tropical rain forest: the ecology of Kenotropyx calcarata (Teiidae) and Mabuya nigropunctata (Scincidae) in the Curuá-Una of Brazil, J. Trop. Ecol., 13, 199 220. Werner Y. L., Okada S., Ota H., Perry G., and Tokunaga S. (1997), Varied and fluctuating foraging modes in nocturnal lizards of the family Gekkonidae, Asiatic Herpetol. Res., 7, 153 165. Werner Y. L., Bouskila A., Davies S. J. J. F., and Werner N. (1997), Observations and comments on active foraging in geckos, Russ. J. Herpetol., 4(1), 34 39.