Ultraviolet reflectance of male nuptial colouration in sand lizards (Lacerta agilis) from the Pyrenees

Similar documents
MA41 Colour variability and the ecological use of colour in the chameleons and geckos of Mahamavo

Ultraviolet reflectance and cryptic sexual dichromatism in the ocellated lizard, Lacerta (Timon) lepida (Squamata: Lacertidae)bij_

Using visual modelling to study the evolution of lizard coloration: sexual selection drives the evolution of sexual dichromatism in lacertids

Predation Cost of Conspicuous Male Coloration in Collared Lizards (Crotaphytus collaris): An Experimental Test Using Clay-Covered Model Lizards

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour

Local adaptation and divergence in colour signal conspicuousness between monomorphic and polymorphic lineages in a lizard

Sexual Dichromatism and Color Conspicuousness in Three Populations of Collared Lizards (Crotaphytus collaris) from Oklahoma 1

DOES EXPERIMENTALLY INDUCED CONSPICUOUS COLORATION INCREASE RISK OF PREDATION AND CONSPECIFIC AGGRESSION IN FIRST-YEAR COLLARED LIZARD MALES?

Plestiodon (=Eumeces) fasciatus Family Scincidae

J.K. McCoy CURRICULUM VITAE. J. Kelly McCoy. Department of Biology Angelo State University San Angelo, TX

Pheromone-Mediated Intrasexual Aggression in Male Lizards, Podarcis hispanicus

Differences in Visual Signal Design and Detectability between Allopatric Populations of Anolis Lizards

No Evidence for Female Association with High-Performance males in the Green Anole Lizard, Anolis carolinensis

Faculty Mentor, Department of Integrative Biology, Oklahoma State University

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Elk (Cervus canadensis) Extra credit assignment. Sad Underwing (Catocala maestosa) 10/11/2017

Cryptic sexual dichromatism occurs across multiple types of plumage in the Green-backed Tit Parus monticolus

Individual quality and nuptial throat colour in male European green lizards

Reproductive activity of Lacerta agilis and Zootoca vivipara (Reptilia: Sauria: Lacertidae) in western Siberia

Contributions of pterin and carotenoid pigments to dewlap coloration in two anole species

A case of achromatopsia. Perceptual Colour Space. Spectral Properties of Light. Subtractive Colour Mixture. Additive Colour Mixture

Perception & Attention Course. George Mather

7 CONGRESSO NAZIONALE

Color Vision by Prof/Faten zakareia King Saud University Physiology Dept

Blue, not UV, plumage color is important in satin bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus violaceus display

Appendix from T. J. Ord and J. A. Stamps, Species Identity Cues in Animal Communication

CAROTENOID-BASED DEWLAP COLOR AS A VISUAL SIGNAL IN SOCIAL. COMMUNICATION OF BROWN ANOLES (Norops sagrei) John Edward Steffen

An experimental test of female choice relative to male structural coloration in eastern bluebirds

Why do Anolis dewlaps glow? An analysis of a translucent visual signal

Temperature-dependent colour change is a function of sex and directionality of temperature shift in the eastern fence lizard (Sceloporus undulatus)

NATURAL AND SEXUAL VARIATION

LIZARD EVOLUTION VIRTUAL LAB

Within-Male Melanin-Based Plumage and Bill Elaboration in Male House Sparrows

The Evolution of Signal Design in Manakin Plumage Ornaments

Variable visual habitats may influence the spread of colourful plumage across an avian hybrid zone

Sexy smells Featured scientist: Danielle Whittaker from Michigan State University

Habitat light, colour variation, and ultraviolet reflectance in the Grand Cayman anole, Anolis compersus

Societas Europaea Herpetologica

Discrete colour polymorphism in the tawny dragon lizard (Ctenophorus decresii) and differences in signal conspicuousness among morphs

Patterns of shape and size sexual dimorphism in a population of Podarcis hispanica* (Reptilia: Lacertidae) from NE Iberia

Seasonal Shifts in Reproductive Investment of Female Northern Grass Lizards ( Takydromus septentrionalis

Structural and melanin coloration indicate parental effort and reproductive success in male eastern bluebirds

Preening, plumage reflectance and female choice in budgerigars

Blue structural coloration of male eastern bluebirds Sialia sialis predicts incubation provisioning to females

Sexual selection based on egg colour: physiological models and egg discrimination experiments in a cavity-nesting bird

2016 Artikel. article Online veröffentlicht / published online: Autor / Author:

Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds

Chapter 16: Evolution Lizard Evolution Virtual Lab Honors Biology. Name: Block: Introduction

P.O. Box 671, Wilderness 6560, South Africa. Anhalt University of Applied Sciences, Dept. 1, Strenzfelder Allee 28, Bernburg, Germany

Visual and Instrumental Evaluation of Mottling and Striping

Morph-specific immunity in male Podarcis muralis

Analysis of Sampling Technique Used to Investigate Matching of Dorsal Coloration of Pacific Tree Frogs Hyla regilla with Substrate Color

Habitat light, colour variation, and ultraviolet reflectance in the Grand Cayman anole, Anolis conspersus

Sexual Dichromatism in the Northern Map Turtle, Graptemys geographica CONSTANCE M. O CONNOR 1,4, AND GABRIEL BLOUIN-DEMERS 3

Ornament size and colour as alternative strategies for effective communication in gliding lizards

UV coloration influences spatial dominance but not agonistic behaviors in male wall lizards

Sexual dimorphism in conspicuousness and ornamentation in the enigmatic leaf-nosed lizard Ceratophora tennentii from Sri Lanka

First record of a melanistic Italian Wall Lizard (Podarcis sicula) in Slovenia

Notes on Varanus salvator marmoratus on Polillo Island, Philippines. Daniel Bennett.

Plumage coloration can be perceived as a multiple condition-dependent signal by Great Tits Parus major

Sex differences in antipredator tail-waving displays of the diurnal yellow-headed gecko Gonatodes albogularis from tropical forests of Colombia

REPTILES OF JAMAICA. Peter Vogel Department of Life Sciences Mona Campus University of the West Indies

2015 Artikel. article Online veröffentlicht / published online: Deichsel, G., U. Schulte and J. Beninde

Variation in body temperatures of the Common Chameleon Chamaeleo chamaeleon (Linnaeus, 1758) and the African Chameleon Chamaeleo africanus

Carotenoid-based plumage coloration and aggression during molt in male house finches

Class Reptilia Testudines Squamata Crocodilia Sphenodontia

Achromatic plumage reflectance predicts reproductive success in male black-capped chickadees

Juvenile Collared Lizards Adjust Tail Display Frequency in Response to Variable Predatory Threat

Chemosensory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by lizards: implications of field spatial relationships between males

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR. Laboratory: a Manual to Accompany Biology. Saunders College Publishing: Philadelphia.

Seasonality provokes a shift of thermal preferences in a temperate lizard, but altitude does not

ECOL /8/2019. Why do birds have colorful plumage? Today s Outline. Evolution of Animal Form & Function. 1. Functions of Colorful Plumage

Everyday Mysteries: Why most male birds are more colorful than females

Plumage and its Function in birds

Geographic Color Variation and Physiological Color Change in Eastern Collared Lizards (Crotaphytus collaris) from Southern New Mexico, USA

Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution

Sex-based hatching asynchrony in an oviparous lizard (Bassiana duperreyi, Scincidae)

What Birds. Evolution has endowed birds with a system of color vision that surpasses that of all mammals, including humans

Does dewlap size predict male bite performance in. Jamaican Anolis lizards? B. VANHOOYDONCK,* A. Y. HERREL,* R. VAN DAMME and D. J.

Reproductive success and symmetry in zebra finches

A New Species of Agama (Sauria: Agamidae)

1 This question is about the evolution, genetics, behaviour and physiology of cats.

Effects of nestling condition on UV plumage traits in blue tits: an experimental approach

Evidence for the signaling function of egg color in the pied flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca

Is it better to be bigger? Featured scientists: Aaron Reedy and Robert Cox from the University of Virginia Co-written by Matt Kustra

Manipulation of parasite load induces significant changes in the structural-based throat color of male iberian green lizards

6. The lifetime Darwinian fitness of one organism is greater than that of another organism if: A. it lives longer than the other B. it is able to outc

IS THE ULTRAVIOLET WAVEBAND A SPECIAL COMMUNICATION CHANNEL IN AVIAN MATE CHOICE?

Supplementary Materials for

Effect of feather abrasion on structural coloration in male eastern bluebirds Sialia sialis

The Brain and Senses. Birds perceive the world differently than humans. Avian intelligence. Novel feeding behaviors

Multiple paternity in clutches of common lizard Lacerta vivipara: data from microsatellite markers

Primate photopigments and primate color vision (opsin genes polymorphism cones evolution)

PERSISTENCE OF EGG RECOGNITION IN THE ABSENCE OF CUCKOO BROOD PARASITISM: PATTERN AND MECHANISM

The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis)

Migration. Migration = a form of dispersal which involves movement away from and subsequent return to the same location, typically on an annual basis.

What determines dewlap diversity in Anolis lizards? An among-island comparison

SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN BODY SHAPE WITHOUT SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN BODY SIZE IN WATER SKINKS (EULAMPRUS QUOYII)

Cnemidophorus lemniscatus (Rainbow Whiptail)

Reptile Identification Guide

Preferred temperatures of Podarcis vaucheri from Morocco: intraspecific variation and interspecific comparisons

Transcription:

Ultraviolet reflectance of male nuptial colouration in sand lizards (Lacerta agilis) from the Pyrenees Guillem Pérez i de Lanuza, Enrique Font Abstract. The evolutionary significance of lacertid colourations is relatively unexplored. However, several studies have demonstrated signalling by means of bright green colouration in Swedish Lacerta agilis males during the breeding season. Unfortunately, most of these studies have been based on human colour perception that differs in several ways from that of lizard. An important difference between human and lizard colour vision is the presence of an ultraviolet (UV) sensitive cone in lizards. The available evidence suggests that male sand lizards colorations do not reflect UV wavelengths, at least in Swedish populations. However, this study, based on objective (spectrophotometric) measures of Pyrenean L. agilis, revealed a secondary reflectance peak in the UV in male green colouration. This secondary peak increases sexual dichromatism and male conspicuousness. Moreover, it could have a social signalling function as do similar UV reflecting patches in other lizard species. Lacerta agilis (Linnaeus, 1758) has one of the largest distribution ranges of all lacertids, extending from Lake Baikal to the Pyrenees and from S Sweden to N Greece (Bischoff, 1988). It is also one of the best studied lacertid species and there is an extensive body of literature on its morphology, physiology, ecology, and behaviour (i.e. approximately 150 papers are cited in Bischoff, 1984). Sand lizards are sexually dichromatic throughout most of their distribution range (Arnold and Ovenden, 2002): during the breeding season females are uniformly grayish-brown whereas males are brownish mid-dorsally and display patches of bright green colouration ventrally and laterally. During the mating season males fight for access to mates. After copulation, males show mate guarding behaviour and reject other males. During contests, males approach rivals with a dorso-ventrally flattened body, displaying lateral green nuptial colouration (Kitzler, 1941; Olsson, 1992, 1994a; Weyrauch, 2005). Institut Cavanilles de Biodiversitat i Biologia Evolutiva, Universitat de València, APT. 22085, 46071, València, Spain Corresponding author; e-mail: guillem.perez-lanuza@uv.es Pivotal work by Olsson and colleagues with sand lizards from a population in the Swedish west coast has shown that the size and/or brightness of the green patches can act as an honest signal of fighting ability (Olsson, 1994a; Olsson and Silverin, 1997) or as a cue to male genetic quality (Olsson et al., 2005a, b). The area covered by the green patches increases allometrically with body size and its pigment saturation (i.e. chroma ) correlates both with body mass and with an index of body condition (Olsson, 1993). Males with artificially enlarged lateral green patches are more likely to win staged encounters in the laboratory (Olsson, 1994b) and have higher mating success in the field (Anderholm et al., 2004). The size of a male s lateral green patches is negatively correlated with male mate guarding duration (Olsson et al., 2000), and males with more diverse MHC genotype have more and brighter nuptial colouration (Olsson et al., 2005a). It seems that females are attracted to males with MHC genotype different from their own (Olsson et al., 2003) and respond to colour-enhanced males by adjusting maternal effort and the sex ratio of their offspring (Olsson et al., 2005b). Unfortunately, most of the above studies have been conducted using methods and techniques optimized for human vision (e.g. Munsell s colour charts to calculate chroma, stan- Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2007. Amphibia-Reptilia 28 (2007): 438-443 Also available online - www.brill.nl/amre

Short Note 439 dard colour photography, dyes mixed to match by eye the lizards natural colouration, etc.) that do not take into account potentially important differences between our visual perception and that of other animals. Many lizards, for example, are capable of seeing into the ultraviolet (UV) range of wavelengths and display elaborate UV reflectance patterns that appear to be used for intraspecific signalling (Fleishman et al., 1993; LeBas and Marshall, 2000; Macedonia et al., 2000, 2003; Blomberg et al., 2001; Fleishman and Persons, 2001; Macedonia, 2001; Stoehr and McGraw, 2001; Thorpe and Richard, 2001; Thorpe, 2002; Thorpe and Stenson, 2003; Font and Molina-Borja, 2004; Molina-Borja et al., 2006; Stapley and Whiting, 2006; Whiting et al., 2006). Only two recent studies have relied on objective techniques (i.e. reflectance spectrophotometry) to asses the colouration of sand lizards or of paints used to manipulate their colour patches (Anderholm et al., 2004; Olsson et al., 2005b). Both studies cite unpublished spectrophotometry data showing that Swedish sand lizards lack reflectance in the ultraviolet spectrum (Olsson and Andersson, unpubl. data). Here we use reflectance spectrophotometry to show that, in contrast to the Swedish lizards, the green patches of males from a sand lizard population in the Pyrenees exhibit a distinct reflectance peak in the UV. Spectral data were gathered for six adult males (mean SVL ± SD = 76 ± 2.04 mm) and one female (SVL = 74 mm). All lizards were captured with a pole and noose in May 2005 in Serra del Cadí, eastern Pyrenees, 42 20 45 N, 1 54 55 E (for a description of the population and study site see Amat et al., 2000, 2003). Lizards were held in cloth bags after data collection, and all were released unharmed within 24 h at their location of capture. Experimental techniques and protocols have been described in detail elsewhere (Font and Molina-Borja, 2004; Molina-Borja et al., 2006). We therefore only present a summary here. We collected reflectance spectra using an Ocean Optics USB2000 portable diode-array spectrometer and a PX-2 xenon strobe light source, both from Ocean Optics B.V. (Duiven, The Netherlands). Spectra were recorded in 0.37-nm steps from 200 to 850 nm and expressed as per cent of light reflected relative to a Spectralon white diffuse reflectance standard. Measurements were taken with a reflectance probe (R200-7, Ocean Optics) held at a 90 angle to and 5 mm from the lizard s skin. A dark current and white standard reference spectrum were taken every 10 min during measurement of lizard colour patches. Spectra were obtained from the following body parts: head (dorsal, anterior to parietal eye), gular area (centre), lateral patches, dorsum (midpoint along the vertebral column), ventrum (centre), leg (outer, upper thigh), and tail (dorsal, slightly distal to base). In addition, we obtained spectra of samples of natural backgrounds that are present in L. agilis habitat: green vegetation and soil. For each measurement the spectrometer averaged 20 spectra that were graphed using OOIBase32 software from Ocean Optics. Integration time was set at 55 ms using data-smoothing level 10. All measurements were taken in a darkened room to minimize interference from external light sources. The mid-dorsal stripe of male sand lizards produces, as does most of the body surface of females, a relatively flat spectrum with negligible reflectance below 400 nm (fig. 1). In contrast, the lateral and ventral surfaces of males show dual reflectance peaks in the green (primary peak) and UV (secondary peak) regions of the spectrum. The primary peak, responsible for the green hue perceived by humans, has the maximum of reflectance near 530 nm, while the UV peak has its maximal reflectance around 350 nm (fig. 1). The green peak is brighter than the UV peak. The ventral surface is brighter than scales of the lateral patches. In the belly, reflectance of the green peak is approximately 58%, while the UV peak reflects 36 % of incident light. On the lateral patches, green peak reflection is 33% and UV peak reflection is 15%. Our results show that the nuptial colouration of male sand lizards from the Pyrenees is highly UV-reflective. Other lacertids, like Gallotia (Thorpe and Richard, 2001; Font and Molina-Borja, 2004; Molina-Borja et al., 2006) and several species of Podarcis (unpubl. data) have colour patches that reflect ultraviolet wavelengths. In fact, the green patches of male sand lizards produce spectra very similar to those recorded from the ventrolateral green ocelli (eyespots) of Gallotia atlantica from Fuerteventura (Canary Islands), which also have a secondary peak at around 360 nm (see Fig. 5D in Font and Molina-Borja, 2004). Colour patches with multiple reflectance peaks, including a secondary UV peak that is confined to or extends into the UV region have also been reported in the dewlaps of Anolis lizards (Macedonia, 1999,

440 Short Note Figure 1. Reflectance spectra of lateral patches (a) and dorsum (b) of L. agilis and natural backgrounds (a, b). Vertical lines = error bars (±1 SEM). 2001; Fleishman and Persons, 2001; Leal and Fleishman, 2002; Stoehr and McGraw, 2001). Chromatic cues may support sex and species discrimination, and male colouration is involved in sexual and agonistic interactions (reviewed in Cooper and Greenberg, 1992). A possible role in social signalling has also been suggested for the UV-reflecting colour patches found in many lizards (e.g. Molina-Borja et al., 2006; Stapley and Whiting, 2006; Whiting et al., 2006). UV reflectance makes male sand lizards more conspicuous to receivers with an ability to see UV because natural visual backgrounds absorb wavelengths below 400 nm (fig. 1). Thus, UV

Short Note 441 reflectance may act as an amplifier (Hasson, 1989) enhancing the visual perception of the size of the nuptial colour patches (Cummings et al., 2006; Lappin et al., 2006). The fact that the UV reflecting patches are located laterally and ventrally, and thus remain relatively concealed until revealed by display postures (e.g. through sagittal compression), is also consistent with a signalling function (Olsson, 1992). However, the presence of UV-reflecting skin patches does not prove that sand lizards can see into the UV part of the spectrum or that UV colour patterns play a role in sand lizard social interactions. Swiezawska (1949) studied colour discrimination within the human-visible spectrum and found that sand lizards are most sensitive in the yellow-green region of the spectrum, which roughly corresponds to the body colouration of males, but no study to date has examined the ability of sand lizards to see into the UV. Nevertheless, the green patches of male sand lizards reflect very close to the absorption wavelength peak of UV receptors in lizards with known UV vision (Fleishman et al., 1993; Loew, 1994; Ellingson et al., 1995; Loew et al., 1996, 2002), and preliminary results using elecroretinography suggest that lacertids have a broad spectral sensitivity extending from UV (350 nm) to deep red (700 nm) (Palacios, Herrera, Desfilis and Font, unpublished data). In contrast with the present study, previous studies of sand lizards from Sweden reported no UV reflectance of the male nuptial coloration. Reasons for the discrepant results of these studies are unclear. One obvious possibility is that the lack of UV reflectance in Swedish populations (or its presence in Pyrenean sand lizards) is the result of geographic colour variation. In fact, Swedish and Pyrenean populations are in opposite north-south ends of the distribution range of sand lizards. Moreover, Swedish populations are relict from the period of warmth that followed the last glacial episode (Gullberg et al., 1999), whereas Pyrenean sand lizard populations may be the result of a recent colonization (Llorente et al., 1997). Geographic variation in colour pattern is widespread in lizards (e.g. Baird et al., 1997; Kwiatkowski and Sullivan, 2002; Macedonia, 2001; Carretero, 2002; Thorpe, 2002; Thorpe and Stenson, 2003; Macedonia et al., 2003, 2004; Stuart-Fox et al., 2004; Rosenblum, 2005). However, there are few reports of geographic variation in UV colouration. Previously we found geographic variation in the UV reflectance of blue lateral patches of Gallotia galloti lizards from Tenerife (Font and Molina-Borja, 2004; Molina-Borja et al., 2006). Although spectrograms of Swedish sand lizards have not been published, the contrasting colouration of Swedish and Pyrenean populations of sand lizards represents, to our knowledge, the most extreme geographic UV colour variation known in any vertebrate. The presence of UV reflectance in the nuptial patches of male sand lizards has strong implications for future studies of the evolution of conspicuous lizard colouration. It also reinforces the need to assess geographic variation in signalling systems, including cases in which the variation has components beyond direct human perception (i.e. below the 400 nm limit of human colour vision). As stressed several times recently (e.g. Cuthill and Bennett, 1993; Bennett et al., 1994; Thorpe, 2002), descriptions of colours or colour patterns that do not take into account the animals perception may result in an incomplete assessment of visual signals and risk misinterpretation. Therefore, the potential visual sensitivity to UV wavelengths should be taken into account in future behavioural studies of UV-reflective lizards. Such elaborate UV colouration as has been found in sand lizards and in other lacertids suggests that lizards may use more complex visual signals than previously thought. Acknowledgements. We are grateful to F. Amat for providing information about Pyrenean populations of sand lizards. This study was supported by projects GV05/138 from Conselleria d Empresa, Universitat i Ciència and CGL2006-03843 from the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia. Thomas Madsen and anonymous referees provided critical comments on the submitted draft.

442 Short Note References Amat, F., Llorente, G.A., Carretero, M.A. (2000): Reproductive cycle of the sand lizard (Lacerta agilis) in its southwestern range. Amphibia-Reptilia 21: 463-476. Amat, F., Llorente, G.A., Carretero, M.A. (2003): A preliminary study on thermal ecology, activity times and microhabitat use of Lacerta agilis (Squamata: Lacertidae) in the Pyrenees. Folia Zool. 52: 413-422. Anderholm, S., Olsson, M., Wapstra, E., Ryberg, K. (2004): Fit and fat from enlarged badges: a field experiment on male sand lizards. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, Biology Letters supplement 271: S142-144. Arnold, N., Ovenden, D. (2002): Field Guide to the Reptiles and Amphibians of Britain and Europe, 2nd Edition. London, Collins. Baird, T.A., Fox, S.F., McCoy, J.K. (1997): Population differences in the roles of size and coloration in intra- and intersexual selection in the collared lizard, Crotaphytus collaris: influence of habitat and social organization. Behav. Ecol. 8: 506-517. Bennett, A.T.D., Cuthill, I.C., Norris, K. (1994): Sexual selection and the mismeasure of color. Amer. Natur. 144: 848-860. Bischoff, W. (1984): Lacerta agilis Linnaeus, 1758 Zauneidechse. In: Handbuch der Reptilien und Amphibien Europas 2, p. 23-68. Böhme, W., Ed., Weisbaden, Aula- Verlag. Bischoff, W. (1988): Zur Verbreitung und Systematik der Zauneidechse, Lacerta agilis Linnaeus, 1758. Mertensiella 1: 11-30. Blomberg, S.P., Owens, I.P.F., Stuart-Fox, D. (2001): Ultraviolet reflectance in the small skink Carlia pectoralis. Herp. Review 32: 16-17. Carretero, M.A. (2002): Sources of colour pattern variation in Mediterranean Psammodromus algirus. Neth. J. Zool. 52: 43-60. Cooper, W.E., Greenberg, N. (1992): Reptilian coloration and behavior. In: Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 18, Physiology E, Hormones, Brain and Behavior, p. 298-422. Gans, C., Crews, D., Eds, Chicago, University of Chicago Press. Cummings, M.E., García De León, F.J., Mollaghan, D.M., Ryan, M.J. (2006): Is UV ornamentation an amplifier in swordtails? Zebrafish 3: 91-100. Cuthill, I.C., Bennett, A.T.D. (1993): Mimicry and the eye of the beholder. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 253: 203-204. Ellingson, J.M., Fleishman, L.J., Loew, E.R. (1995): Visual pigments and spectral sensitivity of the diurnal gecko Gonatodes albogularis. J. Comp. Physiol. A 177: 559-567. Fleishman, L.J., Persons, M. (2001): The influence of stimulus and background colour on signal visibility in the lizard Anolis cristatellus. J. Exp. Biol. 204: 1559-1575. Fleishman, L.J., Loew, E.R., Leal, M. (1993): Ultraviolet vision in lizards. Nature 365: 397. Font, E., Molina-Borja, M. (2004): Ultraviolet reflectance of color patches in Gallotia galloti lizards from Tenerife, Canary Islands. In: The biology of the lacertid lizards. Evolutionary and ecological perspectives, p. 201-223. Pérez-Mellado, V., Riera, N., Perera, A., Eds, Maó, Institut Menorquí d Estudis. Gullberg, A., Olsson, M., Tegelström, H. (1999): Evolution in populations of Swedish sand lizards: genetic differentiation and loss of variability revealed by multilocus DNA fingerprinting. J. Evol. Biol. 12: 17-26. Hasson, O. (1989): Amplifiers and the handicap principle in sexual selection: a different emphasis. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 235: 383-406. Kitzler, G. (1941): Die Paarungsbiologie einiger Eidechsen. Z. Tierpsychol. 4: 335-402. Kwiatkowski, M.A., Sullivan, B.K. (2002): Geographic variation in sexual selection among populations of an iguanid lizard, Sauromalus obesus (= ater). Evolution 56: 2039-2051. Lappin, A.K., Brandt, Y., Husak, J.F., Macedonia, J.M., Kemp, D.J. (2006): Gaping displays reveal and amplify a mechanically based index of weapon performance. Amer. Natur. 168: 100-113. Leal, M., Fleishman, L.J. (2002): Evidence for habitat partitioning based on adaptation to environmental light in a pair of sympatric lizard species. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269: 351-359. LeBas, N.R., Marshall, N.J. (2000): The role of colour in signalling and male choice in the agamid lizard Ctenophorus ornatus. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267: 445-452. Llorente, G.A., Santos, X., Carretero, M.A., Montori, A. (1997): Lacerta agilis (Linné, 1758). Lagarto ágil. In: Distribución y biogeografía de los anfibios y reptiles en España y Portugal. Monografías de Herpetología 3, p. 211-212. Pleguezuelos, J.M., Ed., Universidad de Granada-Asociación Herpetológica Española. Loew, E.R. (1994): A third, ultraviolet-sensitive, visual pigment in the Tokay gecko (Gekko gekko). Vision Res. 34: 1427-1431. Loew, E.R., Govardovskii, V.I., Rohlich, P., Szel, A. (1996): Microspectrophotometric and immunocytochemical identification of ultraviolet photoreceptors in geckos. Visual Neurosci. 13: 247-256. Loew, E.R., Fleishman, L.J., Foster, R.G., Provencio, I. (2002): Visual pigments and oil droplets in diurnal lizards: a comparative study of Caribbean anoles. J. Exp. Biol. 205: 927-938. Macedonia, J.M. (1999): Color signal evolution in an ancestor-descendant species pair of Caribbean anoles. Anolis Newsletter V: 67-80. Macedonia, J.M. (2001): Habitat light, colour variation, and ultraviolet reflectance in the Grand Cayman anole, Anolis conspersus. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 73: 299-320. Macedonia, J.M., James, S., Wittle, L.W., Clark, D.L. (2000): Skin pigments and coloration in the Jamaican radiation of Anolis lizards. J. Herpetol. 34: 99-109. Macedonia, J.M., Echternacht, A.C., Walguarnery, J.W. (2003): Color variation, habitat light, and background contrast in Anolis carolinensis along a geographical transect in Florida. J. Herpetol. 37: 467-478. Macedonia, J.M., Husak, J.F., Brandt, Y.M., Lappin, A.K., Baird, T.A. (2004): Sexual dichromatism and color conspicuousness in three population of collared lizards (Crotaphytus collaris) from Oklahoma. J. Herpetol. 3: 340-354.

Short Note 443 Molina-Borja, M., Font, E., Mesa-Ávila, G. (2006): Sex and population variation in ultraviolet reflectance of colour patches in two Lacertids from Tenerife (Canary Islands). J. Zool. 268: 193-206. Olsson, M. (1992): Contest success in relation to size and residency in male sand lizards Lacerta agilis. Anim. Behav. 44: 386-388. Olsson, M. (1993): Nuptial coloration and predation risk in model sand lizards, Lacerta agilis. Anim. Behav. 46: 410-412. Olsson, M. (1994a): Rival recognition affects male contest behavior in sand lizards (Lacerta agilis). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 35: 249-252. Olsson, M. (1994b): Nuptial coloration in the sand lizard, Lacerta agilis An intra-sexually selected cue to fighting ability. Anim. Behav. 48: 607-613. Olsson, M., Silverin, B. (1997): Effects of growth rate on variation in breeding coloration in male sand lizards (Lacerta agilis, Sauria). Copeia 1997: 456-460. Olsson, M., Wapstra, E., Madsen, T., Silverin, B. (2000): Testosterone, ticks, and travels: a test of the immunocompetence-handicap hypothesis in freeranging lizards. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267: 2339-2343. Olsson, M., Madsen, T., Nordby, J., Wapstra, E., Ujvari, B., Wittsell, H. (2004): MHC and mate choice in sand lizards. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, Biology Letters supplement 270: S254-256. Olsson, M., Madsen, T., Wapstra, E., Silverin, B., Ujvari, B., Wittzell, H. (2005a): MHC, health, color, and reproductive success in sand lizards. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 58: 289-294. Olsson, M., Wapstra, E., Uller, T. (2005b): Differential sex allocation in sand lizards: bright males induce daughter production in a species with heteromorphic sex chromosomes. Biol. Lett. 1: 378-380. Rosenblum, E.B. (2005). The role of phenotypic plasticity in color variation of Tularosa Basin lizards. Copeia 2005: 586-596. Stapley, J., Whiting, M.J. (2006): Ultraviolet signals fighting ability in a lizard. Biol. Lett. 2: 169-172. Stoehr, A.M., McGraw, K.J. (2001): Ultraviolet reflectance of color patches in male Sceloporus undulatus and Anolis carolinensis. J. Herpetol. 35: 168-171. Stuart-Fox, D.M., Moussalli, A., Johnston, G., Owens, I.P.F. (2004): Evolution of color variation in dragon lizards: quantitative tests of the role of crypsis and local adaptation. Evolution 58: 1549-1559. Swiezawska, K. (1949): Colour discrimination of the sand lizard, Lacerta agilis. L. Bull. Int. Acad. Pol. Sci. Lett. Ser. B 569: 1-20. Thorpe, R.S. (2002): Analysis of color spectra in comparative evolutionary studies: molecular phylogeny and habitat adaptation in the St. Vincent anole (Anolis trinitatis). Sys. Biol. 51: 554-569. Thorpe, R.S., Richard, M. (2001): Evidence that ultraviolet markings are associated with patterns of molecular gene flow. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 98: 3929-3934. Thorpe, R.S., Stenson, G. (2003): Phylogeny, paraphyly and ecological adaptation of the colour and pattern in the Anolis roquet complex on Martinique. Mol. Ecol. 12: 117-132. Weyrauch, G. (2005): Verhalten der Zauneidechse. Kampf, Paarung, Kommunikation. Supplement der Zeitschrift für Feldherpetologie, 8. Bielefeld, Laurenti-Verlag. Whiting, M.J., Stuart-Fox, D.M., O Connor, D., Firth, D., Bennett, N.C., Blomberg, S.P. (2006): Ultraviolet signals ultra-aggression in a lizard. Anim. Bebav. 72: 353-363. Received: October 13, 2006. Accepted: January 02, 2007.