ethology international journal of behavioural biology

Similar documents
Tail Autotomy Does Not Increase Locomotor Costs in the Oriental Leaf-toed Gecko Hemidactylus bowringii

Effect of Tail Loss on Sprint Speed and Growth in Newborn Skinks, Niveoscincus metallicus

Jettisoning Ballast or Fuel? Caudal Autotomy and Locomotory Energetics of the Cape Dwarf Gecko Lygodactylus capensis (Gekkonidae)

Seasonal Shifts in Reproductive Investment of Female Northern Grass Lizards ( Takydromus septentrionalis

AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF. Ashley Danielle Messner for the Master of Science Degree. In Biological Sciences presented on 9 April 2015

Evolution of viviparity in warm-climate lizards: an experimental test of the maternal manipulation hypothesis

Maternal Thermal Effects on Female Reproduction and Hatchling Phenotype in the Chinese Skink (Plestiodon chinensis)

Phenotypic Effects of Thermal Mean and Fluctuations on Embryonic Development and Hatchling Traits in a Lacertid Lizard, Takydromus septentrionalis

Eric J. McElroy 1, Philip J. Bergmann 2 1. Introduction

Reproductive Strategy and Cycle of the Toad-headed Agama Phrynocephalus grumgrzimailoi (Agamidae) in Xinjiang, China

Phenotypic Plasticity in Embryonic Development of Reptiles: Recent Research and Research Opportunities in China

Maturity and Other Reproductive Traits of the Kanahebi Lizard Takydromus tachydromoides (Sauria, Lacertidae) in Mito

BODY TEMPERATURE, THERMAL TOLERANCE AND INFLUENCE OF TEMPERATURE ON SPRINT SPEED AND FOOD ASSIMILATION IN ADULT GRASS LIZARDS,

Consequences of Extended Egg Retention in the Eastern Fence Lizard (Sceloporus undulatus)

The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis)

Wen SHEN 1, Jianchi PEI 2, Longhui LIN 3* and Xiang JI Introduction

Barking up the right tree: comparative use of arboreal and terrestrial artificial refuges to survey reptiles in temperate eucalypt woodlands

Lizard malaria: cost to vertebrate host's reproductive success

DECREASED SPRINT SPEED AS A COST OF REPRODUCTION IN THE LIZARD SCELOPORUS OCCIDENTALS: VARIATION AMONG POPULATIONS

IN species where tail autotomy can occur during

Tail autotomy and subsequent regeneration alter the mechanics of locomotion in lizards

Sprint speed capacity of two alpine skink species, Eulamprus kosciuskoi and Pseudemoia entrecasteauxii

Introduction. Lizards: very diverse colour patterns intra- and interspecific differences in colour

Accessory Publication

Phenotypic Responses of Hatchlings to Constant Versus Fluctuating Incubation Temperatures in the Multi-banded Krait, Bungarus multicintus (Elapidae)

Telling Tails: Selective Pressures Acting on Investment in Lizard Tails*

Flexibility in antipredatory behavior allows wall lizards to cope with multiple types of predators

Uncertainty about future predation risk modulates monitoring behavior from refuges in lizards

Incubation temperature affects hatchling growth but not sexual phenotype in the Chinese soft-shelled turtle, Pelodiscus sinensis (Trionychidae)

A comparison of placental tissue in the skinks Eulamprus tympanum and E. quoyii. Yates, Lauren A.

Supporting Online Material for

Lacerta vivipara Jacquin

Reproductive traits of the gray ratsnake Ptyas korros from three geographically distinct populations

Juvenile Collared Lizards Adjust Tail Display Frequency in Response to Variable Predatory Threat

Sexual Dimorphism, Female Reproductive Characteristics and Egg Incubation in an Oviparous Forest Skink (Sphenomorphus incognitus) from South China

Meal Size Effects on Antipredator Behavior of Hatchling Trinket Snakes, Elaphe helena

CHOOSING YOUR REPTILE LIGHTING AND HEATING

Geographical differences in maternal basking behaviour and offspring growth rate in a climatically widespread viviparous reptile

Viviparity in high altitude Phrynocephalus lizards is adaptive because embryos cannot fully develop without maternal thermoregulation

How Does Photostimulation Age Alter the Interaction Between Body Size and a Bonus Feeding Program During Sexual Maturation?

MATERNAL NEST-SITE CHOICE AND OFFSPRING FITNESS IN A TROPICAL SNAKE (TROPIDONOPHIS MAIRII, COLUBRIDAE)

Incubation temperature and phenotypic traits of Sceloporus undulatus: implications for the northern limits of distribution

SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN BODY SHAPE WITHOUT SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN BODY SIZE IN WATER SKINKS (EULAMPRUS QUOYII)

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Zoology 113 (2010) 33 38

Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Short-term Water Potential Fluctuations and Eggs of the Red-eared Slider Turtle (Trachemys scripta elegans)

CAMBRIDGE, MASS. 26 MARCH 2010 NUMBER 519 CRUISE FORAGING OF INVASIVE CHAMELEON (CHAMAELEO JACKSONII XANTHOLOPHUS) IN HAWAI I

STATE-DEPENDENT AND RISK-SENSITIVE ESCAPE DECISIONS IN A FOSSORIAL REPTILE, THE AMPHISBAENIAN BLANUS CINEREUS

Social and Thermal Cues Influence Nest-site Selection in a Nocturnal Gecko, Oedura lesueurii

, SHUI-YU FU 2, magnesium from the yolk but withdraw approximately 35.6% of their total calcium requirements from the eggshell.

WATER plays an important role in all stages

Thermal adaptation of maternal and embryonic phenotypes in a geographically widespread ectotherm

RESEARCH ARTICLE Potentially adaptive effects of maternal nutrition during gestation on offspring phenotype of a viviparous reptile

Supplementary Fig. 1: Comparison of chase parameters for focal pack (a-f, n=1119) and for 4 dogs from 3 other packs (g-m, n=107).

The Seasonal Acclimatisation of Locomotion in a Terrestrial Reptile, Plestiodon chinensis (Scincidae)

Effects of Cage Stocking Density on Feeding Behaviors of Group-Housed Laying Hens

J. CLOBERT,* A. OPPLIGER, G. SORCI,* B. ERNANDE,* J. G. SWALLOW and T. GARLAND JR

Reptiles and amphibian behaviour

Influence of Incubation Temperature on Morphology, Locomotor Performance, and Early Growth of Hatchling Wall Lizards (Podarcis muralis)

Geographic variation in lizard phenotypes: importance of the incubation environment

Offspring performance and the adaptive benefits of. prolonged pregnancy: experimental tests in a viviparous lizard

DOES VIVIPARITY EVOLVE IN COLD CLIMATE REPTILES BECAUSE PREGNANT FEMALES MAINTAIN STABLE (NOT HIGH) BODY TEMPERATURES?

SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN HEAD SIZE IN THE LITTLE BROWN SKINK (SCINCELLA LATERALIS)

Adjustments In Parental Care By The European Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris): The Effect Of Female Condition

Unhatched and Hatched Eggshells of the Chinese Cobra Naja atra

Effects of nest temperature and moisture on phenotypic traits of hatchling snakes (Tropidonophis mairii, Colubridae) from tropical Australia

A description of an Indo-Chinese rat snake (Ptyas korros [Schlegel, 1837]) clutch, with notes on an instance of twinning

Tail autotomy affects bipedalism but not sprint performance in a cursorial Mediterranean lizard

J Exp Biol Advance Online Articles. First posted online on 23 November 2015 as doi: /jeb

Escape Behaviors and Flight Initiation Distance in the Common Water Snake Nerodia sipedon

Embryonic oxygen enhances learning ability in hatchling lizards

Temperature acclimation affects thermal preference and tolerance in three Eremias lizards ( Lacertidae)

Weaver Dunes, Minnesota

Effects of Thermal and Hydric Conditions on Egg Incubation and Hatchling Phenotypes in Two Phrynocephalus Lizards

Tail regeneration after autotomy revives survival: A case from a long-term monitored lizard population under avian...

Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles

RELATIONSHIPS AMONG WEIGHTS AND CALVING PERFORMANCE OF HEIFERS IN A HERD OF UNSELECTED CATTLE

THE EFFECTS OF MORPHOLOGY AND PERCH DIAMETER ON SPRINT PERFORMANCE OF ANOLIS LIZARDS

PHENOTYPES AND SURVIVAL OF HATCHLING LIZARDS. Daniel A. Warner. MASTER OF SCIENCE in Biology

EFFECTS OF BODY SIZE AND SLOPE ON ACCELERATION OF A LIZARD {STELLJO STELLIO)

School of Zoology, University of Tasmania, PO Box 252C-05, Tas, 7001, Australia

Typical Snakes Part # 1

NOTES ON THE ECOLOGY AND NATURAL HISTORY OF TWO SPECIES OF EGERNIA (SCINCIDAE) IN WESTERN AUSTRALIA

*Using the 2018 List. Use the image below to answer question 6.

8/19/2013. Topic 14: Body support & locomotion. What structures are used for locomotion? What structures are used for locomotion?

COMPARING BODY CONDITION ESTIMATES OF ZOO BROTHER S ISLAND TUATARA (SPHENODON GUNTHERI) TO THAT OF THE WILD, A CLINICAL CASE

Offspring size number strategies: experimental manipulation of offspring size in a viviparous lizard (Lacerta vivipara)

Materials and methods

Influence of food type on specific dynamic action of the Chinese skink Eumeces chinensis

Context-dependent avoidance of predatory centipedes by nocturnal geckos (Oedura lesueurii)

Proximate Causes of Intraspecific Variation in Locomotor Performance in the Lizard Gallotia galloti

Lab VII. Tuatara, Lizards, and Amphisbaenids

A Population Analysis of the Common Wall Lizard Podarcis muralis in Southwestern France

THE adaptive significance, if any, of temperature-dependent

Phenotypic variation in smooth softshell turtles (Apalone mutica) from eggs incubated in constant versus fluctuating temperatures

Introduction to Leopard Gecko Care

posted online on 19 July 2016 as doi: /jeb

Interpopulational variation in costs of reproduction related to pregnancy in a viviparous lizard

The Origin of Species: Lizards in an Evolutionary Tree

Habitats and Field Methods. Friday May 12th 2017

Transcription:

ethology international journal of behavioural biology Ethology Tail Autotomy Plays No Important Role in Influencing Locomotor Performance and Anti-Predator Behavior in a Cursorial Gecko Hong-Liang Lu*, Guo-Hua Ding, Ping Ding* & Xiang Ji * Key Laboratory of Conservation Biology for Endangered Wildlife of the Ministry of Education, College of Life Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310058, Zhejiang, China Jiangsu Key Laboratory for Biodiversity and Biotechnology, College of Life Sciences, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing 210046, Jiangsu, China Correspondence Xiang Ji, Jiangsu Key Laboratory for Biodiversity and Biotechnology, College of Life Sciences, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing 210046, Jiangsu, China. E-mail: xji@mail.hz.zj.cn, xiangji150@ hotmail.com Received: January 21, 2010 Initial acceptance: February 22, 2010 Final acceptance: March 15, 2010 (J. Kotiaho) doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01780.x Abstract We used the frog-eyed sand gecko (Teratoscincus scincus) as a model system to evaluate the locomotor costs of tail loss, and to examine whether tailless geckos use alternative anti-predator behavior to compensate for the costs of tail loss. Of the 16 field-captured geckos, eight were used as experimental animals and the remaining ones as controls. Locomotor performance, activity level and anti-predator behavior were measured for experimental geckos before and after the tail-removing treatment. Control geckos never undergoing the tail-removing manipulation were measured to serve as controls for the measurements taken at the same time for experimental geckos. Experimental geckos did not differ from controls in activity level before they underwent the tail-removing manipulation, but became less active thereafter. The mean locomotor stamina of tailless geckos was reduced by about 30% of the mean value for tailed ones. However, as the maximum stamina predicted from the laboratory trials is seldom required in nature, we expect that the costs associated with the reduced locomotor stamina may be relatively minor in T. scincus. All other examined locomotor (overall speed, maximal speed and stride length) and behavioral (distance to refuge, approach distance and flight distance) traits were not affected by the tail-removing manipulation. Overall, our results suggest that tail autotomy plays no important role in influencing locomotor performance and anti-predator behavior in lizards where the tail has no direct role in locomotion but is used to direct predatory strikes away from the torso. Introduction Many lizards use tail autotomy to evade fatal predatory encounters after other anti-predation strategies such as crypsis and flight have failed (Arnold 1988). While providing an immediate survival benefit by facilitating escape from predators, tail autotomy entails a number of costs that may affect the individual s subsequent fitness (Arnold 1988; Bernardo & Agosta 2005; Clause & Capaldi 2006; Bateman & Fleming 2009). For example, the loss of energy stored in the tail may retard growth (Ballinger & Tinkle 1979; Niewiarowski et al. 1997; Goodman 2006), reduce reproductive output (Smyth 1974; Dial & Fitzpatrick 1981; Taylor 1986; Wilson & Booth 1998) and increase mortality during hibernation (Bauwens 1981; Daniels 1984). Tailless lizards may also have reduced abilities to survive subsequent predatory encounters (Dial & Fitzpatrick 1984; Niewiarowski et al. 1997; Fox & McCoy 2000; Downes & Shine 2001), decreased home range size and access to females (Martin & Salvador 1993a; Salvador et al. 1995) and diminished social status (Fox & Rostker 1982; Fox et al. 1990; Martin & Ethology 116 (2010) 627 634 ª 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 627

Salvador 1993a). Many lizards have evolved specialized behavioral shifts to compensate for the costs associated with tail loss. For example, tailless lizards may rely more on crypsis, stay closer to refuges, and flee earlier from predators (Formanowicz et al. 1990; Downes & Shine 2001; Cooper 2003a, 2007). However, these behavioral adjustments often entail additional costs (Martín & Salvador 1993a; Downes & Shine 2001). Of the costs associated with tail autotomy, changes in locomotor performance and anti-predator behavior have received the most attention. The effects of tail autotomy on locomotor performance may vary among lizard species. For example, while reducing running speed in many lizards (Fleming et al. 2009 and references therein), tail loss increases running speed in other lizards such as the Texas banded gecko Coleonyx brevi (Dial & Fitzpatrick 1981), the marbled gecko Christinus (Phyllodactylus) marmoratus (Daniels 1983) and the common wall lizard Podarcis muralis (Brown et al. 1995) because of reduced weight, reduced friction with the substrate or altered antipredator behavior. In species where the tail has no direct role in locomotion, the locomotor costs associated with tail loss are relatively minor or even negligible (Kelehear & Webb 2006; Medger et al. 2008). The effects of tail loss on behavioral performance may also vary among lizard species. For example, tailless Texas banded geckos increase foraging activities to meet the increased energetic demands during tail regeneration (Dial & Fitzpatrick 1981). However, as increasing activity level may potentially increase the risks of predation, not all lizards do the same as C. brevi after undergoing tail loss. For example, Iberian rock lizards (Lacerta monticola; Martin & Salvador 1993b), Mongolian racerunners (Eremias argus; Zhao et al. 2008) and many-lined sun skinks (Mabuya multifasciata; Sun et al. 2009) do not increase foraging activities after undergoing tail autotomy. Taken together, previous studies of lizards show that tail autotomy often affects locomotor and behavioral performances, but not always. In this study, we investigated the impact of tail loss on locomotor performance and anti-predator behavior in a psammophilous cursorial gecko, Teratoscincus scincus. We address four questions: (1) does tail loss affect locomotor performance in cursorial geckos? (2) if so, how are different locomotor variables affected by tail loss? (3) do tailless geckos compensate for any negative effects of tail loss by using alternative anti-predator behaviors? (4) if so, how are different aspects of anti-predator behavior affected by tail loss? Materials and Methods Study Animals The frog-eyed sand gecko (Teratoscincus scincus) is a medium-sized (to 93 mm snout-vent length, SVL), nocturnal gekkonid lizard that is widely distributed in desert regions in central Asia (westwards to Iran), including Northwest China (Liu 1999). In daytime, it hides in burrows in the sand, leaving them after dark to forage outside (Liu 1999). Despite the fact that it is taxonomically and biogeographically well known (Macey et al. 1997, 1999; Hiller 2005), the ecology and biology of T. scincus remain poorly known (but see Marcellini 1977; Szél et al. 1986; Loewe et al. 1996; Seligmann et al. 2007). We collected 16 adult males (SVL > 70 mm) in late Apr. 2008 from a population in Jinghe (44 40 N, 82 55 E), Xinjiang, north-west China. Each individual was intact and showed no evidence of previous tail autotomy. Geckos were placed singly in cloth bags and transported to our laboratory in Hangzhou, where they were weighed, measured for SVL and tail length, and marked by a non-toxic waterproof label for future identification. We housed four geckos in each of four cages (length width height: 50 cm 20 cm 25 cm) that contained a substrate of sand (10 cm in depth), with rocks and pieces of clay tiles provided as the cover. The cages were placed in an indoor animal holding facility where temperatures varied from 22 C to 28 C. Geckos were fed a combination of mealworms (larvae of Tenebrio molitor) and house crickets (Acheta domesticus), and water enriched with vitamins and minerals was provided ad libitum. Geckos were held in captivity 3 wk prior to experimentation and were released at the site of capture soon after the experiments. Experimental Procedures Eight geckos were used as experimental animals, and the remaining ones as controls. Experimental geckos did not differ from controls in SVL, body mass and tail length (ANOVA for SVL and ANCOVA with SVL as the covariate for the other two variables; all p > 0.264). We measured locomotor performance of control and experimental geckos having intact tails at a body temperature of 28 C, which was achieved by placing geckos into an incubator at the corresponding temperature for about 2 h prior to each trial. Geckos were individually chased down the length of a 1.2 m racetrack with one transparent side through which they were filmed with a 628 Ethology 116 (2010) 627 634 ª 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH

PCO1200 high-speed digital camera (100 frames per second; Cooke Corporation, Romulus, MI, USA) connected to a PC computer. Each gecko was chased twice, with a minimum of 30 min rest between the two trials. The clips were examined on a frame-byframe basis using MaxTRAQ 2D software (Innovision Systems Inc., Columbiaville, MI, USA) for the overall speed (the maximal length traveled without stopping divided by the time taken), maximal speed (the maximal stride speed calculated as stride length divided by stride duration) and stride length (the interval between consecutive footfalls of the right hindfoot). The stride length was repeatedly measured for each gecko, with the mean value used for statistical analyses. The locomotor stamina was measured on a TSE treadmill (TSE Systems International Group, Bad Homburg, Germany) at a speed of 22 cm s. We individually introduced geckos into the treadmill, and then tapped on the mid-body with a paintbrush to encourage them to run. The time it took for each gecko to run until exhaustion, as estimated from the loss of any locomotor ability after strong stimulation on the head with the same paintbrush, was considered as its locomotor stamina. Following the measurement of locomotor performance, we moved all geckos into a 4 m 4 m enclosure built in a 60 m 2 room where temperatures were controlled at 22.0 1.0 C. The enclosure contained a substrate of sand (15 cm in depth), with nine plastic pipes (30 cm in length and 15 cm in diameter) half-buried in the substrate in a random way. Three 200 W ceramic heaters suspended above the enclosure created thermal gradients ranging from the room temperature to about 55 C for 24 h daily, so that geckos had an ample opportunity to regulate body temperature. We recorded activity level (the number of geckos that could be seen) and behavioral responses (distance to nearest refuge, approach distance and flight distance) to an approaching person (always the first author beginning at a fixed point 5 m away from the centre of the enclosure) at a speed of 60 m min during the period of 20:00 23:00 hours (Liu 1999) for 7 d. Distance to refuge was the distance between a sighted gecko and the nearest plastic pipe. Approach distance was the distance between the approaching person and a gecko when it began to flee. Flight distance was the distance between a gecko s initial position and its first stop position after fleeing. After obtaining data for experimental geckos having intact tails, we removed their tails by grasping the tail base with forceps, thereby producing tailless geckos. All these individuals were not anesthetized for the autotomy so that tail separation always occurred at natural fracture planes. Tailless geckos were allowed to heal the wound for 7 d to minimize the possible influence of handling stress, and were thereafter examined following the procedures described above. Control geckos were also examined to serve as controls for the measurements taken at the same time for tailless geckos. Statistical Analyses All data were tested for normality (Kolmogorov Smirnov test) and homogeneity of variances (F-max test). No data needed to be transformed to meet the assumptions of parametric analyses. We used Wilcoxon matched pairs test to examine whether the tailremoving manipulation induced variation in activity level. None of the examined locomotor and behavioral variables was dependent on body size (SVL) (linear regression analysis; all p > 0.15), so we used repeated measures ANOVA to analyze the corresponding data, with tail-removing manipulation as the between-subject factor and time step (before and after the manipulation) as the within-subject factor. Descriptive statistics are presented as mean SE, and the significance level is set at a = 0.05. Results Experimental geckos did not differ from control ones in activity level before they underwent the tailremoving manipulation (Z = 1.83, p = 0.068), but became less active when their tails were removed (Z = 2.37, p < 0.02) (Fig. 1). Activity levels did not differ in control geckos measured before and after the tail-removing episode (Z = 0.27, p = 0.787) (Fig. 1). The tail-removing manipulation reduced locomotor stamina, but had no influence on the other three locomotor variables (Fig. 2; Table 1). Mean values for the three behavioral variables (distance to refuge, approach distance and flight distance) were apparently greater in tailed geckos (Fig. 3) but, statistically, these values did not differ between experimental and control geckos, and between geckos measured before and after the tail-removing episode (Table 2). Discussion Our sample size was relatively low (eight geckos in each group), and this limitation might hamper detection of differences. However, as the observed Ethology 116 (2010) 627 634 ª 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 629

Fig. 1: Mean values (+SE) for activity levels of tailed and tailless frogeyed sand geckos. E, experimental geckos; C, control geckos; solid bars: activity levels measured before the tail-removing treatment; open bars: activity levels measured after the tail-removing treatment. The asterisk denotes a significant difference (p < 0.05) in activity level between tailed and tailless geckos. non-significant differences are so small, we believe that our conclusions (acceptance or rejection of the null hypothesis) are likely to be identical even when sample size would be much larger. The tail is one of the major sites of energy storage and represents a considerable portion of body mass in T. scincus (Liu 1999). We predicted that T. scincus would become faster after undergoing tail loss as a result of reduced weight or friction with the substrate, as for C. brevi (Dial & Fitzpatrick 1981) and C. marmoratus (Daniels 1983). Contrary to our expectation, tailless frog-eyed sand geckos did not differ from tailed ones in running speed (Fig. 2; Table 1), thus suggesting that the tail may not be a major physical burden for T. scincus to carry over short distances (such as those tested in this study). On the racetrack and during encounters with an approaching human predator, tailed frog-eyed sand geckos always raised their tails to run. Moreover, tailed and tailless geckos never run the entire length of the racetrack but exhibited intermittent locomotion, with movement sequences interrupted by frequent short pauses. During short pauses, tailed geckos raised or vigorously waggled their tails. What can be inferred from these observations are: (1) the tail plays no important role in influencing locomotor speed in T. scincus; and (2) the frog-eyed sand gecko is among lizard species that use tail displays to attract predatory strikes away from the torso (Bustard 1965; Congdon et al. 1974; Daniels et al. 1986; McConnachie & Whiting 2003; Kelehear & Webb 2006). In this study, tail loss impacted only one aspect of locomotion by reducing locomotor stamina. The negative impact of tail loss on locomotor stamina has also been found in the eastern water skink Sphenomorphus (Eulamprus) quoyii (Daniels 1985), the metallic skink Niveoscincus metallicus (Chapple & Swain 2002) and the Cape dwarf gecko Lygodactylus capensis (Fleming et al. 2009). It is hypothesized in N. metallicus that the increase of stride frequency due to tail loss may reduce locomotor stamina (Chapple & Swain 2002). Our data do not support this idea, because tailed geckos did not differ from tailless ones in both stride length and locomotor speed (Fig. 2; Table 1), and thus stride frequency. Fleming et al. Fig. 2: Mean values (+SE) for the four locomotor variables (overall speed, maximal speed, stride length and locomotor stamina) of tailed and tailless frog-eyed sand geckos. E, experimental geckos; C, control geckos; solid bars: measurements taken before the tail-removing treatment; open bars: measurements taken after the tail-removing treatment. The asterisk denotes a significant difference (p < 0.05) in locomotor stamina between tailed and tailless geckos. 630 Ethology 116 (2010) 627 634 ª 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH

Table 1: Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on the four locomotor variables (overall speed, maximal speed, stride length and locomotor stamina) measured for tailed and tailless frog-eyed sand geckos, with animal category (control vs. experimental geckos) as the between-subject factor and time step (measurements taken before and after the tail-removing treatment) as the within-subject factor Locomotor variables Overall speed Maximal speed Stride length Locomotor stamina Control vs. experimental geckos F 1,14 = 0.99, p = 0.337 F 1,14 = 0.04, p = 0.840 F 1,14 = 0.05, p = 0.828 F 1,14 = 3.13, p = 0.099 Time step F 1,14 = 0.43, p = 0.522 F 1,14 = 0.01, p = 0.927 F 1,14 = 0.002, p = 0.967 F 1,14 = 9.27, p < 0.01 T1 > T2 Interaction F 1,14 = 1.28, p = 0.277 F 1,14 = 0.94, p = 0.348 F 1,14 = 1.02, p = 0.331 F 1,14 = 9.91, p < 0.01 T1, measurements taken before the tail-removing treatment; T2, measurements taken after the tail-removing treatment. (2009) interpret a reduction in locomotor stamina in tailless L. capensis as reflecting a reduction in active metabolic rate after tail autotomy. However, we are Fig. 3: Mean values (+SE) for the three anti-predator behavioral variables (distance of refuge, approach and flight distance) of tailed and tailless frog-eyed sand geckos. E, experimental geckos; C, control geckos; solid bars: measurements taken before the tail-removing treatment; open bars: measurements taken after the tail-removing treatment. currently unable to test whether this interpretation also applies to T. scincus because our experiment was not designed to look at the effects of tail loss on locomotor energetics. The mean locomotor stamina of tailless geckos was reduced by about 30% of the mean value for tailed ones (Fig. 2). However, as free-ranging frog-eyed sand geckos are able to assess predation risks and react accordingly by modulating approach distance, speed and pauses (Liu 1999), the maximum stamina predicted from the laboratory trials may be seldom required in nature. We therefore expect that the costs associated with the reduced locomotor stamina may be relatively minor in T. scincus. Tail regeneration is the most effective way to counteract the costs of tail loss. Thus, in most cases a successful autotomous escape is followed by regeneration of the lost tail. Tail regeneration is energetically expensive and may, therefore, require the diversion of energetic resources from other functions such as growth and reproduction (Congdon et al. 1974; Ballinger & Tinkle 1979; Dial & Fitzpatrick 1981; Bellairs & Bryant 1985; Martin & Salvador 1993b). Regeneration costs may also be met by increasing food intake (Dial & Fitzpatrick 1981). Assuming a positive correlation between foraging activity and food intake, one may expect that to increase food intake tailless lizards would forage longer than tailed ones. For example, tailless Texas banded geckos have to be more active to increase foraging time (Dial & Fitzpatrick 1981). However, as tailless lizards must forage without their primary defense mechanism, the risks of predation to tailless lizards may be concomitantly greater. Thus, if being active is costly for a lizard species, tailless lizards that reduce activity levels could be more likely to minimize predation risks while simultaneously saving energy that can be shunt to tail regeneration (Formanowicz et al. 1990; Martin & Salvador 1993b; Salvador et al. 1995; Downes & Shine 2001; Cooper 2007; Zhao et al. 2008). Our finding that tailless Ethology 116 (2010) 627 634 ª 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 631

Table 2: Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on the three anti-predator behavioral variables (distance of refuge, approach and flight distance) measured for tailed and tailless frog-eyed sand geckos, with animal category (control vs. experimental geckos) as the between-subject factor and time step (measurements taken before and after the tail-removing treatment) as the within-subject factor Anti-predator behavioral variables Distance to refuge Approach distance Flight distance Control vs. experimental geckos F 1,14 = 0.003, p = 0.959 F 1,14 = 0.05, p = 0.822 F 1,14 = 0.12, p = 0.734 Time step F 1,14 = 2.01, p = 0.178 F 1,14 = 3.20, p = 0.095 F 1,14 = 4.24, p = 0.059 Interaction F 1,14 = 1.90, p = 0.189 F 1,14 = 0.53, p = 0.477 F 1,14 = 0.29, p = 0.600 geckos reduced activity levels is of interest, because it might suggest that tailless frog-eyed sand geckos use this behavioral adjustment to minimize predation risks in nature. More interestingly, a reduction of activity level following tail autotomy appears to be common in lizards that use tail displays to direct predatory strikes away from the torso (Bustard 1965; Congdon et al. 1974; Daniels et al. 1986; Kelehear & Webb 2006). Lizards often use alternative anti-predator behaviors when undergoing tail autotomy (Martín & Salvador 1993b; Cooper 2003b; 2007). However, the effects of tail autotomy on aspects of anti-predator behavior may vary among species. For example, tailless keeled earless lizards (Holbrookia propinqua) tend to stay closer to refuges, and take a greater flight distance but a similar approach distance (Cooper 2003a). In the striped plateau lizard (Sceloporus virgatus), however, tailless lizards do not change the distance to refuges, but run sooner and flee further during encounters with predators (Cooper 2007). Our finding that none of the three examined aspects of anti-predator behavior is affected by tail loss in T. scincus is of interest, because it suggests that the effects of tail autotomy on anti-predator behavior may be more diverse in lizards than thought before. There are three possible explanations for this finding. First, because locomotor speed is not negatively affected by tail autotomy, it is not necessary for tailless frog-eyed sand geckos to alter their anti-predator behaviors because their ability to flee from an approaching predator is apparently unaffected by tail autotomy. Secondly, field (Liu 1999) and laboratory (Fig. 3) studies show that both tailed and tailless frog-eyed sand geckos both stay quite close to refuges. Hence, we expect that changes in anti-predator behavior may contribute little (if any) to the survival benefit, because tailless geckos may not take a longer time than tailed ones to enter available retreat sites during encounters with predators. Finally, it is conceivable that our experimental design, in which behavioral trials were carried out in a small laboratory enclosure, did not allow us to detect changes in a gecko s ability to use alternative antipredator behaviors to compensate for the costs associated with tail loss. Consistent with the results reported for O. lesueurii (Kelehear & Webb 2006) where the tail also has no direct role in locomotion but is used to attract predatory strikes away from the torso, our data show that tail autotomy has no important role in influencing locomotor performance and anti-predator behavior in T. scincus. Acknowledgements We thank Hong Li, Jian-Fang Gao, Lai-Gao Luo and Rui-Bin Hu for their assistance in the laboratory. The work was carried out in compliance with the current laws in China on animal welfare and research, and was supported by grants from Nanjing Normal University to XJ and a post-doc fellowship from Zhejiang University to HLL. Literature Cited Arnold, E. N. 1988: Caudal autotomy as a defence. In: Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 16 (Gans, C. & Huey, R., eds). Alan R. Liss, New York, pp. 235 273. Ballinger, R. E. & Tinkle, D. W. 1979: On the cost of tail regeneration to body growth in lizards. J. Herpetol. 13, 374 375. Bateman, P. W. & Fleming, P. A. 2009: To cut a long tail short: a review of lizard caudal autotomy studies carried out over the last 20 years. J. Zool. (Lond.) 277, 1 14. Bauwens, D. 1981: Survivorship during hibernation in the European common lizard, Lacerta vivipara. Copeia 1981, 741 744. Bellairs, A. D. A. & Bryant, S. V. 1985: Autotomy and regeneration in reptiles. In: Biology of the Reptilia, Vol. 15 (Gans, C. & Billet, F., eds). Wiley, New York, pp. 301 410. Bernardo, J. & Agosta, S. J. 2005: Evolutionary implications of hierarchical impacts of nonlethal injury on 632 Ethology 116 (2010) 627 634 ª 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH

reproduction, including maternal effects. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 86, 309 331. Brown, R. M., Taylor, D. H. & Gist, D. H. 1995: Effect of caudal autotomy on locomotor performance of wall lizards (Podarcis muralis). J. Herpetol. 29, 98 105. Bustard, H. R. 1965: Observations on Australian geckos. Herpetologica 21, 294 302. Chapple, D. G. & Swain, R. 2002: Effect of caudal autotomy on locomotor performance in a viviparous skink, Niveoscincus metallicus. Funct. Ecol. 16, 817 825. Clause, A. R. & Capaldi, E. A. 2006: Caudal autotomy and regeneration in lizards. J. Exp. Zool. A 305, 965 973. Congdon, J. D., Vitt, L. J. & King, W. W. 1974: Geckos: adaptive significance and energetics of tail autotomy. Science 184, 1379 1380. Cooper, W. E. 2003a: Shifted balance of risk and cost after autotomy affects use of cover, escape, activity, and foraging in the keeled earless lizard (Holbrookia propinqua). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 54, 179 187. Cooper, W. E. 2003b: Effect of risk on aspects of escape behavior by a lizard, Holbrookia propinqua, in relation to optimal escape theory. Ethology 109, 617 626. Cooper, W. E. 2007: Compensatory changes in escape and refuge use following autotomy in the lizard Sceloporus virgatus. Can. J. Zool. 85, 99 107. Daniels, C. B. 1983: Running: an escape strategy enhanced by autotomy. Herpetologica 39, 162 165. Daniels, C. B. 1984: The importance of caudal lipid in the gecko Phyllodactylus marmoratus. Herpetologica 40, 337 344. Daniels, C. B. 1985: The effect of tail autotomy on the exercise capacity of the water skink, Sphenomorphus quoyii. Copeia 1985, 1074 1077. Daniels, C. B., Flaherty, S. P. & Simbotwe, M. P. 1986: Tail size and effectiveness of autotomy in a lizard. J. Herpetol. 20, 93 96. Dial, B. E. & Fitzpatrick, L. C. 1981: The energetic costs of tail autotomy to reproduction in the lizard Coleonyx brevis (Sauria: Gekkonidae). Oecologia 51, 310 317. Dial, B. E. & Fitzpatrick, L. C. 1984: Predator escape success in tailed versus tailless Scincella lateralis (Sauria: Scincidae). Anim. Behav. 32, 301 302. Downes, S. J. & Shine, R. 2001: Why does tail loss increase a lizard s later vulnerability to snake predators? Ecology 82, 1293 1303. Fleming, P. A., Verburgt, L., Scantlebury, M., Medger, K. & Bateman, P. W. 2009: Jettisoning ballast or fuel? Caudal autotomy and locomotory energetics of the Cape dwarf gecko Lygodactylus capensis (Gekkonidae). Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 82, 756 765. Formanowicz, D. R., Jr, Brodie, E. D., Jr & Bradley, P. J. 1990: Behavioural compensation for tail loss in the ground skink, Scincella lateralis. Anim. Behav. 40, 782 784. Fox, S. F., Heger, N. A. & Delay, L. S. 1990: Social cost of tail loss in Uta stansburiana: lizard tails as status-signaling badges. Anim. Behav. 39, 549 554. Fox, S. F. & McCoy, J. K. 2000: The effects of tail loss on survival, growth, reproduction, and sex ratio of offspring in the lizard Uta stansburiana in the field. Oecologia 122, 327 334. Fox, S. F. & Rostker, M. A. 1982: Social cost of tail loss in Uta stansburiana. Science 218, 692 693. Goodman, R. M. 2006: Effects of tail loss on growth and sprint speed of juvenile Eumeces fasciatus (Scincidae). J. Herpetol. 40, 99 102. Hiller, U. 2005: Morphology and function of the dorsal sound producing scales in the tail of Teratoscincus scincus (Reptilia: Gekkonidae). J. Morphol. 144, 119 130. Kelehear, C. & Webb, J. K. 2006: Effects of tail autotomy on anti-predator behavior and locomotor performance in a nocturnal gecko. Copeia 2006, 803 809. Liu, Y.-Z. 1999. Teratoscincus Strauch. In: Fauna Sinica, Reptilia, Vol. 2 (Zhao, E.-M., Zhao, K.-T. & Zhou, K.-Y., eds). Science Press, Beijing, China, pp. 271 360. Loewe, R., Govardovskii, V., Röhlich, P. & Szél, A. 1996: Microspectrophotometric and immunocytochemical identification of ultraviolet photoreceptors in geckos. Visual Neurosci. 13, 247 256. Macey, J. R., Ananjeva, N. B., Wang, Y. & Papenfuss, T. J. 1997: A taxonomic reevaluation of the gekkonid lizard genus Teratoscincus in China. Rus. J. Herpetol. 4, 8 16. Macey, J. R., Wang, Y., Ananjeva, N. B., Larson, A. & Papenfuss, T. J. 1999: Vicariant patterns of fragmentation among gekkonid lizards of the genus Teratoscincus produced by the Indian Collision: a molecular phylogenetic perspective and an area cladogram for Central Asia. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 12, 320 332. Marcellini, D. 1977: Acoustic and visual display behavior of gekkonid lizards. Am. Zool. 17, 251 260. Martin, J. & Salvador, A. 1993a: Tail loss reduces mating success in the Iberian rock-lizard, Lacerta monticola. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 32, 185 189. Martín, J. & Salvador, A. 1993b: Tail loss and foraging tactics of the Iberian rock-lizard, Lacerta monticola. Oikos 66, 318 324. McConnachie, S. & Whiting, M. 2003: Costs associated with tail autotomy in an ambush foraging lizard, Cordylus melanotus melanotus. Afr. J. Zool. 38, 57 65. Medger, K., Verburgt, L. & Bateman, P. W. 2008: The influence of tail autotomy on the escape response of the cape dwarf gecko, Lygodactylus capensis. Ethology 114, 42 52. Niewiarowski, P. H., Congdon, J. D., Dunham, A. E., Vitt, L. J. & Tinkle, D. W. 1997: Tales of lizard tails: effects of tail autotomy on subsequent survival and growth of free-ranging hatchling Uta stansburiana. Can. J. Zool. 75, 542 548. Ethology 116 (2010) 627 634 ª 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 633

Salvador, A., Martín, J. & López, P. 1995: Tail loss reduces home range size and access to females in male lizards, Psammodromus algirus. Behav. Ecol. 6, 382 387. Seligmann, H., Anderson, S. C., Autumn, K., Bouskila, A., Saf, R., Tuniyev, B. S. & Werner, Y. L. 2007: Analysis of the locomotor activity of a nocturnal desert lizard (Reptilia: Gekkonidae: Teratoscincus scincus) under varying moonlight. Zoology 110, 104 117. Smyth, M. 1974: Changes in the fat stores of the skinks Morethia boulengeri and Hemiergis peronii (Lacertilia). Aust. J. Zool. 22, 135 145. Sun, Y.-Y., Yang, J. & Ji, X. 2009: Many-lined sun skinks (Mabuya multifasciata) do not compensate for the costs of tail loss by increasing feeding rate or digestive efficiency. J. Exp. Zool. A 331, 125 133. Szél, A., Röhlich, P. & Govardovskii, V. 1986: Immunocytochemical discrimination of visual pigments in the retinal photoreceptors of the nocturnal gecko Teratoscincus scincus. Exp. Eye. Res. 43, 895 904. Taylor, J. A. 1986: Seasonal energy storage in the Australian lizard, Ctenotus taeniolatus. Copeia 1986, 445 453. Wilson, R. S. & Booth, D. T. 1998: Effect of tail loss on reproductive output and its ecological significance in the skink Eulamprus quoyii. J. Herpetol. 32, 128 131. Zhao, Q., Wang, Z., Liu, L.-L., Zhao, W.-G. & Ji, X. 2008: Selected body temperature, surface activity and food intake in tailed versus tailless Mongolian racerunners Eremias argus from three populations. Acta Zool. Sinica 54, 60 66. 634 Ethology 116 (2010) 627 634 ª 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH