PATTERN EVOKED RESPONSE DEFICIENCY IN PATTERN DEPRIVED CATS 1

Similar documents
M. uch interest has recently been focused. Visual development in cats. 394 Pettigrew Investigative Ophthalmology. S.

Effects of Early Monocular Lid Suture on Spatial and Temporal Sensitivity of Neurons in Dorsal Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of the Cat

Experimental analysis of amblyopia

lowering of the visual acuity. When closure was extended through the first by varying the age at eye closure. Waiting until 1 month of age

Differential Effects of Early Monocular Deprivation on Binocular and Monocular Segments of Cat Striate Cortex

CLARSBISHOP AREA IN THE CAT: LOCATION AIVD RETINOTOPICAL PROJECTION

Consequences of alternating monocular deprivation on eye alignment and convergence in cats. Randolph Blake, M. L. ]. Crawford, and Helmut V. B.

THE PRETRIGEMINAL CAT AS AN INSTRUMENT FOR INVESTIGATION OF THE OCULAR FIXATION REFLEX

preferring rightward movement. A changeover later than 5 weeks of age peak of the critical period for directional deprivation may occur earlier

deprived eye (reverse occlusion). beyond 1 year of age; only two of six animals recovered sufficient vision to enable

Pre-natal construction of neural circuits (the highways are genetically specified):

The contralateral impairment of the orienting ocular-following reflex after lesions of the lateral suprasylvian cortex in cats

abnormal lateral geniculate body. His anatomical study suggested that chiasm instead of remaining uncrossed. They thus reach the wrong hemispheres,

Binocular Exposure causes Suppression of the Less Experienced Eye in Cats Previously Reared with Unequal Alternating Monocular Exposure

THE POSTNATAL DEVELOPMENT OF THE VISUAL CORTEX AND THE INFLUENCE OF ENVIRONMENT

Development of Neuronal Response Properties in the Cat Dorsal Lateral Geniculate Nucleus During Monocular

Area Centralis Position Relative to the Optic Disc Projection in Kittens as o Function of Age

David H. Hubel. A Biographical Memoir by Robert H. Wurtz

geniculate nucleus of kittens raised with convergent squint in one eye,

Do blue-eyed white cats have normal or abnormal retinofugal pathways? R. W. Guillery, T. L. Hickey, and P. D. Spear

Effects of Retinal Image Degradation on Ocular Growth in Cats

Effects of Convergent Strabismus on the Development of Physiologically Identified Retinogeniculate Axons ih Cats

The Role of Early Experience in the Development and Maintenance of Orientation Selectivity in the Cat's Visual Cortex: M. Stryker

The Critical Period for Ocular Dominance Plasticity in the Ferret s Visual Cortex

Spatial and Temporal Sensitivity of Normal and Amblyopic Cats

Neuroscience Letters

Binocular Impulse Blockade Prevents the Formation of Ocular Dominance Columns in Cat Visual Cortex

THE VISUAL MECHANISMS OF TENEBRIO MOLITOR: VARIATIONS TAKING PLACE IN THE ERG OF PUPA AND ADULT DURING DEVELOPMENT

Supplementary Material

Binocular Interactions in Striate Cortical Neurons of Cats Reared with Discordant Visual Inputs

(Received 22 November 1984) studies were made on twenty such pairs; eight X on-centre, seven Y on-centre, two

spider monkeys by recording extracellularly from single units and stimulating

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar

1Ila and V. Canberra, A.C.T. 2601, Australia (Received 21 March 1979)

Expression of a Surface-Associated Antigen on Y-Cells in the Cat Lateral Geniculate Nucleus Is Regulated by Visual Experience

1250 Reports. Axial lengths and refractive errors in kittens reared with an optically induced anisometropia. EARL L. SMITH, III, GREGORY W.

Inhibitory mechanisms in the LGN: A possible substrate for amblyopia?

A case of achromatopsia. Perceptual Colour Space. Spectral Properties of Light. Subtractive Colour Mixture. Additive Colour Mixture

Perception & Attention Course. George Mather

injected eve. (Received 1 November 1977) with electrolytic lesions. A good correspondence was found between the location of

striking it with unsheathed claws, was accompanied

Morphology of Retinogeniculate X and Y Axon Arbors in Cats Raised With Binocular Lid Suture

The effect of environmental temperature on the growth of vertebrae in the tail of the mouse

UTILITY OF THE NEUROLOGICAL EXAMINATION IN RATS

Rapid Anatomical Plasticity of Horizontal Connections in the Developing Visual Cortex

Weekly Schedule of Neuroscience (2017/2018) Week 1

BEHAVIOUR OF THE DOMESTIC DOG (Canis familiaris)

RETINITIS PIGMENTOSA*

Department of Optometry, Oxford Eye Hospital Vision Related Electrodiagnostic Tests

(Received 29 June 1972)

Energy summation in lateral geniculate postsynaptic responses and optic tract spike responses to large light flashes

Lens luxation when the lens gets wobbly

Report. Darkness Alters Maturation of Visual Cortex and Promotes Fast Recovery from Monocular Deprivation

Weekly Schedule of Neuroscience (2018/2019) Week 1

Rules of Connectivity between Geniculate Cells and Simple Cells in Cat Primary Visual Cortex

Taste and Smell. Bởi: OpenStaxCollege

Effects of Feedback Projections From Area 18 Layers 2/3 to Area 17 Layers 2/3 in the Cat Visual Cortex

A SINGLE VIBRISSAL COLUMN IN THE FIRST SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX OF THE MOUSE DEMONSTRATED WITH 2-DEOXYGLUCOSE

Previous experiments on ferrets which were designed to determine the way in

Behavioral Characteristics of Scent Marking Behavior in. The Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus)

Visual and Instrumental Evaluation of Mottling and Striping

Serendipity and the Siamese Cat: The Discovery That Genes for Coat and Eye Pigment Affect the Brain. Jon H. Kaas

Horizontal Interactions in Cat Striate Cortex: 111. Receptive Fields and Transient Exuberance of Tangential Interactions

Parallel Processing in the Visual System THE CLASSIFICATION OF RETINAL GANGLION CELLS AND ITS IMPACT ON THE NEUROBIOLOGY OF VISION

Permanent Alterations in Muscarinic Receptors and Pupil Size Produced by Chronic Atropinization in Kittens

Cortical Cell Orientation Selectivity Fails to Develop in the Absence of ON-Center Retinal Ganglion Cell Activity

Properties of the flash visual evoked potential recorded in the cat primary visual cortex

Appendix from T. J. Ord and J. A. Stamps, Species Identity Cues in Animal Communication

texp. Biol. (196a), 39,

Behavioural Brain Research

iiiii,., IIIIL25 I I W I MIEROCOPY RESOLUTION TEST CHART NATIONAL BURLAU Of STANDARDS 1963 A

Key words: Mouse motor cortex, intracortical microstimulation, motor representation,.corticomotor asymmetry.

An Evaluation of Respondent Conditioning Procedures to Decrease Barking in an Animal Shelter

The Laminar and Size Distribution of Commissural Efferent Neurons in the Cat Visual Cortex*

Behavioral Properties of the Trigeminal Somatosensory System in Rats Performing Whisker-Dependent Tactile Discriminations

Regional Variation in the Representation of the Visual Field in the Visual Cortex of the Siamese Cat

[Frontiers in Bioscience 13, , May 1, 2008] Binocular phasic coactivation does not prevent ocular dominance segregation

Variations of blood flow at optic nerve head induced by sinusoidal flicker stimulation in cats

Plasticity in primary somatosensory cortex resulting from environmentally enriched stimulation and sensory discrimination training

Although owls can t move their eyes, many other adaptations help these raptors spot prey.

F1 IN THE NAME OF GOD

Temperature Gradient in the Egg-Laying Activities of the Queen Bee

My recollections of Hubel and Wiesel and a brief review of functional circuitry in the visual pathway

AnOn. Behav., 1971, 19,

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Elk (Cervus canadensis) Extra credit assignment. Sad Underwing (Catocala maestosa) 10/11/2017

Spatial Analysis of Ocular Dominance Patterns in Monocularly Deprived Cats

Wagner, 1980; Schuurmans, 1981). Recently several studies have concluded that the

Housing for Health, Wellness and Success: Standards for Facility Design and Environment. What is a healthy environment made of?

Veterinary Ophthalmology


INTRODUCTION. I. METHOD A. Animal preparation

Proceeding of the SEVC Southern European Veterinary Conference

Derived copy of Taste and Smell *

Cortical Areas Involved in Horizontal OKN in Cats: Metabolic Activity

examination, the slight resistance encountered being sufficient By J. HERBERT PARSONS.

Gun range noise attenuation prototype August 21, 2012 Pontiac Lake Recreation Area 7800 Gale Road Gun Range Waterford, Michigan The project:

INFLUENCE OF CONTAMINATION OF ENVIRONMENT AND BREEDING CONDITIONS ON DEVELOPMENT OF COCCIDIOSIS IN CHICKENS

Introducing the Client to CELTIC ART THERAPY and the Celtic Art Response Survey

Incubation Conditions and Integrity in Pekin Ducks

Distribution Unlimited

Transcription:

Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1973, 35: 569-573 Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company, Amsterdam - Printed in The Netherlands 569 PATTERN EVOKED RESPONSE DEFICIENCY IN PATTERN DEPRIVED CATS 1 JEROME M. SIEGEL, PAUL D. COLEMAN AND AUSTIN H. RIESEN Departments of Anatomy and Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, N. Y, 14642 and Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, Calif. 92502 (U.S.A.) (Accepted for publication: June 4, 1973) Depriving cats of patterned light has been shown to have profound effects on visually guided behavior. The animals are unable to learn pattern discriminations and lack visual placing, visual cliff discrimination and normal behavioral responses to impending collisions; these animals, however, are capable of light intensity discriminations (Riesen and Aarons 1959; Riesen 1965; Wiesel and Hubel 1965a, b). Human visual evoked potentials have been shown to be altered when different patterned stimuli are presented (White and Eason 1966; John 1967; Harter and White 1968). The development of this pattern response has been found to correlate with age in a human infant (Harter and Suitt 1970). Alterations in this pattern evoked response have also been found in the amblyopic eye of children with amblyopia ex anopsia (Lombroso et al. 1969). Spehlmann (1965), studying the pattern evoked response in normal humans, has shown that the latency and amplitude of late positive waves can be altered by presentation of patterns containing different amounts of contour. These findings suggest that a physiological correlate of defective pattern perception in the diffuse light reared cat might be found in its evoked response to patterned stimuli. We, therefore, undertook a study of slow wave evoked responses of pattern deprived cats and nondeprived litter mates to patterned visual stimuli containing varying amounts of contour to determine whether this physiological correlate exists. 1 This work was supported by Public Health Service- Grants NS-07870 and EY-NS-00573. METHOD Fourteen cats from four litters were reared by A.R. The litters were born in the lighted laboratory and moved into a totally darkened room one week after birth, before the eyes had opened. Daily exposures to light began on the 10th day after birth at which time each litter was divided into pattern deprived and control groups. Seven kittens were reared under each condition. Both groups were on a 21 h dark 3 h light schedule. Light exposure periods were divided into two 90 min intervals, and the kittens were away from their mothers during these times. During their hours in the light the control kittens were without visual restriction in a large area. The pattern deprived kittens were in the same area at the same time with fine percale hoods over their heads which diffused the light and thereby prevented pattern vision. When the cats reached the age of 6 months they were flown in light-tight boxes to Rochester, New York, where the physiological studies were performed. The experimenters did not know the rearing conditions of the cats until all the physiological measurements had been completed. The cats were anesthetized- with 65 mg/kg alpha chloralose given intravenously. The pupils were dilated and accommodation relaxed with cyclogyl (Schieffelin). The nictitating membrane of the right eye was retracted from the cornea and anchored to a head holder by suture threads and a contact lens was placed over the cornea to prevent drying. The animal was then positioned so that his area centralis was centered where visual stimuli were to be presented, using an

570 ophthalmoscope to observe the optic disk and the measurements of Vakkur et al. (1963) to determine the relative location of the area centralis. The stimulus patterns were brought into focus on the area centralis using a Copeland Streak Retinoscope and trial lenses. Experiments were performed in an electrically shielded sound-proof room. Body temperature was maintained between 35 and 38 C throughout each experiment. Seven different stimuli were used: 6 patterned cards and one blank white card. The patterns were all 50% black and 50% white and, therefore, reflected the same amount of light. A neutral density filter was placed in front of the eye when the blank white stimulus card was used, to reduce its brightness to the level of the other patterns. Three "low contour" patterns employed evenly spaced 3/4" black and white stripes, arranged into either horizontal stripes, vertical stripes, or a grating pattern. Two "high contour" patterns consisted of alternate black and white 1/8" stripes arranged in vertical stripe and curved stripe patterns. A checkerboard containing 1/4" black and white checks was the third "high contour" pattern. The stimulus cards were 18" x 24" and were mounted on a black background 1 m in front of the eye. The stimulus cards were presented in a counterbalanced order. They were illuminated from outside the soundproof room by a Slaughter model 163 stroboscope which was flashed every 3 sec by a PDP-5 computer. Forty visual evoked responses (VERs) were recorded and averaged by the computer for each stimulus pattern. Small electrode holes were drilled through the outer table of the skull and into the diploe on the left side of the skull. The holes were drilled only part way through the cranium in order to prevent deterioration of, or damage to, the brain. Silver ball electrodes were inserted into the holes and electrode paste was used to maintain a good electrical connection. The head holder was used as ground. Electrodes were placed over primary visual cortex, mid-suprasylvian gyrus and motor cortex. Electrode placements were verified visually when the brain was removed for a quantitative histological analysis of the brains which is now in progress. RESULTS J. M. SIEGEL et al. The VER recorded over primary visual cortex had two consistent components, an early positive wave (P1`) peaking 47.6 msec after stimulus presentation and a later positive wave (P2) peaking 111.2 msec after the stimulus. Average P1 amplitude was greater in the pattern deprived cats than in the normal controls (P< 0.0262-Mann-Whitney U test, 2-tailed). The latency of P1 was the same in both groups. Neither the latency nor the amplitude of the P1 wave appeared to change when patterns containing different amounts of contour were presented. The amplitude of the later P2 wave in the pattern deprived cats did not differ from that of the control group. The P2 latency, however, was shorter in the pattern deprived cats than in the controls (P<0.0042-Mann-Whitney U test, 2- tailed). In all control animals the average latency of the P2 wave after presentation of the high contour patterns consistently differed from its latency after low contour pattern presentation. The visual cortex P2 peaked later for the small patterns than for the large patterns in 5 out of 7 controls and earlier in the other two. In these two cats the small patterns appeared to produce a considerably shorter latency than that evoked by the blank stimulus as well. This appears to rule out small differences in sharpness of retinal focus which might obscure the contour in the small patterns and cause them to give VERs similar to the VERs evoked by the blank stimulus. In any single animal all electrode sites over primary visual cortex showed the same direction of latency change with changing amounts of contour. The average absolute difference between the latencies of small and large pattern evoked P2 waves was 13.3 msec in normally reared cats. In contrast to the normally reared cats, none of the pattern deprived cats showed substantial difference (P>0.10 Mann-Whitney U test, 2- tailed) between the average latencies of their P2 response to small patterns and to large patterns, the largest latency difference being only 4.27 msec. Two of the cats had no detectable difference in the latency of response to large and small patterns. The average absolute difference between the latency of small and large pattern

PATTERN EVOKED RESPONSES IN CATS 571 evoked P2 waves was 1.8 msec in the pattern deprived animals. This difference in latency of the P2 wave to high and low contour patterns was expressed by taking the average latencies to small patterns, subtracting the average latency to large patterns and dividing by the average P2 latency to both small and large patterns. The absolute value of this measure, the Contour Response (CR), was then used to rank the cats. The CR for P2 latency produced a complete separation of the deprived and control cats, all of the deprived cats showing a smaller CR than any of the normal cats (Table I). The difference between normal and deprived cats was, therefore, highly significant (P< 0.00001 U test, 1-tailed). The suprasylvian VER had two principle components, an early positive wave (P1) peaking at 49.0 msec and a later positive wave (P2) peaking at 87 msec. The suprasylvian P1 wave showed no CR for latency or amplitude in either group. The suprasylvian P2 wave had the same average latency and amplitude in both normal and pattern deprived groups. Although suprasylvian P2 latency in normals did not show the CR found in primary visual P2, the amplitude of the suprasylvian P2 did show a CR (P< 0.0361 U test, 1-tailed) in normally reared cats. No CR was observed in either the latency or amplitude of TABLE I Contour Responses of latency of P2 component of evoked response to patterned stimuli recorded over primary visual cortex of normally reared cats and pattern deprived litter mates. S-L Contour response = x 100 where S = latency to 1/2 (S + L) small patterns; L = latency to large patterns. Litters Normal Pattern deprived Cat CR Cat CR I 2D 9.0% 2C 3.8% 2B 28.2% 2A 1.9% 2E 21.4% II 1B 12.2% 1A 0% 1C 2.5% HI M 4.4% K 2.5% O 4.2% L 2.2% IV T 5.8% S 0% suprasylvian P2 in the deprived cats. Neither of the waves that showed a CR in normal cats (suprasylvian P2 amplitude and primary visual P2 latency) appeared to be responding to the configuration of the individual patterns. All high contour patterns produced VERs of essentially the same amplitude and latency. VERs produced by the various low contour patterns also had the same amplitude and latency, although these amplitudes and latencies were different from those produced by high contour patterns. The motor cortex VER consisted of a single positive wave peaking at.88.6 msec. The amplitude of this wave was significantly smaller in pattern deprived cats than in normals (P< 0.049 U test, 1-tailed). The latency was the same in both groups. Neither group showed a CR in either the latency or amplitude. DISCUSSION The lack of VER latency and amplitude response to contour in pattern deprived cats provides a striking parallel to the lack of behavioral responsiveness to contour in these animals. Thus, CR appears to be a promising tool which might provide a quantifiable physiological correlate of both normal perceptual development and changes in perceptual abilities resulting from enriched or impoverished environments. Previous visual evoked response studies on binocularly pattern deprived cats (Scherrer and Fourment 1964; Baxter 1966) have not employed patterned stimuli and have not controlled peripheral variables, such as pupil size, focus and orientation of the retina towards the stimulus as we have. These earlier studies are, therefore, not directly comparable to this one.,. The findings that diffuse light rearing causes a diminution of the motor cortex VERs confirms similar findings of Glass (1971) on monocularly deprived cats and extends them to the binocular condition. The effect is not nearly as strong after our binocular deprivation as it was after Glass' monocular deprivation. The difference is probably due to the imbalance caused by Glass' having one half of the visual system functional. Monocular deprivation has also been shown to produce more severe abnormalities in unit firing

572 than binocular deprivation (Wiesel and Hubel 1965a). The diminution of motor cortex VERs indicates that the physiological connections that exist in a normal animal between visual and motor systems (Buser et al 1968) are susceptible to environmental influences. The susceptibility of visuo-motor coordination and placing responses to deprivation of sight of limbs has been clearly shown (Hein and Held 1967). Our pattern deprivation condition similarly prevents reafference stimulation from the limbs. This factor suggests that the lack of visuo-motor coordination rather than the total lack of pattern vision might be responsible for the diminution of the motor cortex VER. Physiological investigation of the motor cortex of cats deprived of limb vision might clarify this issue. Our finding of visual CRs in normal anesthetized animals now makes it possible to experiment on the physiological processes underlying both the human pattern evoked response and the CR. One phenomenon meriting further investigation is the difference in the manifestation of the CR in the suprasylvian and primary visual cortical areas, the CR primarily affecting amplitude in the former and latency in the latter. The differing behavior of the late wave in these two areas seems to indicate that there is an overriding modulation, specific to each area, of any common subcortical (Rose and Lindsley 1968; Creutzfeldt et al. 1969) or retinal (Bignall and Rutledge 1964) influences generating the P2 wave. Another problem of interest is the relation of unit firing to the CR Receptive field studies do not appear to explain either the presence of the CR in normals or their absence in pattern deprived animals. Studies of unit evoked responses to patterned stimuli in conjunction with slow potential studies in the visual system would be helpful in answering these questions. SUMMARY Normally reared cats showed a change in the latency of VERs (visual evoked responses) recorded over primary visual cortex and a change in the amplitude of VERs recorded over the suprasylvian association cortex when patterns containing different amounts of contour were J. M. S1EGEL et al. presented. Pattern deprived cats did not show this contour response. A reduction in the amplitude of motor cortex VERs and an increase in the amplitude of the early visual cortex response were also observed in the pattern deprived cats. RESUME IMPERFECTION DU PATTERN DE LA REPONSE EVO- QUEE CHEZ DBS CHATS PRIVES DE PATTERN Chez les chats eleves normalement, s'observe une modification de la latence de la reponse evoquee visuelle (VER) enregistree sur le cortex visuel primaire et une modification de l'amplitude des VERs enregistrees sur le cortex associatif supra-sylvien lorsque des patterns contenant difterentes quantites de contour sont presentes. Les chats eleves en condition de privation de pattern ne montrent pas cette reponse de contour. Une reduction de l'amplitude de la VER sur le cortex moteur et une augmentation de l'amplitude de la reponse corticale visuelle precoce sont egalement observees chez les chats prives de pattern. We thank Anne LeBlanc and Carol P. Thomas for their capable technical assistance. REFERENCES BAXTER, B. Effect of visual deprivation during postnatal maturation on the electroencephalogram of the cat. ' Exp. Neurol., 1966,14:224-237. BIGNALL, K. and RUTLEDGE, L. Origin of a photically evoked afterdischarge in cat visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol., 1964,27:1048-1062. BUSI-R. P.. KITSIKIS, A. and WIESEN'DANGER, M. Modulation of visual input to single neurones of the motor cortex by the primary visual area in the cat. Brain Res., 1968, 10:262-265. CREUTZFELDT, O., ROSINA, A., ITO, M. and PROBST, W. Visual evoked response of single cells of the EEG in primary visual area of the cat. J. Neurophysiol., 1969, 32:127-139. GLASS, J. D. Photically evoked potentials in cat neocortex altered by visual pattern deprivation. Brain Res., 1971, 30:207-210. HARTER, M. and Surrr, C. Visually evoked cortical responses and pattern vision in the infant: a longitudinal study. Psychonom. Sci., 1970,18:235-247. HARTER, M. R. and WHITE, C. T. Effects of contour sharpness and check-size on visually evoked cortical potentials.

PATTERN EVOKED RESPONSES IN CATS 573 Vision Res., 1968,8:701-711. HEIN, A. and HELD, R. Dissociation of the visual placing response into elicited and guided components. Science, 1967,158:390-392. JOHN, E. R. Mechanisms of Memory. Academic Press, New York, 1967,468 p. LOMBROSO, F., DUFFY, F. and ROBB, R. Selective suppression of cerebral evoked potentials to patterned light in amblyopia ex anopsia. Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol, 1969, 27: 238-247. RIESEN, A. H. Effects of early deprivation of photic stimulation. In S. F. OstER and R. E. COOKE (Eds.), Thebiosocial bases of mental retardation. The Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore, Md., 1965: 61-85. RIESEN, A. H. and AARONS, L. Visual movement and intensity descrimination in cats after early deprivation of pattern vision. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1959, 52: 142-149. ROSE, G. and LINDSLEY, D. Development of visually evoked potentials in kittens: specific and nonspecific responses. J. Neurophysiol., 1968,31:607-623. SCHERRER, J. and FOURMENT, A. Electrocortical effects of sensory deprivation during development. Progr. Brain Res., 1964,9: 103-112. SPEHLMANN, R. The averaged electrical responses to diffuse and to patterned light in the human. Electroenceph. din. Neurophysiol., 1965,19:560-569. VAKKUR, G. J., BISHOP, P. O. and KOZAK, W. Visual optics in the cat, including posterior nodal distance and retinal landmarks. Vision Res., 1963, 3: 289-314. WHITE, C. T. and EASON, R. G. Evoked cortical potentials in relation to certain aspects of visual perception. Psychol. Monogr., 1966,80:1-14. WIESEL, T. N. and HUBEL, D. H. Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit responses in kittens. J. Neurophysiol., 1965a,28:1029-1040. WIESEL, T. N. and HUBEL, D. H.'Extent of recovery from the effects of visual deprivation in kittens. J. Neurophysiol., 1965b,28:1060-1072.